Advertisement

How to Pursue the Adaptationist Program in Psychology

  • Brian D. Haig
Chapter
Part of the Studies in Applied Philosophy, Epistemology and Rational Ethics book series (SAPERE, volume 45)

Abstract

This chapter is concerned with the methodological foundations of evolutionary psychology. Evolutionary psychologists have offered adaptation explanations for a wide range of human psychological characteristics. Critics, however, have argued that such endeavours are problematic because the appropriate evidence required to demonstrate adaptation is unlikely to be forthcoming. More specifically, doubts have been raised over both the methodology employed by evolutionary psychologists for studying adaptations and about the possibility of ever developing acceptably rigorous evolutionary explanations of human psychological phenomena. In this chapter, it is argued that by employing a wide range of methods for inferring adaptation and by adopting an inference to the best explanation strategy for evaluating adaptation explanations, these two doubts can be adequately addressed.

References

  1. Aiello, L. C. (1996). Terrestriality, bipedalism and the origin of language. In W. G. Runciman, J. Maynard-Smith, & R. I. M. Dunbar (Eds.), Evolution of social behavior patterns in primates and man. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  2. Aitchison, J. (1998). On discontinuing the continuity–discontinuity debate. In J. R. Hurford, M. Studdert-Kennedy, & C. Knight (Eds.), Approaches to the evolution of language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  3. Alexander, R. D., & Noonan, K. M. (1979). Concealment of ovulation, parental care and human social evolution. In N. A. Chagnon & W. Irons (Eds.), Evolutionary biology and human social behavior: an anthropological perspective. North Scituat, MA: Duxbury Press.Google Scholar
  4. Brandon, R. N. (1990). Adaptation and environment. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  5. Bruch, S. G. (1989). Prediction and theory evaluation: The case of light bending. Science, 246, 1124–1129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Buss, D. M. (1995). Evolutionary psychology: A new paradigm for psychological science. Psychological Inquiry, 6, 1–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Buss, D. M., Haselton, M. G., Shackelford, T. K., Bleske, A. L., & Wakefield, J. C. (1998). Adaptations, exaptations, and spandrels. American Psychologist, 53, 533–548.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Caro, T. M., & Borgerhoff Mulder, M. (1987). The problem of adaptation in the study of human behavior. Ethology & Sociobiology, 8, 61–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Carstairs-McCarthy, A. (1999). The origins of complex language: An inquiry into the evolutionary beginnings of sentences, syllables, and truth. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  10. Chomsky, N. (1972). Language and mind. New York: Harcourt, Brace & Jovanovich.Google Scholar
  11. Chomsky, N. (1991). Linguistics and cognitive science: Problems and mysteries. In A. Kasher (Ed.), The Chomskyan turn. Cambridge, MA: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
  12. Churchland, P. M. (1989). A neurocomputational perspective: The nature of mind and the structure of science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  13. Corballis, M. C. (1999). The gestural origins of language. American Scientist, 87, 138–145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Darwin, C. (1859). On the origin of species by means of natural selection. London: Murray.Google Scholar
  15. Davies, P. S. (1996). Evolutionary theory in cognitive psychology. Minds and Machines, 6, 445–462.Google Scholar
  16. Dawkins, R. (1986). The blind watchmaker. London: Penguin.Google Scholar
  17. Deacon, T. (1997). The symbolic species: The co-evolution of language and the human brain. London: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
  18. Dennett, D. C. (1995). Darwin’s dangerous idea: Evolution and the meanings of life. New York: Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
  19. Duchlin, L. E. (1990). The evolution of articulate speech: comparative anatomy of the oral cavity in Pan and Homo. Journal of Human Evolution, 19, 687–697.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Duhem, P. (1954). The aim and structure of physical theory. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  21. Dunbar, R. I. M. (1993). Coevolution of neocortical size, group size and language in humans. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 681–735.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Dunbar, R. I. M. (1996). Grooming, gossip, and the evolution of language. London: Faber & Faber.Google Scholar
  23. Dunbar, R. I. M., Duncan, N. D. C., & Nettle, D. (1995). Size and structure of freely forming conversational groups. Human Nature, 6, 67–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Foley, R. (1996). The adaptive legacy of human evolution: A search for the environment of evolutionary adaptedness. Evolutionary Anthropology, 4, 194–203.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Frumhoff, P. C., & Reeve, H. K. (1994). Using phylogenies to test hypotheses of adaptation: A critique of some current proposals. Evolution, 48, 172–180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Gangestad, S. W., & Simpson, J. A. (2000). The evolution of human mating: Trade-offs and strategic pluralism. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 573–644.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Godfrey-Smith, P. (1999). Adaptationism and the power of natural selection. Biology and Philosophy, 14, 181–194.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Gould, S. J. (1978). Sociobiology: The art of storytelling. New Scientist, 16, 530–533.Google Scholar
  29. Gould, S. J., & Lewontin, R. C. (1979). The spandrels of San Marco and the Panglossian paradigm: A critique of the adaptationist programme. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 205, 581–598.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Griffiths, P. E. (1996). The historical turn in the study of adaptation. British Journal for the Philosophy of Science, 47, 511–532.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Harcourt, A. H., Harvey, P. H., Larson, S. G., & Short, R. V. (1981). Testis weight, body weight and breeding system in primates. Nature, 293, 55–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Harvey, P. H., & Pagel, M. D. (1991). The comparative method in evolutionary biology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  33. Holcomb, H. R. (1996). Just so stories and inference to the best explanation in evolutionary psychology. Minds and Machines, 6, 525–540.Google Scholar
  34. Hrdy, S. B. (1981). The women that never evolved. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  35. Josephson, J. R., & Josephson, S. G. (1994). Abductive inference: Computation, philosophy, technology. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Ketelaar, T., & Ellis, B. J. (2000). Are evolutionary explanations unfalsifiable? Evolutionary psychology and the Lakatosian philosophy of science. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 1–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Kirkpatrick, L. (1999). Toward an evolutionary psychology of religion and personality. Journal of Personality, 67, 920–952.Google Scholar
  38. Knight, C., Studdert-Kennedy, M., & Hurford, J. R. (Eds.). (2000). The evolutionary emergence of language: Social function and the origins of linguistic form. Cambridge & New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  39. Krause, M. A. (1997). Evolutionary psychology, adaptation and the evolution of language: Is there need for comparison? International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 10, 180–195.Google Scholar
  40. Kuhn, T. S. (1977). The essential tension. Chicago: Chicago University Press.Google Scholar
  41. Lakatos, I. (1978). Falsification and the methodology of scientific research programmes. In I. Lakatos & A. Musgrave (Eds.), Criticism and the growth of knowledge. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  42. Larson, A., & Losos, A. L. (1996). Phylogenetic systematics of adaptation. In M. R. Rose & G. V. Lauder (Eds.), adaptation. San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  43. Lauder, G. V. (1996). The argument from design. In M. R. Rose & G. V. Lauder (Eds.), Adaptation. San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  44. Lewontin, R. C. (1979). Sociobiology as an adaptationist program. Behavioral Science, 24, 5–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Lewontin, R. C. (1998). The evolution of cognition. In D. Scarborough & S. Sternberg (Eds.), An invitation to cognitive science: Methods, models, and conceptual issues. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  46. Lloyd, E. (1999). Evolutionary psychology: The burdens of proof. Biology and Philosophy, 14, 211–233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Looren de Jong, H., & van der Steen, W. J. (1998). Biological thinking in evolutionary psychology: Rockbottom or quicksand? Philosophical Psychology, 11, 183–205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Manning, J. T., Scutt, D., & Lewis-Jones, D. I. (1998). Developmental stability, ejaculate size, and sperm quality in men. Evolution and human behavior, 19, 273–282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Miller, G. F. (2000). The mating mind: How sexual choice shaped the evolution of human nature. New York: Doubleday.Google Scholar
  50. Moller, A. P., Soler, M., & Thornhell, R. (1995). Breast asymmetry, sexual selection, and human reproductive success. Ethology & Sociobiology, 16, 207–219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Orzack, S. H., & Sober, E. (1994). Optimality models and the test of adaptationism. American Naturalist, 143, 361–380.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Piatelli-Palmarini, M. (1989). Evolution, selection, and cognition: From learning to parameter setting in biology and the study of language. Cognition, 31, 1–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Pinker, S. (1997). How the mind works. London: Allen Lane, Penguin Press.Google Scholar
  54. Pinker, S., & Bloom, P. (1990). Natural language and natural selection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 13, 707–784.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Reeve, H. K., & Sherman, P. W. (1993). Adaptation and the goals of evolutionary research. Quarterly Review of Biology, 68, 1–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Richardson, R. C. (1996). The prospects for an evolutionary psychology: Human language and human reasoning. Minds and Machines, 6, 541–557.Google Scholar
  57. Rose, S., Kamin, L. J., & Lewontin, R. C. (1984). Not in our genes: Biology, ideology and human nature. London: Pelican Books.Google Scholar
  58. Rose, M. R., & Lauder, G. V. (1996). Post-spandrel adaptationism. In M. R. Rose & G. V. Lauder (Eds.), adaptation. San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  59. Shapiro, L. A. (1998). Do’s and don’ts for Darwinizing psychology. In D. D. Cummins & C. Allen (Eds.), The evolution of mind. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  60. Sillen-Tullberg, B., & Moller, A. P. (1993). The relationship between concealed ovulation and mating systems in anthropoid primates: A phylogenetic analysis. American Naturalist, 141, 1–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Smith, E. A., Borgerhoff Mulder, M., & Hill, K. (2001). Controversies in the evolutionary social sciences: A guide for the perplexed. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 16, 128–135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Sober, E. (1984). The nature of selection: Evolutionary theory in philosophical focus. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  63. Sterelny, K., & Griffiths, P. E. (1999). Sex and death: An introduction to the philosophy of biology. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  64. Thagard, P. (1978). The best explanation: Criteria for theory choice. Journal of Philosophy, 75, 76–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Thagard, P. (1992). Conceptual revolutions. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  66. Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1989). Adaptation versus phylogeny: The role of animal psychology in the study of human behavior. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 2, 175–188.Google Scholar
  67. Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1990a). The past explains the present: Emotional adaptations and the structure of ancestral environments. Ethology and Sociobiology, 11, 375–424.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1990b). On the universality of human nature and the uniqueness of the individual: The role of genetics and adaptation. Journal of Personality, 58, 17–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1992). The psychological foundations of culture. In J. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  70. Whiten, A., & Byrne, R. W. (Eds.). (1997). Machiavellian intelligence II: Extensions and evaluations. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  71. Williams, G. C. (1966). Adaptation and natural selection. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  72. Wilson, D. S. (1994). Adaptive genetic variation and human evolutionary psychology. Ethology and Sociobiology, 15, 219–235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Wright, L. (1973). Functions. Philosophical Review, 82, 139–168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Zurif, E. B. (1995). Brains regions of relevance to syntactic processing. In L. R. Gleitman & M. Liberman (Eds.), An invitation to cognitive science (second edition): Language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of CanterburyChristchurchNew Zealand

Personalised recommendations