Advertisement

Exploring the Validity of Methods to Track Emotions Behind the Wheel

  • Monique DittrichEmail author
  • Sebastian Zepf
Conference paper
Part of the Lecture Notes in Computer Science book series (LNCS, volume 11433)

Abstract

Emotions accompany us anytime, even while driving. Thereby especially negative emotional experiences influence our driving behavior and the safety on our roads. A psychological intervention to regulate feelings is to track them, e.g. by labeling them along categories. Thus, the aim of this work is to establish an empirical base to guide the development of a system that encourages the driver to label his or her emotions. This involves asking what the relevant emotions are and how they can be validly labeled in the driving context. For this purpose, a driving study was conducted to collect data on emotional experiences in-situ. For the labeling task, three methodological approaches were used: free responses, dimensional emotion rating (DER), and categorical emotion rating (CER). As a result, while DER and CER lack validity due to ambiguity or priming effects, respectively, the free response method has practical limitations. Following, it is recommended to develop an in-car emotion tracker based on CER and use the free response data to determine the appropriate number and naming of categories that cover a significant range of emotions. An initial analysis of the free responses revealed 40 distinct categories of emotional experiences.

Keywords

Emotion labeling Validity Driving context 

References

  1. 1.
    Torre, J.B., Liebermann, M.D.: Putting feelings in to words: affect labeling as implicit emotion regulation. Emot. Rev. 10(2), 116–124 (2018)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. 2.
    Nesbit, S.M., Conger, J.C., Conger, A.J.: A quantitative review of the relationship between anger and aggressive driving. Aggress. Violent Behav. 12, 156–176 (2007)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. 3.
    National Highway Traffic Safety Administration: Visual-manual NHTSA driver distraction guidelines for in-vehicle electronic devices. National Highway Traffic Safety Administration, Department of Transportation, Washington, DC (2012)Google Scholar
  4. 4.
    Oinas-Kukkonen, H., Harjumaa, M.: Persuasive systems design: key issues, process model, and system features. Commun. Assoc. Inf. Syst. 24(1), 485–500 (2009)Google Scholar
  5. 5.
    Underwood, G., Chapman, P., Wright, S., Crundall, D.: Anger while driving. Transp. Res. Part F 2(1), 55–68 (1999)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. 6.
    Jeon, M., Walker, B.N.: What to detect? Analyzing factor structures of affect in driving contexts for an emotion detection and regulation system. In: Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society 55th Annual Meeting, pp. 1889–1893. Sage Publications, Los Angeles (2011)Google Scholar
  7. 7.
    Mesken, J., Hagenzieker, M.P., Rothengatter, T., de Waard, D.: Frequency, determinants, and consequences of different drivers’ emotions: an on-the-road study using self-reports, (observed) behaviour, and physiology. Transp. Res. Part F 10, 458–475 (2007)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. 8.
    Niven, K., Totterdell, P., Holman, D.: A classification of controlled interpersonal affect regulation strategies. Emotion 9(4), 498–509 (2009)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. 9.
    DeLoach, S.L., Tiemann, T.K.: Not driving alone? American commuting in the twenty-first century. Transportation 39(3), 521–537 (2012)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. 10.
    Scherer, K.R.: What are emotions? And how can they be measured? Soc. Sci. Inf. 44(4), 695–729 (2005)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. 11.
    Healey, J.: Recording affect in the field: towards methods and metrics for improving ground truth labels. In: D’Mello, S., Graesser, A., Schuller, B., Martin, J.-C. (eds.) ACII 2011. LNCS, vol. 6974, pp. 107–116. Springer, Heidelberg (2011).  https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-24600-5_14CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. 12.
    Cicchetti, D.V.: Guidelines, criteria, and rules of thumb for evaluating normed and standardized assessment instruments in psychology. Psychol. Assess. 6(4), 284–290 (1994)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. 13.
    Crispim, C., Cruz, R.M., Baasch, D., Amorim, L., Trevisan, R.L., Da Silva, M.A.: Measurement of affect: from theoretical and instrumental perspectives. Psychol. Res. 5(2), 96–107 (2015)Google Scholar
  14. 14.
    Brooks, J.A., Shablack, H., Gendron, M., Satpute, A.B., Parrish, M.H., Lindquist, K.A.: The role of language in the experience and perception of emotion: a neuroimaging meta-analysis. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 12(2), 169–183 (2017)Google Scholar
  15. 15.
    Cohen, J.: Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioural Sciences, 2nd edn. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale (1988)zbMATHGoogle Scholar
  16. 16.
    Ekman, P.: An argument for basic emotions. Cogn. Emot. 6(3–4), 169–200 (1992)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. 17.
    Watling, D., Damaskinou, N.: Children’s facial emotion recognition skills: longitudinal associations with lateralization for emotion processing. Child Dev. 1–16 (2018) Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Daimer AG Research and DevelopmentBöblingenGermany

Personalised recommendations