Advertisement

Magnitude and Number Sensitivity of the Approximate Number System in Conceptual Spaces

  • Aleksander GemelEmail author
  • Paula QuinonEmail author
Chapter
Part of the Synthese Library book series (SYLI, volume 405)

Abstract

In this paper, we propose a conceptual-spaces model of numerical cognition, and more precisely, of representations generated by Approximate Number System. The model is an extended and improved version of our earlier result (Gemel A, Quinon P: The approximate numbers system and the treatment of vagueness in conceptual spaces. In: Lukowski L, Gemel A, Zukowski B (eds) Cognition, meaning and action. Jagiellonian-Lodz University Press, Kraków, pp 87–108, 2015), where only purely quantitative information was accounted for. We focused on the idea that ANS evolved to detect numerosity in the input, and to abstract this information from all possible magnitude-related cues, such as size of compared objects, aggregate area of those objects, or density of their location. The idea is that when one sees a pile of apples, ANS acts as a “number sense”, informing one of the approximate quantity of apples. Consequently, the original model was very simplified, accounting only for one, uniform perceptual discrete visual input. With inspiration from computational models (ex. Dehaene S, Changeux JP: J Cogn Neurosci 5:390–407, 1993;, Lourenco SF, Longo MR: Psychol Sci 21(6):873–881, 2010) that can process more complex stimuli, we propose in this paper a conceptual-spaces model for non-uniform input. The improved version of the model accounts additionally for magnitude-related cues related to both “number sense” and “magnitude sense”.

Notes

Acknowledgements

We are very grateful to Peter Gärdenfors, as well as to the anonymous reviewers for the valuable comments and helpful remarks. Aleksander Gemel gratefully acknowledges financial support by the Approximate Number System in Conceptual Spaces project funded under the Grant of the Dean of the Faculty of Education Science University of Lodz (B1611800000228.01).

References

  1. Banks, M. S. (1980). The development of visual accommodation during early infancy. Child Development, 51(3), 646–666.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Burr, D., & Ross, J. (2008). A visual sense of number. Current Biology, 18(6), 425–428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Carey, S. (2001). Cognitive foundations of arithmetic: Evolution and ontogenisis. Mind & Language, 16(1), 37–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Carey, S. (2009). The origin of concepts. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Carey, S., & Sarnecka, B. W. (2006). The development of human conceptual representations. In M. Johnson & Y. Munakata (Eds.), Processes of change in brain and cognitive development: Attention and performance (Vol. XXI, pp. 473–496). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  6. Dehaene, S. (1997). The number sense: How the mind creates mathematics. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  7. Dehaene, S., & Brannon, E. M. (2010). Space, time, and number: A Kantian research program. Trends in Cognitive Science, 14(12), 517–519.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Dehaene, S., & Changeux, J. P. (1993). Development of elementary numerical abilities: A neuronal model. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 390–407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Dobson, V., & Teller, D. Y. (1978). Visual acuity in human infants: A review and comparison of behavioral and electrophysiological studies. Vision Research, 18(11), 1469–1483.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Douven, I., Decock, L., Dietz, R., & Egré, P. (2013). Vagueness: A conceptual spaces approach. Journal of Philosophical Logic, 42(1), 137–160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Feigenson, L., Carey, S., & Hauser, M. (2002a). The representations underlying infants’ choice of more: Object files versus analog magnitudes. Psychological Science, 13(2), 150–156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Feigenson, L., Carey, S., & Spelke, E. (2002b). Infants’ discrimination of number vs. continuous extent. Cognitive Psychology, 44, 33–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Feigenson, L., Dehaene, S., & Spelke, E. S. (2004). Core systems of number. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(10), 307–314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Gärdenfors, P. (2000). Conceptual spaces: On the geometry of thought. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Gebuis, T., & Reynvoet, B. (2012a). The interplay between nonsymbolic number and its continuous visual properties. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141(4), 642–648.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Gebuis, T., & Reynvoet, B. (2012b). Continuous visual properties explain neural responses to nonsymbolic number. Psychophysiology, 49(11), 1481–1491.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Gemel, A., & Quinon, P. (2015). The approximate numbers system and the treatment of vagueness in conceptual spaces. In L. Lukowski, A. Gemel, & B. Żukowski (Eds.), Cognition, meaning and action (pp. 87–108). Kraków: Jagiellonian-Lodz University Press.Google Scholar
  18. Goodman, N. (1976). Languages of art: An approach to a theory of symbols. Indianapolis: Hackett.Google Scholar
  19. Halberda, J., & Feigenson, L. (2008). Developmental change in the acuity of the “number sense”: The approximate number system in 3-, 4-, 5-, and 6-year-olds and adults. Developmental Psychology, 44(5), 1457–1465.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Leibovich, T., & Ansari, D. (2016). The symbol-grounding problem in numerical cognition: A review of theory, evidence and outstanding questions. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70(1), 12–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Leibovich, T., & Henik, A. (2013). Magnitude processing in non-symbolic stimuli. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 375.Google Scholar
  22. Leibovich, T., Henik, A., & Salti, M. (2015). Numerosity processing is context driven even in the subitizing range: An fMRI study. Neuropsychologia, 77, 137–147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Leibovich, T., Katzin, N., Harel, M., & Henik, A. (2017). From “sense of number” to “sense of magnitude”: The role of continuous magnitudes in numerical cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 40, e164.  https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X16000960.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Libertus, M. E., Feigenson, L., & Halberda, J. (2011). Preschool acuity of the approximate number system correlates with school math ability. Developmental Science, 14(6), 1292–1293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Lourenco, S. F., & Longo, M. R. (2010). General magnitude representation in human infants. Psychological Science, 21(6), 873–881.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Mix, K. S., Huttenlocher, J., & Levine, S. C. (2002a). Multiple cues for quantification in infancy: Is number one of them? Psychological Bulletin, 128, 278–294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Mix, K. S., Huttenlocher, J., & Levine, S. C. (2002b). Quantitative development in infancy and early childhood. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Nieder, A., & Dehaene, S. (2009). Representation of number in the brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 32, 185–208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Slusser, E., & Sarnecka, B. W. (2011). Find the picture of eight turtles: A link between children’s counting and their knowledge of number-word semantics. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 110, 38–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Spelke, E. (2000). Core knowledge. American Psychologist, 55(11), 1233–1243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Starkey, P., & Cooper, R. G. (1980). Perception of numbers by human infants. Science, 210, 1033–1034.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Tversky, A. (1977). Features of similarity. Psychological Review, 84(4), 327–352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Verguts, T., & Fias, W. (2004). Representation of number in animals and humans: A neural model. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(9), 1493–1504.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Xu, F. (2000). Numerical competence in infancy: Two systems of representation. Paper presented at the 12th Biennial International Conference on Infant Studies, Brighton, UK, 16–19 July 2000.Google Scholar
  35. Xu, F. (2003). Numerosity discrimination in infants: Evidence for two systems of representations. Cognition, 89, B15–B25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Xu, F., & Spelke, E. S. (2000). Large number discrimination in 6-month-old infants. Cognition, 74, B1–B11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Cognitive Science, Institute of PsychologyUniversity of LodzLodzPoland
  2. 2.Department of Administration and Social SciencesWarsaw University of TechnologyWarsawPoland
  3. 3.Department of Philosophy and Cognitive SciencesLund UniversityLundSweden

Personalised recommendations