Advertisement

Cross-Modal Learning in the Auditory System

  • Patrick BrunsEmail author
  • Brigitte Röder
Chapter
Part of the Springer Handbook of Auditory Research book series (SHAR, volume 68)

Abstract

Unisensory auditory representations are strongly shaped by multisensory experience, and, likewise, audition contributes to cross-modal learning in other sensory systems. This applies to lower-level sensory features like spatial and temporal processing as well as to higher-level features like speech identification. Cross-modal learning has particularly profound influences during development, but its effects on unisensory processing are ubiquitous throughout life. Moreover, influences of cross-modal learning on unisensory processing have been observed at various timescales, ranging from long-term structural changes over months to short-term plasticity of auditory representations after minutes or only seconds of cross-modal exposure. This chapter focuses particularly on cross-modal learning and its underlying neural mechanisms in the healthy adult auditory system. Recent findings suggest that cross-modal learning operates in parallel on different neural representations and at different timescales. With an increasing amount of exposure to new cross-modal associations, cross-modal learning seems to progress from higher level multisensory representations to lower level modality-specific representations, possibly even in primary auditory cortex. In addition to cortically mediated learning mechanisms, auditory representations are shaped via subcortical multisensory pathways including the superior colliculi in the midbrain. The emerging view from these findings is that auditory-guided behavior is jointly shaped by cross-modal learning in distinct neural systems. To fully understand the dynamic nature of the auditory system, it will be important to identify how short-term and long-term learning processes interact in the mature brain.

Keywords

Attention Audiovisual Multisensory Plasticity Recalibration Sensory representations Space Time Ventriloquism aftereffect Visual system 

Notes

Acknowledgments

The authors’ work was supported by German Research Foundation (DFG) Grants BR 4913/2-1 and TRR 169 Subproject A1 and the City of Hamburg Grant “Crossmodal Learning.”

Compliance with Ethics Requirements

Patrick Bruns declares that he has no conflict of interest.

Brigitte Röder declares that she has no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Alais, D., & Burr, D. (2004). The ventriloquist effect results from near-optimal bimodal integration. Current Biology, 14, 257–262.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Bergan, J. F., Ro, P., Ro, D., & Knudsen, E. I. (2005). Hunting increases adaptive auditory map plasticity in adult barn owls. The Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 9816–9820.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  3. Bertelson, P., & Aschersleben, G. (1998). Automatic visual bias of perceived auditory location. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5, 482–489.Google Scholar
  4. Bertelson, P., Vroomen, J., de Gelder, B., & Driver, J. (2000). The ventriloquist effect does not depend on the direction of deliberate visual attention. Perception & Psychophysics, 62, 321–332.Google Scholar
  5. Bertelson, P., Vroomen, J., & de Gelder, B. (2003). Visual recalibration of auditory speech identification: A McGurk aftereffect. Psychological Science, 14, 592–597.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Bertelson, P., Frissen, I., Vroomen, J., & de Gelder, B. (2006). The aftereffects of ventriloquism: Patterns of spatial generalization. Perception & Psychophysics, 68, 428–436.Google Scholar
  7. Bertini, C., Leo, F., Avenanti, A., & Làdavas, E. (2010). Independent mechanisms for ventriloquism and multisensory integration as revealed by theta-burst stimulation. European Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 1791–1799.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Bonath, B., Noesselt, T., Martinez, A., Mishra, J., Schwiecker, K., Heinze, H. J., & Hillyard, S. A. (2007). Neural basis of the ventriloquist illusion. Current Biology, 17, 1697–1703.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Bruns, P., & Röder, B. (2010). Tactile capture of auditory localization: An event-related potential study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 1844–1857.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Bruns, P., & Röder, B. (2015). Sensory recalibration integrates information from the immediate and the cumulative past. Scientific Reports, 5, 12739.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  11. Bruns, P., Liebnau, R., & Röder, B. (2011a). Cross-modal training induces changes in spatial representations early in the auditory processing pathway. Psychological Science, 22, 1120–1126.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Bruns, P., Spence, C., & Röder, B. (2011b). Tactile recalibration of auditory spatial representations. Experimental Brain Research, 209, 333–344.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Bruns, P., Maiworm, M., & Röder, B. (2014). Reward expectation influences audiovisual spatial integration. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 76, 1815–1827.Google Scholar
  14. Caclin, A., Soto-Faraco, S., Kingstone, A., & Spence, C. (2002). Tactile “capture” of audition. Perception & Psychophysics, 64, 616–630.Google Scholar
  15. Chen, L., & Vroomen, J. (2013). Intersensory binding across space and time: A tutorial review. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75, 790–811.Google Scholar
  16. Dahmen, J. C., & King, A. J. (2007). Learning to hear: Plasticity of auditory cortical processing. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 17, 456–464.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Dahmen, J. C., Keating, P., Nodal, F. R., Schulz, A. L., & King, A. J. (2010). Adaptation to stimulus statistics in the perception and neural representation of auditory space. Neuron, 66, 937–948.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  18. Dean, I., Harper, N. S., & McAlpine, D. (2005). Neural population coding of sound level adapts to stimulus statistics. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 1684–1689.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Eramudugolla, R., Kamke, M. R., Soto-Faraco, S., & Mattingley, J. B. (2011). Perceptual load influences auditory space perception in the ventriloquist aftereffect. Cognition, 118, 62–74.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Ernst, M. O., & Bülthoff, H. H. (2004). Merging the senses into a robust percept. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 162–169.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Frissen, I., Vroomen, J., de Gelder, B., & Bertelson, P. (2003). The aftereffects of ventriloquism: Are they sound-frequency specific? Acta Psychologica, 113, 315–327.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Frissen, I., Vroomen, J., de Gelder, B., & Bertelson, P. (2005). The aftereffects of ventriloquism: Generalization across sound-frequencies. Acta Psychologica, 118, 93–100.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Frissen, I., Vroomen, J., & de Gelder, B. (2012). The aftereffects of ventriloquism: The time course of the visual recalibration of auditory localization. Seeing and Perceiving, 25, 1–14.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Fujisaki, W., Shimojo, S., Kashino, M., & Nishida, S. (2004). Recalibration of audiovisual simultaneity. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 773–778.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Gutfreund, Y., & King, A. J. (2012). What is the role of vision in the development of the auditory space map? In B. E. Stein (Ed.), The New Handbook of Multisensory Processing (pp. 573–587). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
  26. Heron, J., Roach, N. W., Whitaker, D., & Hanson, J. V. M. (2010). Attention regulates the plasticity of multisensory timing. European Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 1755–1762.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Heron, J., Roach, N. W., Hanson, J. V. M., McGraw, P. V., & Whitaker, D. (2012). Audiovisual time perception is spatially specific. Experimental Brain Research, 218, 477–485.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  28. Ikumi, N., & Soto-Faraco, S. (2014). Selective attention modulates the direction of audio-visual temporal recalibration. PLoS One, 9, e99311.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  29. Kim, R. S., Seitz, A. R., & Shams, L. (2008). Benefits of stimulus congruency for multisensory facilitation of visual learning. PLoS One, 3, e1532.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  30. Kim, R., Peters, M. A. K., & Shams, L. (2012). 0 + 1 > 1: How adding noninformative sound improves performance on a visual task. Psychological Science, 23, 6–12.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. King, A. J. (2009). Visual influences on auditory spatial learning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B: Biological Sciences, 364, 331–339.Google Scholar
  32. Knudsen, E. I., & Brainard, M. S. (1991). Visual instruction of the neural map of auditory space in the developing optic tectum. Science, 253, 85–87.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Knudsen, E. I., & Knudsen, P. F. (1990). Sensitive and critical periods for visual calibration of sound localization by barn owls. The Journal of Neuroscience, 10, 222–232.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  34. Kopčo, N., Lin, I.-F., Shinn-Cunningham, B. G., & Groh, J. M. (2009). Reference frame of the ventriloquism aftereffect. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 13809–13814.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  35. Leo, F., Bolognini, N., Passamonti, C., Stein, B. E., & Làdavas, E. (2008). Cross-modal localization in hemianopia: New insights on multisensory integration. Brain, 131, 855–865.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Lewald, J. (2002). Rapid adaptation to auditory-visual spatial disparity. Learning & Memory, 9, 268–278.Google Scholar
  37. Linkenhoker, B. A., & Knudsen, E. I. (2002). Incremental training increases the plasticity of the auditory space map in adult barn owls. Nature, 419, 293–296.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Lüttke, C. S., Ekman, M., van Gerven, M. A. J., & de Lange, F. P. (2016). McGurk illusion recalibrates subsequent auditory perception. Scientific Reports, 6, 32891.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  39. Maiworm, M., Bellantoni, M., Spence, C., & Röder, B. (2012). When emotional valence modulates audiovisual integration. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74, 1302–1311.Google Scholar
  40. McGurk, H., & MacDonald, J. (1976). Hearing lips and seeing voices. Nature, 264, 746–748.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Morein-Zamir, S., Soto-Faraco, S., & Kingstone, A. (2003). Auditory capture of vision: Examining temporal ventriloquism. Cognitive Brain Research, 17, 154–163.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Murray, M. M., Lewkowicz, D. J., Amedi, A., & Wallace, M. T. (2016). Multisensory processes: A balancing act across the lifespan. Trends in Neurosciences, 39, 567–579.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  43. Navarra, J., Soto-Faraco, S., & Spence, C. (2007). Adaptation to audiotactile asynchrony. Neuroscience Letters, 413, 72–76.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Navarra, J., Hartcher-O’Brien, J., Piazza, E., & Spence, C. (2009). Adaptation to audiovisual asynchrony modulates the speeded detection of sound. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106, 9169–9173.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  45. Noel, J.-P., De Niear, M., Van der Burg, E., & Wallace, M. T. (2016). Audiovisual simultaneity judgment and rapid recalibration throughout the lifespan. PLoS One, 11, e0161698.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  46. Pages, D. S., & Groh, J. M. (2013). Looking at the ventriloquist: Visual outcome of eye movements calibrates sound localization. PLoS One, 8, e72562.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  47. Passamonti, C., Frissen, I., & Làdavas, E. (2009). Visual recalibration of auditory spatial perception: Two separate neural circuits for perceptual learning. European Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 1141–1150.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Polley, D. B., Steinberg, E. E., & Merzenich, M. M. (2006). Perceptual learning directs auditory cortical map reorganization through top-down influences. The Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 4970–4982.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  49. Powers, A. R., III, Hillock, A. R., & Wallace, M. T. (2009). Perceptual training narrows the temporal window of multisensory binding. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 12265–12274.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  50. Powers, A. R., III, Hevey, M. A., & Wallace, M. T. (2012). Neural correlates of multisensory perceptual learning. The Journal of Neuroscience, 32(18), 6263–6274.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  51. Radeau, M., & Bertelson, P. (1974). The after-effects of ventriloquism. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 26, 63–71.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. Radeau, M., & Bertelson, P. (1977). Adaptation to auditory-visual discordance and ventriloquism in semirealistic situations. Perception & Psychophysics, 22, 137–146.Google Scholar
  53. Radeau, M., & Bertelson, P. (1978). Cognitive factors and adaptation to auditory-visual discordance. Perception & Psychophysics, 23, 341–343.Google Scholar
  54. Recanzone, G. H. (1998). Rapidly induced auditory plasticity: The ventriloquism aftereffect. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 95, 869–875.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  55. Recanzone, G. H. (2009). Interactions of auditory and visual stimuli in space and time. Hearing Research, 258, 89–99.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  56. Recanzone, G. H., & Sutter, M. L. (2008). The biological basis of audition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 119–142.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  57. Renzi, C., Bruns, P., Heise, K.-F., Zimerman, M., Feldheim, J. F., Hummel, F. C., & Röder, B. (2013). Spatial remapping in the audio-tactile ventriloquism effect: A TMS investigation on the role of the ventral intraparietal area. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25, 790–801.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  58. Rohe, T., & Noppeney, U. (2015). Cortical hierarchies perform Bayesian causal inference in multisensory perception. PLoS Biology, 13, e1002073.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  59. Roseboom, W., & Arnold, D. H. (2011). Twice upon a time: Multiple concurrent temporal recalibrations of audiovisual speech. Psychological Science, 22, 872–877.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  60. Seitz, A. R., Kim, R., & Shams, L. (2006). Sound facilitates visual learning. Current Biology, 16, 1422–1427.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  61. Shams, L., & Seitz, A. R. (2008). Benefits of multisensory learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 411–417.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  62. Shams, L., Wozny, D. R., Kim, R., & Seitz, A. (2011). Influences of multisensory experience on subsequent unisensory processing. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 264.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  63. Slutsky, D. A., & Recanzone, G. H. (2001). Temporal and spatial dependency of the ventriloquism effect. NeuroReport, 12, 7–10.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  64. Stein, B. E., & Stanford, T. R. (2008). Multisensory integration: Current issues from the perspective of the single neuron. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9, 255–266.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  65. Stein, B. E., Huneycutt, W. S., & Meredith, M. A. (1988). Neurons and behavior: The same rules of multisensory integration apply. Brain Research, 448, 355–358.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  66. Strelnikov, K., Rosito, M., & Barone, P. (2011). Effect of audiovisual training on monaural spatial hearing in horizontal plane. PLoS One, 6, e18344.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  67. Talsma, D., Senkowski, D., Soto-Faraco, S., & Woldorff, M. G. (2010). The multifaceted interplay between attention and multisensory integration. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 400–410.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  68. Tzounopoulos, T., & Leão, R. M. (2012). Mechanisms of memory and learning in the auditory system. In L. O. Trussel, A. N. Popper, & R. R. Fay (Eds.), Synaptic Mechanisms in the Auditory System (pp. 203–226). New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
  69. Van der Burg, E., Alais, D., & Cass, J. (2013). Rapid recalibration to audiovisual asynchrony. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 14633–14637.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  70. Van der Burg, E., Alais, D., & Cass, J. (2015). Audiovisual temporal recalibration occurs independently at two different time scales. Scientific Reports, 5, 14526.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  71. Vatakis, A., Navarra, J., Soto-Faraco, S., & Spence, C. (2007). Temporal recalibration during asynchronous audiovisual speech perception. Experimental Brain Research, 181, 173–181.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  72. Von Kriegstein, K., & Giraud, A.-L. (2006). Implicit multisensory associations influence voice recognition. PLoS Biology, 4, e326.Google Scholar
  73. Vroomen, J., & de Gelder, B. (2004). Temporal ventriloquism: Sound modulates the flash-lag effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30, 513–518.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  74. Vroomen, J., & Keetels, M. (2010). Perception of intersensory synchrony: A tutorial review. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72, 871–884.Google Scholar
  75. Vroomen, J., Bertelson, P., & de Gelder, B. (2001). The ventriloquist effect does not depend on the direction of automatic visual attention. Perception & Psychophysics, 63, 651–659.Google Scholar
  76. Vroomen, J., Keetels, M., de Gelder, B., & Bertelson, P. (2004). Recalibration of temporal order perception by exposure to audio-visual asynchrony. Cognitive Brain Research, 22, 32–35.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  77. Wallace, M. T., & Stein, B. E. (1997). Development of multisensory neurons and multisensory integration in cat superior colliculus. The Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 2429–2444.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  78. Wallace, M. T., & Stein, B. E. (2007). Early experience determines how the senses will interact. Journal of Neurophysiology, 97, 921–926.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  79. Woods, T. M., & Recanzone, G. H. (2004). Visually induced plasticity of auditory spatial perception in macaques. Current Biology, 14, 1559–1564.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  80. Wozny, D. R., & Shams, L. (2011). Recalibration of auditory space following milliseconds of cross-modal discrepancy. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 4607–4612.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  81. Yu, L., Rowland, B. A., & Stein, B. E. (2010). Initiating the development of multisensory integration by manipulating sensory experience. The Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 4904–4913.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  82. Zaidel, A., Turner, A. H., & Angelaki, D. E. (2011). Multisensory calibration is independent of cue reliability. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 13949–13962.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  83. Zwiers, M. P., van Opstal, A. J., & Paige, G. D. (2003). Plasticity in human sound localization induced by compressed spatial vision. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 175–181.PubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Biological Psychology and NeuropsychologyUniversity of HamburgHamburgGermany

Personalised recommendations