Advertisement

Models of Developmental Psychopathology

  • Michael Lewis

Abstract

Models of development represent world views about human nature and environments that create a human life course (Lewis, 1972; Reese & Overton, 1970; Riegel, 1976). Models of abnormal development also reflect these views, and the data from normal and abnormal lives inform our theories of development. So, for example, the trait notion of personality (Block & Block, 1980) and the invulnerable child (Anthony, 1970; Garmezy, 1974; Rutter, 1981) both share the view that some fixed pattern of behavior may be unaffected by environmental factors. Likewise, information about the regression to old behavior patterns requires that we reconsider the notion that all developmental processes are transformational; that all old behavior patterns are changed into new ones. Clearly, models of development must be applicable for normal and for abnormal development.

Keywords

Transformational Model Environmental Model Maladaptive Behavior Insecure Attachment Active Child 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1973). The development of infant-mother attachment. In B. M. Caldwell & H. N. Ricciuti (Eds.), Review of child development research (Vol. 3, pp. 1–95). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  2. Allport, F. H. & Allport, G. W. (1921). Personality traits: Their Classification and measurement. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 16, 1–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Anthony, E. J. (1970). The behavior disorders of children. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Carmichael’s manual of child psychology (pp. 667–764). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  4. Barker, R. G. (1965). Explorations in ecological psychology. American Psychologist, 20, 1–11.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bellin, H. (1971). The development of physical concepts. In T. Michel (Ed.), Cognitive development and epistemology (pp. 85–119). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  6. Bern, S. L. (1987). Masculinity and femininity exist only in the mind of the perceiver. In J. M. Reinish, L. A. Rosenblum, & S.A. Sanders (Eds.), Masculinity/Femininity: Basic perspectives (pp. 304–314). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  7. Block, J., & Block, T. H. (1980). The role of ego control and ego resiliency in the organization of behavior. In W. A. Collins (Ed.), Minnesota symposium on child psychology (Vol. 13, pp. 325–377). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  8. Bornstein, M. H. (Ed.) (1987). Sensitive periods in development. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  9. Bowlby, J. (Ed.). (1969). Attachment and loss (Vol. 1). New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  10. Chess, S., & Thomas, A. (Eds.). (1984). Origins and evolution of behavior disorders. New York: Brunner/Mazel.Google Scholar
  11. Chomsky, N. (Ed.). (1957). Syntactic structures. The Hague, Netherlands: Mouton.Google Scholar
  12. Chomsky, N. (Ed.). (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  13. Clarke-Stewart, A. (1974). Interactions between mothers and their young children: Characteristics and consequences. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, J8(5, Serial No. 153).Google Scholar
  14. Cole, M., & Scribner, S. (Eds.). (1974). Culture and thought. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  15. Cole, M., Gay, J., Glick, J. A., & Sharp, D. W. (Eds.). (1971). The cultural context of learning and thinking. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  16. Dunn, J., & Kendrick, C. (1982). Siblings: Love, envy and understanding. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  17. Fagot, B. I., & Patterson, G. R. (1969). An in vivo analysis of reinforcing contingencies for sex role behaviors in the preschool child. Developmental Psychology, 1, 566–568.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Fredricksen, N. (1972). Toward a taxonomy of situations. American Psychologist, 27, 114–123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Garmezy, N. (1974). The study of competence in children at risk for severe psychopathology. In E. Anthony & C. Koupernik (Eds.), The child in his family (Vol. 3, pp. 77–98). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  20. Garmezy, N. (1989). Stress-resistant children: The search for protective factors. In J. E. Stevenson (Ed.), Aspects of current child psychiatry research (Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry Book Supplement No. 4, pp. 213–233). Oxford, England: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
  21. Garmezy, N., Masten, A. S., & Teilegen, A. (1984). The study of stress and competence in children: A building block for developmental psychopathology. Child Development, 55, 97–111.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Gibson, E. J. (1969). Principles of perceptual learning and development. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
  23. Gottesman, I., & Shields, J. (Eds.). (1982). Schizophrenia: The epigenetic puzzle. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  24. Hartup, W. W. (1979). Two social worlds: Family relations and peer relations. In M. Rutter (Ed.), Scientific foundations of developmental psychiatry (pp. 56–72). London: Heinemann.Google Scholar
  25. Kallman, F. J. (1946). The genetic theory of schizophrenia: An analysis of 691 schizophrenic twin index families. American Journal of Psychiatry, 103, 309–322.Google Scholar
  26. Kringlon, (1968). An epidemiological twin study of schizophrenia. In D. Rosenthal & S. Kety (Eds.), The transmission of schizophrenia (pp. 49–63). New York: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
  27. Lamb, M. E. (Ed.). (1980). The role of the father in child development (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  28. Lamb, M., Thompson, R., Gardner, W., & Charnov, E. (1985) Infant-mother attachment: The origins and developmental significance of individual differences in strange situation behavior. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  29. Lennenberg, E. H. (1967). Biological foundations of language. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  30. Lerner, R. H. (1984). On the nature of human plasticity. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Lewis, M. (1972). State as an infant-environment interaction: An analysis of mother-infant interaction as a function of sex. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 18, 95–121.Google Scholar
  32. Lewis, M. (1978). Situational analysis and the study of behavioral development. In L. Pervin & M. Lewis (Eds.), Perspectives in interactional psychology (pp. 49–66). New York: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Lewis, M. (1983). Newton, Einstein, Piaget, and the concept of the self. In L. S. Liben (Ed.), Piaget, and the foundations of knowledge (pp. 141–177). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  34. Lewis, M. (Ed.). (1984). Beyond the dyad (Vol. 4). New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  35. Lewis, M. (1987a). Early sex role behavior and school age adjustment. In J. M. Reinish, L. A. Rosenblum, & S. A. Sanders (Eds.), Masculinity/femininity: Basic perspectives (pp. 202–226). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  36. Lewis, M. (1987b). Social development in infancy and early childhood. In J. Osofsky (Ed.), Handbook of infancy (2nd ed., pp. 419–492). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  37. Lewis, M., & Ferring, C. (1981). Direct and indirect interactions in social relationships. In L. Lipsitt (Ed.), Advances in infancy research (Vol. 1, pp. 131–163). New York: Ablex.Google Scholar
  38. Lewis, M., & Feiring, C. (in press). Attachment as personal characteristic or a measure of the environment. In J. Gerwitz & B. Kurtines (Eds.), Intersections with attachment. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  39. Lewis, M., & Feiring, C. (1989). Infant, mother, and mother-infant interaction behavior and subsequent attachment. Child Development, 60, 831–837.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Lewis, M., & Rosenblum, L. (1975). Introduction. In M. Lewis & L. Rosenblum (Eds.), Friendship and peer relations: The origins of behavior (Vol. 4, pp. 1–9). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  41. Lewis, M., & Schaeffer, S. (1981). Peer behavior and mother-infant interaction in maltreated children. In M. Lewis & L. A. Rosenblum (Eds.), The uncommon child (Vol. 3, pp. 193 – 224). New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  42. Lewis, M., Feiring, C., McGuffog, C., & Jaskir, J. (1984). Predicting psychopathology in six-year-olds from early social relations. Child Development, 55, 123–126.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Luria, A. R. (1976). Cognitive development: Its cultural and social foundations. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  44. Masters, W. H., Johnson, V. E., & Kolodny, R. C. (Eds.). (1977). Ethical issues in sex therapy and research. Boston: Little, Brown.Google Scholar
  45. Mischel, W. (1965). Personality assessment. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  46. Parke, R., & O’Leary, S. (1975). Father-mother-infant interaction in the newborn period: Some findings, some observations, and some unrestricted issues. In K. Riegel & J. Meacham (Eds.), The developing individual in a changing world: Vol II. Social and environmental issues (pp. 653–663). The Hague, Netherlands: Mouton.Google Scholar
  47. Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children (M. Cook, Trans.). New York: International Universities Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Puig-Antich, J. (1982). Psychobiological correlates of major depressive disorder in children and adolescents. In L. Greenspan (Ed.), Psychiatry 1982: Annual Review (pp. 41–64). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
  49. Reese, H. W., & Overton, W. F. (1970). Models of development and theories of development. In L. R. Goulet & P. B. Baltes (Eds.), Life-span developmental psychology: Research and theory (pp. 115–145). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  50. Riegel, K. F. (1976). Psychology of development and history. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  51. Riegel, K. F. (1978). Psychology, mon amour: A countertext. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
  52. Rutter, M. (1979). Protective factors in children’s responses to stress and disadvantage. In M. W. Kent & J. G. Rolf (Eds.), Primary prevention of psychopathology: (Vol. 3). Social competence in children (pp. 150–162). Hanover, NH: University Press of New England.Google Scholar
  53. Rutter, M. (1981). Stress, coping and development. Some issues and some questions. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 22, 323–356.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Sameroff, A. (1975). Transactional models in early social relations. Human Development, 18, 65–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Sameroff, A., & Chandler, M. J. (1975). Reproductive risk and the continuum of caretaking causality. In F. D. Horowitz (Ed.), Review of child development research (Vol. 4, pp. 187–244). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  56. Schneider-Rosen, K., & Cicchetti, D. (1984). The relationship between affect and cognition in maltreated infants: Quality of attachment and the development of visual self-recognition. Child Development, 55, 648–658.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Singer, L. M., Brodzinsky, D. M., Ramsay, D., Steir, M., & Waters, E. (1985). Mother-infant attachment in adoptive families. Child Development, 56, 1543–1551.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  59. Szasz, T. S. (1961). The myth of mental illness. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
  60. Thomas, A., & Chess, S. (1977). Temperament and development. New York: Brunner/Mazel.Google Scholar
  61. Thompson, R. A., Lamb, M. E., & Estes, D. (1982). Stability of infant-mother attachment and its relationship to changing life circumstances in an unselected middle class sample. Child Development, 53, 144–148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Tinsley, B. R., & Parke, R. D. (1984). Grandparents as support and socialization agents. In M. Lewis (Ed.), Beyond the dyad (pp. 161–194). New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  63. Weinraub, M., Brooks, J., & Lewis, M. (1977). The social network: A reconsideration of the concept of attachment. Human Development, 20, 31–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Zahn-Waxler, C., Cummings, E. M., McKnew, D. H. & Radke-Yarrow, N. (1984). Altruism, aggression, and social interactions in young children with a manic-depressive parent. Child Development, 55, 112–122.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Press, New York 1990

Authors and Affiliations

  • Michael Lewis
    • 1
  1. 1.Institute for the Study of Child Development, Robert Wood Johnson Medical SchoolUniversity of Medicine and Dentistry of New JerseyNew BrunswickUSA

Personalised recommendations