Advertisement

Deepening Connections in Later Infancy: 6 to 12 Months

  • Amy C. WilliamsonEmail author
  • Laura Hubbs-Tait
  • Janna Colaizzi
  • Amy Huffer
Chapter

Abstract

During the developmental period of 6–12 months, infants deepen and solidify their developing relationships. By 6 or 7 months of age, attachments to and preference for primary caregivers are observable. Infants also begin to exhibit fear of strangers and actively seek proximity to attachment figures during this time. Chapter 4 presents research on typical and atypical social and emotional development for infants between the ages of 6 months and 12 months. It discusses how infants’ new capacity for mobility impacts social and emotional development, particularly in the areas of joint attention, social referencing, stranger anxiety, and understanding of the minds of others. The biobehavioral context of development at this age is discussed as well as caregiver practices that promote healthy social and emotional growth during this time period.

Keywords

Later infancy Social and emotional development Parenting Caregiver-child relationships Biobehavioral context 

References

  1. Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  2. Anderson, D. I., Campos, J. J., Witherington, D. C., Dahl, A., Rivera, M., He, M., … Barbu-Roth, M. (2013). The role of locomotion in psychological development. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 440. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00440 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  3. Azak, S., Murison, R., Wentzel-Larsen, T., Smith, L., & Gunnar, M. R. (2013). Maternal depression and infant daytime cortisol. Developmental Psychobiology, 55(4), 334–351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bakeman, R., & Adamson, L. B. (1984). Coordinating attention to people and objects in mother-infant and peer-infant interaction. Child Development, 55, 1278–1289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Behne, T., Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2005). Unwilling versus unable: infants’ understanding of intentional action. Developmental Psychology, 41(2), 328–337. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.41.2.328 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Beijers, R., Riksen-Walraven, J. M., & de Weerth, C. (2013). Cortisol regulation in 12-month-old human infants: associations with the infants’ early history of breastfeeding and co-sleeping. Stress, 16(3), 267–277. https://doi.org/10.3109/10253890.2012.742057 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Biringen, Z., Emde, R. N., Campos, J. J., & Appelbaum, A. (2008). Development of autonomy: role of walking onset and its timing. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 106, 395–414. https://doi.org/10.2466/PMS.l06.2.395-414 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Boccia, M., & Campos, J. J. (1989). Maternal emotional signals, social referencing, and infants’ reactions to strangers. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 1989, 25–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Brandone, A. C. (2015). Infants’ social and motor experience and emerging understanding of intentional actions. Developmental Psychology, 51(4), 512–523. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038844 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Brooker, R. J., Buss, K. A., Lemery-Chalfant, K., Aksan, N., Davidson, R. J., & Goldsmith, H. H. (2013). The development of stranger fear in infancy and toddlerhood: normative development, individual differences, antecedents, and outcomes. Developmental Science, 16, 864–878.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  11. Campos, J. J. (1990). Crawling onset organizes affective development in infancy. Research & Clinical Center for Child Development, 12, 41–47.Google Scholar
  12. Campos, J. J., Anderson, D. I., Barbu-Roth, M. A., Hubbard, E. M., Hertenstein, M. J., & Witherington, D. (2000). Travel broadens the mind. Infancy, 1(2), 149–219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Campos, J. J., Kermoian, R., & Zumbahlen, M. R. (1992). Socioemotional transformations in the family system following infant crawling onset. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 1992(55), 25–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Campos, J. J., & Stenberg, C. (1981). In M. Lamb & L. Sherrod (Eds.)., Infant social cognition: Empirical and theoretical considerations Perception, appraisal, and emotion: The onset of social referencing (pp. 274–313). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.Google Scholar
  15. Cárcamo, R., van IJzendoorn, M., Vermeer, H., & van der Veer, R. (2014). The validity of the Massie-Campbell Attachment During Stress Scale (ADS). Journal of Child and Family Studies, 23(5), 767–775. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-013-9728-z CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., Tomasello, M., Butterworth, G., & Moore, C. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 63, i–174. https://doi.org/10.2307/1166214 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Del Giudice, M., Ellis, B. J., & Shirtcliff, E. A. (2011). The Adaptive Calibration Model of stress responsivity. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 1562–1592.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Egeland, B., & Farber, E. A. (1984). Infant-mother attachment: Factors related to its development and changes over time. Child Development, 55, 753–771. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130127 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Feldman, R. (2007). Parent–infant synchrony: Biological foundations and developmental outcomes. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(6), 340–345. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00532.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Feldman, R. (2012). Parent-infant synchrony: A biobehavioral model of mutual influences in the formation of affiliative bonds. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 77(2), 42–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Feldman, R., Granat, A., Pariente, C., Kanety, H., Kuint, J., & Gilboa-Schechtman, E. (2009). Maternal depression and anxiety across the postpartum year and infant social engagement, fear regulation, and stress reactivity. Journal of the American Academy of Child Adolescent Psychiatry, 48(9), 919–927. https://doi.org/10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181b21651 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Feldman, R., Singer, M., & Zagoory, O. (2010). Touch attenuates infants’ physiological reactivity to stress. Developmental Science, 13(2), 271–278. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00890.x CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Gaffan, E. A., Martins, C., Healy, S., & Murray, L. (2010). Early social experience and individual differences in infants’ joint attention. Social Development, 19, 369–393.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Geangu, E., Benga, O., Stahl, D., & Striano, T. (2010). Contagious crying beyond the first days of life. Infant Behavior & Development, 33, 279–288. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2010.03.004 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Goldsmith, H. H., & Rothbart, M. K. (1996). Laboratory Temperament Assessment Battery (LAB-TAB): Prelocomotor and locomotor versions. Madison, WI: University of Wisconsin.Google Scholar
  26. Grant, K., McMahon, C., Austin, M., Reilly, N., Leader, L., & Ali, S. (2009). Maternal prenatal anxiety, postnatal caregiving and infants’ cortisol responses to the still-face procedure. Developmental Psychobiology, 51(8), 625–637. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.20397 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Grienenberger, J. F., Kelly, K., & Slade, A. (2005). Maternal reflective functioning, mother–infant affective communication, and infant attachment: Exploring the link between mental states and observed caregiving behavior in the intergenerational transmission of attachment. Attachment & Human Development, 7, 299–311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Gunnar, M., & Quevedo, K. (2007). The neurobiology of stress and development. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 145–173. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085605 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Gunnar, M. R., Larson, M., Hertsgaard, L., Harris, M., & Brodersen, L. (1992). The stressfulness of separation among 9-month-old infants: effects of social context variables and infant temperament. Child Development, 63, 290–303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Haley, D. W., & Stansbury, K. (2003). Infant stress and parent responsiveness: Regulation of physiology and behavior during still-face and reunion. Child Development, 74(5), 1534–1546.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Hamlin, J. K., Hallinan, E. V., & Woodward, A. L. (2008). Do as I do: 7-month-old infants selectively reproduce others’ goals. Developmental Science, 11(4), 487–494. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00694.x CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  32. Hamlin, J. K., Newman, G. E., & Wynn, K. (2009). Eight-month-old infants infer unfulfilled goals, despite ambiguous physical evidence. Infancy, 14(5), 579–590. https://doi.org/10.1080/15250000903144215 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Hamlin, J. K., Wynn, K., & Bloom, P. (2007). Social evaluation by preverbal infants. Nature, 450, 557–560. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature06288 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Hamlin, J. K., Wynn, K., Bloom, P., & Mahajan, N. (2011). How infants and toddlers react to antisocial others. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, USA, 108, 19931–19936.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Hibel, L. C., Granger, D. A., Blair, C., & Cox, M. J. (2009). Intimate partner violence moderates the association between mother-infant adrenocortical activity across an emotional challenge. Journal of Family Psychology, 23(5), 615–625. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016323 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Hibel, L. C., Granger, D. A., Blair, C., & Finegood, E. D. (2014). Maternal-child adrenocortical attunement in early childhood: Continuity and change. Developmental Psychobiology, 57, 83–95. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21266 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  37. Holochwost, S. J., Gariépy, J., Propper, C. B., Mills-Koonce, W. R., & Moore, G. A. (2014). Parenting behaviors and vagal tone at six months predict attachment disorganization at twelve months. Developmental Psychobiology, 56(6), 1423–1430. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21221 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Howes, C., & Spieker, S. (2016). Attachment relationships in the context of multiple caregivers. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (3rd ed., pp. 314–329). New York, NY: Guildford Press.Google Scholar
  39. Leerkes, E. M. (2011). Maternal sensitivity during distressing tasks: A unique predictor of attachment security. Infant Behavior and Development, 34(3), 443–446.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Lyons-Ruth, K., Bronfman, E., & Parsons, E. (1999). Maternal Frightened, Frightening, or Atypical Behavior and Disorganized Infant Attachment Patterns. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 64(3), 67-96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Madigan, S., Moran, G., & Pederson, D. R. (2006). Unresolved states of mind, disorganized attachment relationships, and disrupted interactions of adolescent mothers and their infants. Developmental Psychology, 42, 293–304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Madigan, S., Moran, G., Schuengel, C., Pederson, D., & Otten, R. (2007). Unresolved maternal attachment representations, disrupted maternal behavior and disorganized attachment in infancy: Links to toddler behavior problems. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 1042–1050.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Main, M., & Solomon, J. (1986). Discovery of an insecure-disorganized/disoriented attachment pattern. In T. B. Brazelton & M. W. Yogman (Eds.), Affective development in infancy (pp. 95–124). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
  44. Main, M., & Solomon, J. (1990). Procedures for identifying infants as disorganized/disoriented during the Ainsworth strange situation. In M. T. Greenberg, D. Cicchetti, & E. M. Cummings (Eds.), Attachment in the preschool years: Theory, research, and intervention (pp. 121–160). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  45. Mangelsdorf, S. C. (1992). Developmental changes in infant-stranger interaction. Infant Behavior and Development, 15, 191–208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Massie, H., & Campbell, K. (1983). The Massie-Campbell scale of mother-infant attachment indicators during stress. In J. Call, E. Galenson, & R. Tyson (Eds.), Frontiers of infant psychiatry (pp. 394–412). New York, NY: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  47. Meltzoff, A. N. (2011). Social cognition and the origins of imitation, empathy and theory of mind. In U. Goswami (Ed.), The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development (2nd ed.). Oxford, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
  48. Mesman, J., van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2009). The many faces of the still-face paradigm: A review and meta-analysis. Developmental Review, 29(2), 120–162. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2009.02.001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Middlemiss, W., Granger, D. A., Goldberg, W. A., & Nathans, L. (2012). Asynchrony of mother-infant hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis activity following extinction of infant crying responses induced during the transition to sleep. Early Human Development, 88, 227–232. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2011.08.010 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Mills-Koonce, W. R., Gariépy, J., Propper, C., Sutton, K., Calkins, S., Moore, G., & Cox, M. (2007). Infant and parent factors associated with early maternal sensitivity: a caregiver-attachment systems approach. Infant Behavior & Development, 30(1), 114–126. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2006.11.010 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Mills-Koonce, W. R., Garrett-Peters, P., Barnett, M., Granger, D. A., Blair, C., & Cox, M. J. (2011). Father contributions to cortisol responses in infancy and toddlerhood. Developmental Psychology, 47(2), 388–395. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021066 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  52. Moore, G. A., Hill-Soderlund, A. L., Propper, C. B., Calkins, S. D., Mills-Koonce, W. R., & Cox, M. J. (2009). Mother-infant vagal regulation in the face-to-face still-face paradigm is moderated by maternal sensitivity. Child Development, 80(1), 209–223. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01255.x CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Mortensen, J. A., & Barnett, M. A. (2015). Teacher-child interactions in infant/toddler child care and socioemotional development. Early Education and Development, 26, 209–229. https://doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2015.985878 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Mundy, P. (2018). A review of joint attention and social-cognitive brain systems in typical development and autism spectrum disorder. The European Journal of Neuroscience. https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.13720 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Mundy, P., Block, J., Delgado, C., Pomares, Y., Vaughan Van Hecke, A., & Parlade, M. V. (2007). Individual differences and the development of joint attention in infancy. Child Development, 78(3), 938–954. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01042.x CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  56. NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (1997). The effects of infant child care on infant-mother attachment security: Results of the NICHD Study of Early Child Care. Child Development, 68, 860–879.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Roggman, L. A., Cook, G. A., Innocenti, M. S., Jump Norman, V., & Christiansen, K. (2013). Parenting interactions with children: Checklist of Observation Linked to Outcomes (PICCOLO) in diverse ethnic groups. Infant Mental Health Journal, 34(4), 290–306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Roggman, L. A., Langlois, J. H., Hubbs-Tait, L., & Rieser-Danner, L. (1994). Infant day care, attachment, and the ‘file-drawer problem’. Child Development, 65, 1421–1435.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Roth-Hanania, R., Davidov, M., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (2011). Empathy development from 8 to 16 months: Early signs of concern for others. Infant Behavior & Development, 34, 447–458. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2011.04.007 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Schaffer, H. (1966). The onset of fear of strangers and the incongruity hypothesis. Joumal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 7, 95–106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Slade, A. (2005). Parental reflective functioning: An introduction. Attachment & Human Development, 7, 269–281.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Slade, A., Grienenberger, J., Bernbach, E., Levy, D., & Locker, A. (2005). Maternal reflective functioning, attachment, and the transmission gap: A preliminary study. Attachment & Human Devlopment, 7, 283–298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Sorce, J. F., Emde, R. N., Campos, J. J., & Klinnert, M. D. (1985). Maternal emotional signaling: Its effect on the visual cliff behavior of 1-year-olds. Developmental Psychology, 21, 195–200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Spangler, G., & Grossmann, K. (1993). Biobehavioral organization in securely and insecurely attached infants. Child Development, 64(5), 1439–1450.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Sroufe, L. A. (1977). Wariness of strangers and the study of infant development. Child Development, 48, 731–746.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Sroufe, L. A., Egeland, B., Carlson, E. A., & Collins, W. A. (2005). The development of the person: The Minnesota study of risk and adaptation from birth to adulthood. New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  67. Stenius, F., Theorell, T., Lilja, G., Scheynius, A., Alm, J., & Lindblad, F. (2008). Comparisons between salivary cortisol levels in six-months-olds and their parents. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 33(3), 352–359. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2007.12.001 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  68. Stern, D. N. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant. New York, NY: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  69. Tronick, E., Als, H., Adamson, L., Wise, S., & Brazelton, T. B. (1978). Infants’ response to entrapment between contradictory messages in face-to-face interaction. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 17, 1–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Tryphonopoulos, P. D., Letourneau, N., & Ditommaso, E. (2014). Attachment and caregiver-infant interaction: a review of observational-assessment tools. Infant Mental Health Journal, 35(6), 642–656. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.21461 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  71. Vaish, A., & Striano, T. (2004). Is visual reference necessary? Contributions of facial versus vocal cues in 12-month-olds’ social referencing behavior. Developmental Science, 7, 261–269.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Van Ijzendoorn, M. H., Schuengel, C., & Bakermans-kranenburg, M. J. (1999). Disorganized attachment in early childhood: Meta-analysis of precursors, concomitants, and sequelae. Development and Psychopathology, 11, 225–250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Walden, T. A., & Ogan, T. A. (1988). The development of social referencing. Child Development, 59, 1230–1240. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00344.x CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  74. Waters, E. (1978). The reliability and stability of individual differences in infant-mother attachment. Child Development, 49, 483–494.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Waters, E., Wippman, J., & Sroufe, L. A. (1979). Attachment, positive affect, and competence in the peer group: Two studies in construct validation. Child Development, 50, 821–829.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Waters, S. F., West, T. V., & Berry Mendes, W. (2014). Stress contagion: Physiological covariation between mothers and infants. Psychological Science, 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613518352 CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  • Amy C. Williamson
    • 1
    Email author
  • Laura Hubbs-Tait
    • 1
  • Janna Colaizzi
    • 2
  • Amy Huffer
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Human Development and Family ScienceOklahoma State UniversityStillwaterUSA
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyOklahoma State UniversityStillwaterUSA

Personalised recommendations