Advertisement

Higher Brain Functions

  • Rachel Casas
  • Daniel Tranel

Introduction

We will use the term higher brain functions to refer to the operations of the brain that stand at the pinnacle of evolution and that, in may respects, are unique to humans. Verbal communication, the capacity to “think in the future,” and the ability to hold multiple tracks of complex information “on-line” at the same time are examples of complex mental functions subserved by various structures in the human brain. The “higher” capacities of the human brain can also be captured under the terms cognition and behavior. Cognition comprises capacities such as intellectual function, memory, speech and language, complex perception, orientation, attention, judgment, planning, and decision-making. Behavior is the manifestation of these cognitive functions and also subsumes the important domain of emotional processing and regulation. Behavior is influenced and guided by another facet of higher brain function, namely, personality, which describes the psychological makeup, traits, and...

Keywords

Primary Visual Cortex Association Cortex Prefrontal Region Perirhinal Cortex Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Notes

Acknowledgments:

This work was supported by the American Psychological Association Diversity Program in Neuroscience Fellowship (R.C.), Program Project Grant NINDS NS19632 (D.T.), and NIDA R01 DA022549 (D.T.).

Selected Readings

  1. Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., & Damasio, A.R. (1998). The human amygdala in social judgment. Nature, 393, 470–474.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A.R. (1994). Impaired recognition of emotion in facial expressions following bilateral damage to the human amygdala. Nature, 372, 669–672.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Anderson, S.W., Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., & Damasio, A.R. (1999). Impairment of social and moral behavior related to early damage in the human prefrontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 1032–1037.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Baddeley, A.D. (1992). Working memory. Science, 255, 566–569.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bauer, R.M. (1993). Agnosia. In Clinical Neuropsychology, 3rd edition. Edited by Heilman KM, Valenstein E. New York, Oxford University Press, pp 215–278.Google Scholar
  6. Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., & Damasio, A.R. (1997). Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science, 275, 1293–1295.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bechara, A., Tranel, D., & Damasio, H. (2000). Characterization of the decision-making deficit of patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions. Brain, 123, 2189–2202.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Benton, A.L. (1988). Neuropsychology: Past, present, and future. In Handbook of Neuropsychology, Vol. 1. Edited by Boller F, Grafman J. Amsterdam, Elsevier, pp 1–27.Google Scholar
  9. Benton, A.L. (1991). The Hecaen-Zangwill legacy: Hemispheric dominance examined. Neuropsychology Review, 2, 267–280.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Benton, A., & Tranel, D. (2000). Historical notes on reorganization of function and neuroplasticity. In Cerebral Reorganization of Function After Brain Damage. Edited by Levin HS, Grafman J. New York: Oxford University Press, pp 3–23.Google Scholar
  11. Cabeza, R., & Nyberg, L. (2000). Imaging cognition II: An empirical review of 275 PET and fMRI studies. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 1–47.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Damasio, A.R. (1989). Time-locked multiregional retroactivation: a systems-level proposal for the neural substrates of recall and recognition. Cognition, 33, 25–62.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Damasio, A.R. (1994). Descartes' Error: Emotion, Reason, and the Human Brain. New York: Grossett/Putnam.Google Scholar
  14. Damasio, A.R. (1999). The Feeling of What Happens. New York: Harcourt Brace.Google Scholar
  15. Damasio, A.R., & Tranel, D. (1993). Nouns and verbs are retrieved with differently distributed neural systems. The Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 90, 4957–4960.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Damasio, A.R., Tranel, D., & Rizzo, M. (2000). Disorders of complex visual processing. In M.M. Mesulam (Ed.), Principles of Behavioral and Cognitive Neurology, 2nd edition. New York: Oxford University Press, pp 332–372.Google Scholar
  17. Damasio, H., & Damasio, A.R. (1997). The lesion method in behavioral neurology and neuropsychology. In Behavioral Neurology and Neuropsychology. Edited by Feinberg TE, Farah MJ. New York: McGraw-Hill, pp 69–82.Google Scholar
  18. Damasio, H., Grabowski, T.J., Tranel, D., Hichwa, R.D., & Damasio, A.R. (1996). A neural basis for lexical retrieval. Nature, 380, 499–505.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Denburg, N.L., & Tranel, D. (2003). Acalculia and disturbances of the body schema. In Clinical neuropsychology, Fourth Edition. Edited by Heilman KM, Valenstein E. New York: Oxford University Press, pp 161–184.Google Scholar
  20. Geschwind, N. (1965). Disconnexion syndromes in animals and man. Brain, 88, 237–294, 585–644.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Harlow, J.M. (1868). Recovery from the passage of an iron bar through the head. Publications of the Massachusetts Medical Society, 2, 327–347.Google Scholar
  22. Heilman, K.M., Watson, R.T., & Valenstein, E. (1993). Neglect and related disorders. In Clinical Neuropsychology, 3rd edition. Edited by Heilman KM, Valenstein E. New York: Oxford University Press, pp 279–336.Google Scholar
  23. Kopelman, M.D. (1992). The neuropsychology of remote memory. In Handbook of Neuropsychology, Vol. 8. Edited by Boller F, Grafman J. Amsterdam: Elsevier, pp 215–238.Google Scholar
  24. Levin, H.S., Eisenberg, H.M., & Benton, A.L. (editors) (1991). Frontal Lobe Function and Dysfunction. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  25. Lezak, M.D. (1995). Neuropsychological Assessment, 3rd edition. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  26. Martin, A., Haxby, J.V., Lalonde, F.M., Wiggs, C.L., & Ungerleider, L.G. (1995). Discrete cortical regions associated with knowledge of color and knowledge of action. Science, 270, 102–105.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Milner, B. (1972). Disorders of learning and memory after temporal lobe lesions in man. Clinical Neurosurgery, 19, 421–446.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Newcombe, F., & Ratcliff, G. (1989). Disorders of visuospatial analysis. In Handbook of Neuropsychology, Vol. 2. Edited by Boller F, Grafman J. Amsterdam: Elsevier, pp 333–356.Google Scholar
  29. Scoville, W.B., & Milner, B. (1957). Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 20, 11–21.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Smith, E.E., Jonides, J., & Koeppe, R.A. (1996). Dissociating verbal and spatial working memory using PET. Cerebral Cortex, 6, 11–20.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Sperry, R.W. (1968). The great cerebral commissure. Scientific American, 210, 42–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Squire, L.R. (1992). Memory and hippocampus: A synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans. Psychological Review, 99, 195–231.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Stuss, D.T., & Benson, D.F. (1986). The Frontal Lobes. New York: Raven Press.Google Scholar
  34. Teuber, H-L. (1968). Alteration of perception and memory in man: reflections on methods. In Analysis of Behavioral Change. Edited by Weiskrantz L. New York: Harper & Row, pp 274–328.Google Scholar
  35. Thompson, R.F.L. (1986). The neurobiology of learning and memory. Science, 233, 941–947.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Tranel, D., & Anderson, S. (1999). Syndromes of aphasia. In Concise Encyclopedia of Language Pathology. Edited by Fabbro F. Oxford, England: Elsevier Science Limited, pp 305–319.Google Scholar
  37. Tranel, D., Bechara, A., & Damasio, A.R. (2000). Decision making and the somatic marker hypothesis. In The New Cognitive Neurosciences. Edited by Gazzaniga MS. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press, pp 1047–1061.Google Scholar
  38. Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. R. (1993). The covert learning of affective valence does not require structures in hippocampal system or amygdala. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 79–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Tranel, D., & Damasio, A.R. (2000). Neuropsychology and behavioral neurology. In Handbook of Psychophysiology. Edited by Cacioppo JT, Tassinary LG, Berntson GG. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press, pp 119–141.Google Scholar
  40. Tranel, D., Damasio, A.R., Damasio, H., & Brandt, J.P. (1994). Sensorimotor skill learning in amnesia: Additional evidence for the neural basis of nondeclarative memory. Learning and Memory, 1, 165–179.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Tranel, D., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A.R. (1995). Double dissociation between overt and covert face recognition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7, 425–432.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Tranel, D., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A.R. (1997). A neural basis for the retrieval of conceptual knowledge. Neuropsychologia, 35, 1319–1327.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Tranel, D., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A.R. (2000). Amnesia caused by herpes simplex encephalitis, infarctions in basal forebrain, and anoxia/ischemia. In Handbook of neuropsychology, 2nd Edition; Volume 2. Edited by Boller F, Grafman J (Cermak L, Section Editor). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science, pp 85–110.Google Scholar
  44. Van Hoesen, G.W. (1982). The parahippocampal gyrus. Trends in Neurosciences, 5, 345–350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Humana Press, Totowa, NJ 2008

Authors and Affiliations

  • Rachel Casas
    • 1
  • Daniel Tranel
    • 2
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of Iowa Hospitals and ClinicsIowa CityUSA
  2. 2.Department of NeurologyUniversity of Iowa Hospitals and ClinicsIowa CityUSA

Personalised recommendations