Skip to main content

The Emergence of Hubs in Complex Syntactic Networks and the DP Hypothesis: The Relevance of a Linguistic Analysis

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Complexity Applications in Language and Communication Sciences

Abstract

A series of analyses of linguistic corpora of L1 acquisition of different languages (Catalan, Basque, Dutch, Italian, German, French, Spanish and English) showed that functional words emerge as hubs of the network. Such emergence always takes place late and abruptly, in coincidence with that of a new topology of the network: the small-world network. This kind of network is the middle stage between completely random and completely regular networks. In earlier analyses, determiners stood out among functional words as hubs in all languages, regardless of their linguistic phylogeny. But there are two different ways to analyze the syntactic relationship between determiners and nouns: determiners could either be “governors” or “dependents” of nouns. Here we explore the two possible analyses and argue that the first one should be preferred over the second one, in line with contemporary syntactic theorizing.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 119.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 159.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    We would like to note that the term «word » is highly ambiguous and therefore not really suitable as a term for a unit. The boundaries of «words» can vary dramatically from one language to another (e.g., Catalan vs. Basque , or Frisian vs. West Greenlandic ) and from one module to the other (cf. words in morphology vs. in phonology ).

  2. 2.

    The term c-network is used in order to make clear the radical difference between the networks with which we work here and other networks used in cognitive science such as Neural Networks or Connectionism . Complex networks have no relation whatsoever with the networks developed by connectionists, the word «network » being the only point in common.

  3. 3.

    A word is said to be a phonological neighbor of a target word if the substitution, addition or deletion of a single phoneme in any position in that word converts it to the target word .

  4. 4.

    “OM” stands for “Object Agreement Marker” and “SM” for “Subject Agreement Marker”.

  5. 5.

    An example typically used to illustrate C is, thinking in nodes as people and edges as friendship relationships, “how many of my friends are also friends of each other?”.

References

  • Abney, S. P. (1987). The English noun phrase in its sentential perspective. Doctoral dissertation. Cambridge: Massachusetts Institute of Technology. Retrieved form http://www.ai.mit.edu/projects/dm/theses/abney87.pdf.

  • Arbesman, S., Strogatz, S. H., & Vitevitch, M. S. (2010). The structure of phonological networks across multiple languages. International Journal of Bifurcation and Chaos, 20, 679–685. https://doi.org/10.1142/s021812741002596x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barceló-Coblijn, L., Corominas-Murtra, B., & Gomila, A. (2012). Syntactic trees and small-world networks: Syntactic development as a dynamical process. Adaptive Behavior, 20, 427–442. https://doi.org/10.1177/1059712312455439.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barceló-Coblijn, L., Real Puigdollers, C., Irurtzun, A., López-Navarro, E. & Gomila, A. (2018). How children develop their ability to combine words: A network based approach. Manuscript submitted for publication.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barceló-Coblijn, L., Serna Salazar, D., Isaza, G., Castillo Ossa, L. F., & Bedia, M. G. (2017). Netlang: A software for the linguistic analysis of corpora by means of complex networks. PLoS ONE, 12(8), e0181341. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0181341.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berk, S., & Lillo-Martin, D. (2012). The two-word stage: motivated by linguistic or cognitive constraints? Cognitive Psychology, 65, 118–140. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2012.02.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bernstein, J. B. (2001). The DP hypothesis: Identifying clausal properties in the nominal domain. In M. Baltin & C. Collins (Eds.), The Handbook of Contemporary Syntactic Theory (pp. 536–561). Oxford: Blackwell Publishers Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Čech, R., Mačutek, J., & Žabokrtský, Z. (2011). The role of syntax in complex networks: Local and global importance of verbs in a syntactic dependency network. Physica A: Statistical Mechanics and its Applications, 390, 3614–3623. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physa.2011.05.027.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corominas-Murtra, B. (2007, April 30). Network statistics on early English Syntax: Structural criteria. Retrieved from https://arxiv.org/pdf/0704.3708.pdf.

  • Corominas-Murtra, B., Valverde, S., & Solé, R. V. (2009). The ontogeny of scale-free syntax networks: Phase transitions in early language acquisition. Advances in Complex Systems (ACS), 12, 371–392. https://doi.org/10.1142/s0219525909002192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Demuth, K., & Tremblay, A. (2008). Prosodically-conditioned variability in children’s production of French determiners. Journal of Child Language, 35, 99–127. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0305000907008276.

  • Fenson, L., Marchman, V., Thal, D., Dale, P., Reznick, J., & Bates, E. (2007). The MacArthur-Bates communicative development inventories user’s guide and technical manual (2nd ed.). Baltimore: Brookes Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ferrer-i-Cancho, R., & Solé, R. V. (2001). The small world of human language. Proceedings Biological Sciences/The Royal Society, 268, 2261–2265. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2001.1800.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hudson, R. A. (1990). English word grammar. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liu, H., & Hu, F. (2008). What role does syntax play in a language network? Europhysics Letters, 83: 18002

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Longobardi, G. (1994). Reference and proper names: A theory of N-movement in syntax and logical form. Linguistic Inquiry, 25, 609–665.

    Google Scholar 

  • Longobardi, G. (2001). The structure of DPs: Some principles, parameters, and problems. In M. Baltin & C. Collins (Eds.), The handbook of contemporary syntactic theory (pp. 562-603). Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacWhinney, B. (2000). The childes project: The database. New York & London: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moro, A. (1997). The raising of predicates: Predicative noun phrases and the theory of clause structure. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moro, A. (2000). Dynamic antisymmetry. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newman, M. (2010). Networks: An introduction. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Popescu, M. (2003). Dependency grammar anotator. In F. Hristea & M. Popescu (Eds.), Building awareness in language technology (pp. 17–34). Bucharest: Editura Universităţii din Bucureşti.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramos, J. R. (1992). Introducció a la sintaxi. València: Tandem Edicions.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sagae, K., Davis, E., Lavie, A., Macwhinney, B., & Wintner, S. (2010). Morphosyntactic annotation of CHILDES transcripts. Journal of Child Language, 37, 705–729. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0305000909990407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scarborough, H. S. (1990). Index of productive syntax. Applied Psycholinguistics, 11, 1–22. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0142716400008262.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shannon, P., Markiel, A., Ozier, O., Baliga, N. S., Wang, J. T., Ramage, D., et al. (2003). Cytoscape: A software environment for integrated models of biomolecular interaction networks. Genome Research, 13, 2498–2504. https://doi.org/10.1101/gr.1239303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Solé, R. V., Corominas-Murtra, B., Valverde, S., & Steels, L. (2010). Language networks: Their structure, function, and evolution. Complexity, 15, 20–26. https://doi.org/10.1002/cplx.20305.

  • Soto Valle, R. (2012). La adquisición del euskera: Aproximación a una secuencia típica del desarrollo morfosintáctico de 21/2 a 5 años. Doctoral dissertation. University of the Basque Country UPV/EHU.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stowell, T. (1989). Subjects, specifiers, and X-bar theory. In M. R. Baltin & A. Kroch (Eds.), Alternative conceptions of phrase structure (pp. 232–262). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Szabolcsi, A. (1983). The possessor that run away from home. The Linguistic Review, 3, 89–102. https://doi.org/10.1515/tlir.1983.3.1.89.

  • Szabolcsi, A. (1994). The noun phrase. In F. Kiefer & K. É. Kiss (Eds.), The syntactic structure of Hungarian, volume 27 (pp. 179–274). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vitevitch, M. S. (2008). What can graph theory tell us about word learning and lexical retrieval? Journal of speech, language, and hearing research, 51, 408–422. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2008/030).

  • Watts, D. J., & Strogatz, S. H. (1998). Collective dynamics of “small-world” networks. Nature, 393, 440–442. https://doi.org/10.1038/30918.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wijnen, F., & Verrips, M. (1998). The acquisition of Dutch syntax. In S. Gillis & A. De Houwer (Eds.), The acquisition of Dutch (pp. 223–300). Amsterdam/Baltimore: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zamparelli, R. (2000). Layers in the Determiner Phrase. London & New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research benefited from the following grants: EC FP7/SSH-2013-1 AThEME 613465 (European Commission), IT769-13 (Eusko Jaurlaritza), TIN2016-80347-R (MICINN), FFI2016-78034-C2-2-P and FEDER, FFI2017-87140-C4-1-P, FFI2014-53675-P (MINECO).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Lluís Barceló-Coblijn .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2019 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Barceló-Coblijn, L., Duguine, M., Irurtzun, A. (2019). The Emergence of Hubs in Complex Syntactic Networks and the DP Hypothesis: The Relevance of a Linguistic Analysis. In: Massip-Bonet, À., Bel-Enguix, G., Bastardas-Boada, A. (eds) Complexity Applications in Language and Communication Sciences. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-04598-2_15

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-04598-2_15

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-030-04596-8

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-030-04598-2

  • eBook Packages: Social SciencesSocial Sciences (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics