Current Ornithology pp 33-66

Part of the Current Ornithology book series (CUOR, volume 13)

Predicting Cognitive Capacity from Natural History

Examples from Four Species of Corvids
  • Russell P. Balda
  • Alan C. Kamil
  • Peter A. Bednekoff

Abstract

Birds have been studied for centuries because they are numerous, conspicuous, and aesthetically pleasing to humans Despite their overall regard for birds, historically, many ornithologists have considered birds as instinct-driven organisms of little intellectual capacity. For example, the ornithological textbook of choice from the 1960s states the following view of avian intelligence:

Flight has proven to be an enormously successful evolutionary venture, but one that has cost birds dearly in mental development. In effect, flight has become a substitute for cleverness; birds solve many potential problems merely by flying away from them…. As a consequence, much [avian] behavior is, by mam-malian standards, fragmentary, stereotyped, and at times amazingly stupid. (Welty, 1962, p.159).

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Anderson, J. R., 1990, Cognitive Psychology and Its Implications, 3rd edition, Freeman and Co., New York.Google Scholar
  2. Andersson, M., 1982, Female choice selects for extreme tail length in a widowbird, Nature 299: 818–820.Google Scholar
  3. Armstrong, D. P., Gass, C.L., and Sutherland, G. D., 1987, Should foragers remember where they’ve been? Explorations of a simulation model based on the behavior and energetics of a territorial hummingbird, in: Foraging Behavior ( A. C. Kamil, J R. Krebs, and H. R. Pulliam, eds.), Plenum Press, New York, pp. 563–586.Google Scholar
  4. Balda, R. P., 1980, Recovery of cached seeds by a captive Nucifraga caryocatactes Z. Tierpsychol, 52: 331–346.Google Scholar
  5. Balda, R. P:, 1987, Avian impact on pinyon-juniper woodland, in: Proceedings—Pinyonjuniper Conference (R. L. Everett, ed.), U.S.D.A. Gen. Tech. Report INT-215, pp. 526–533.Google Scholar
  6. Balda, R. P., and Bateman, G. C., 1971, Flocking and annual cycle of the Pinyon Jay (Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus), Condor 73: 287–302.Google Scholar
  7. Balda, R. P., and Bateman, G. C., 1972, The breeding biology of the Pinyon Jay, Living Bird 11: 5–42.Google Scholar
  8. Balda, R. P., and Kamil, A. C., 1989, A comparative study of cache recovery by three corvid species, Anim. Behay. 38: 486–495.Google Scholar
  9. Barnea, A., and Nottebohm, F., 1994, Seasonal recruitment of hippocampal neurons in adult free-ranging Black-capped Chickadees, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 91: 11217–11221.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Basil, J. A., Kamil, A. C., Balda, R. P., and Fite, K. V., 1996, Differences in hippocampal volume among food-storing corvids, Brain Behay. Evol. 47: 156–164.Google Scholar
  11. Basolo, A. L., 1990, Female preference predates the evolution of the sword in swordtail fish, Science 250: 808–810.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Bateman, G. C., and Balda, R. P.,1973, Growth, development, and food habits of young Pinyon Jays, Auk 90:39–61.Google Scholar
  13. Beatty, W. W., and Shavalia, D. A., 1980, Spatial memory in rats: time course of working memory and effect of anesthetics, Behay. Neur. Biol. 28: 454–462.Google Scholar
  14. Bednekoff, P. A., and Balda, R. P., 1996a, Social caching and observational spatial memory in Pinyon Jays, Behaviour, 133: 807–826.Google Scholar
  15. Bednekoff, P. A., and Balda, R. P., 1996b, Observational spatial memory in Clark’s Nutcrackers and Mexican Jays, Anim. Behay. 52: 833–839.Google Scholar
  16. Bednekoff, P. A., and Balda, R. P., 1996b, Observational spatial memory in Clark’s Nutcrackers and Mexican Jays, Anim. Behay. 52: 833–839.Google Scholar
  17. Beecher, M. D., Campbell, S. E., and Burt, J. M., 1994, Song perception in the Song Sparrow: birds classify by song type but not by singer, Anim. Behay. 43: 283–287.Google Scholar
  18. Benkman, C. W., Balda, R. P., and Smith, C. C., 1984, Adaptations for seed dispersal and the compromises due to seed predation in limber pine, Ecology 65: 632–642.Google Scholar
  19. Berger, L. R., and Ligon, J. D., 1977, Vocal communication and individual recognition in the Pinon Jay Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus, Anim. Behay. 25: 567–584.Google Scholar
  20. Bitterman, M. E., 1960, Toward a comparative psychology of learning, Am. Psycho. 15: 704–712.Google Scholar
  21. Bock, W. J., Balda, R. P., and Vander Wall, S. B.,1973, Morphology of the sublingual pouch and tongue musculature in Clark’s Nutcracker, Auk 90: 491–519.Google Scholar
  22. Brodbeck, D. R., 1994, Memory for spatial and local cues: a comparison of a storing and a nonstoring species, Anim. Learn. Behay. 22: 119–133.Google Scholar
  23. Brooks, D. R., and McLennan, D. A., 1991, Phylogeny, Ecology, and Behavior, University of Chicago Press, Chicago.Google Scholar
  24. Brown, J. L., and Brown, E. R., 1981, Kin selection and individual selection in babblers, in: Natural Selection and Social Behavior: Recent Research and New Theory ( R. D. Alexander and D. W. Tinkle, eds.), Chiron Press, New York, pp. 244–256.Google Scholar
  25. Brown, J. L., and Brown, E. R., 1990, Mexican jays: uncooperative breeding, in: Cooperative Breeding in Birds( P. B. Stacy, and W. D. Koenig, eds.), Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, England, pp. 267–288.Google Scholar
  26. Catchpole, C. K., 1986, Song repertories and reproductive success in the Great Reed Warbler, Acrocephalus arundinaceus, Behay. Ecol. Sociobiol. 19: 439–445.Google Scholar
  27. Catchpole, C. K., Leisler, B., and Dittami, J., 1986, Sexual differences in the responses of captive Great Reed Warblers (Acrocephalus arundinaceus) to variation in song structure and size, Ethology 72: 69–77.Google Scholar
  28. Chance, M. R. A., and Mead, A. P., 1953, Social behavior and primate evolution, Evolution 7: 395–439.Google Scholar
  29. Cheney, D. L., and Seyfarth, R. M., 1990, How Monkeys See the World, University of Chicago Press, Chicago.Google Scholar
  30. Clayton, N. S., 1992, The ontogeny of food-storing and retrieval in Marsh Tits, Behaviour 122: 11–25.Google Scholar
  31. Clayton, N. S., 1994, The role of age and experience in the behavioural development of food-storing and retrieval in Marsh Tits, Parus palustris, Anim. Behay. 47: 1435–1444.Google Scholar
  32. Clayton, N. S., 1995a, Development of memory and the hippocampus: Comparison of food-storing and nonstoring birds on a one-trial associative memory task, J. Neurosci. 15: 2796–2807.Google Scholar
  33. Clayton, N. S., 1995b, The neuroethological development of food-storing memory: a case of use it or lose it! Behay. Brain Res. 70: 95–102.Google Scholar
  34. Clayton, N. S., and Krebs, J. R., 1993, Lateralization in Paridae: Comparison of a storing and a non-storing species on a one-trial associative memory task, J. Comp. Physiol. A 171: 807–815.Google Scholar
  35. Clayton, N. S., and Krebs, J. R., 1994a, One-trial associative memory: Comparison of food-storing and nonstoring species of birds, Anim. Learn. Behay. 22: 366–372.Google Scholar
  36. Clayton, N. S., and Krebs, J. R., 1994b, Memory for spatial and object-specific cues in food-storing and non-storing birds, J. Comp. Physiol. A 174: 371–379.Google Scholar
  37. Cole, S., Hainsworth, F. R., Kamil, A. C., Mercier, T., and Wolf, L. L., 1982, Spatial learning as an adaptation in hummingbirds, Science 217: 655–657.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Crook, J. H., 1964, The evolution of social organization and visual communication in the weaverbirds (Ploceidae), Behay. Suppl. 10: 1–178.Google Scholar
  39. Crook, J. H., 1965, The adaptive significance of avian social organization, Sympos. Zool. Soc. London 14: 181–218.Google Scholar
  40. Davies, N. B., 1992, Dunnock Behavior and Social Evolution, Oxford University Press, Oxford, England.Google Scholar
  41. Dimmick, C. R., 1993, Life History and the Development of Cache-recovery Behaviors in Clark’s Nutcrackers, Unpublished Ph. D. Dissertation, Northern Arizona University, Flagstaff.Google Scholar
  42. Dukas, R., and Real, L. A., 1991, Learning foraging tasks by bees: a comparison between social and solitary species, Anim. Behay. 44: 269–274.Google Scholar
  43. Emlen, S. T., and Wrege P. H., 1988, The role of kinship in helping decisions among White-fronted Bee-eaters, Behay. Ecol. Sociobiol. 23: 305–315.Google Scholar
  44. Endler, J. A., 1980, Natural selection on color patterns in Poecilia reticulata, Evolution 34: 76–91.Google Scholar
  45. Falls, J. B., 1992, Playback: a historical perspective, in: Playback and Studies in Animal Communication (P. K. McGregor, ed.), Plenum Press, New York, pp. 11–33. Felsenstein, J., 1985, Phylogenies and the comparative method, Am. Nat. 125: 1–15.Google Scholar
  46. Felsenstein, J. 1988, Phylogenies and quantitative characters, Ann. Rev. Ecol. System. 19: 445–471.Google Scholar
  47. Gardner, H., 1987, The Mind’s New Science: A History of the Cognitive Revolution, 2nd. edition, Basic Books, New York.Google Scholar
  48. Gaulin, S. J. C., and FitzGerald, R. W., 1986, Sex differences in spatial ability: an evolutionary hypothesis and test, Amer. Natl. 127: 74–88.Google Scholar
  49. Gaulin, S. J. C., and FitzGerald, R. W., 1989, Sexual selection for spatial-learning ability, Anim. Behay. 37: 322–331.Google Scholar
  50. Gill, F. B., 1988, Trapline foraging by Hermit Hummingbirds: competition for an undefended, renewable resource, Ecology 69: 1933–1942.Google Scholar
  51. Gillan, J., 1981, Reasoning in the chimpanzee, II. Transitive inference, J. Exp. Psycho. Anim. Behay. Proc. 7: 150–164.Google Scholar
  52. Giuntoli, M., and Mewaldt, L. R., 1978, Stomach contents of Clark’s Nutcrackers collected in western Montana, Auk 95: 595–598.Google Scholar
  53. Goodwin, D., 1986, Crows of the World, 2nd edition, British Museum of Natural History, London.Google Scholar
  54. Goss-Custard, J. D., 1981, Feeding behavior of Red Shank, Tringa totanus, and optimal foraging theory, in: Foraging Behavior: Ecological, Ethological, and Psychological Approaches (A. C. Kamil and T. D. Sargent, eds.), New York, Garland Press, pp. 115134.Google Scholar
  55. Grant, B. R., and Grant, P. R., 1989, Natural selection in a population of Darwin’s Finches, Am. Nat. 133: 377–393.Google Scholar
  56. Guntert, M., Hay, D. B., and Balda, R. P., 1988, Communal roosting of the Pygmy Nuthatch: a winter survival strategy, in: Proceedings of the International Ornithological Congress ( H. Ouellet, ed.), University of Ottawa Press, Ottawa, pp. 1963–1972.Google Scholar
  57. Harvey, P. H., and Pagel, M. D., 1991, The Comparative Method in Evolutionary Biology, Oxford University Press, Oxford, England.Google Scholar
  58. Healy, S. D., and Krebs, J. R., 1992, Food storing and the hippocampus in corvids: amount and volume are correlated, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B248: 241–245.Google Scholar
  59. Healy, S. D., Clayton, N. S., and Krebs, J. R., 1994, Development of hippocampal spe-cialisation in two species of tits (Parus spp.), Behay. Brain Res. 61: 23–28Google Scholar
  60. Herrnstein, R. J., 1985, Riddles of natural categorization, in: Animal Intelligence ( L. Weiskrantz, ed.), Clarendon Press, Oxford, England, pp. 129–142.Google Scholar
  61. Hilton, S. C., and Krebs, J. R., 1990, Spatial memory of four species of Parus:performance in an open-field analogue of a radial maze,Quart. J Exper. Psychol.42B: 345–368.Google Scholar
  62. Hollis, K. L., 1984, The biological function of Pavlovian conditioning: the best defense is a good offense, J. Exper. Psychol.: Anim. Behay. Proc. 10: 413–425.Google Scholar
  63. Hope, S., 1989, Phylogeny of the Avian Family Corvidae, Unpublished Ph. D. Dissertation, City University of New York, New York.Google Scholar
  64. Humphrey, N. K., 1976, The social function of intellect, in: Growing Points in Ethology ( P. P. G. Bateson and R. A. Hinde, eds.), Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, England, pp. 303–317.Google Scholar
  65. Jacobs, L. F., Gaulin, S. J. C., Sherry, D. F., and Hoffman, G. E., 1990, Evolution of spatial cognition: sex-specific patterns of spatial behavior predict hippocampal size,Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 87: 6349–6352.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  66. Jolly, A., 1966, Lemur social behaviour and primate intelligence, Science 153: 501–506.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  67. Kamil, A. C., 1978, Systematic foraging by a nectar-feeding bird, the Amakihi (Loxops virens), J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 92: 388–396.Google Scholar
  68. Kamil, A. C., 1988a, A synthetic approach to the study of animal intelligence, in: Comparative Perspectives in Modern Psychology, Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, volume 35, ( D. W. Leger, ed.), University of Nebraska Press, Lincoln, pp. 230–257.Google Scholar
  69. Kamil, A. C., 1988b, Experimental design in ornithology, in: Current Ornithology, volume 5 ( D. M. Power, ed.), Plenum Press, New York, pp. 313–346.Google Scholar
  70. Kamil, A. C., and Yoerg, S. I., 1982, Learning and foraging behavior, in: Perspectives on Ethology, volume 5 ( P. P. G. Bateson and P. H. Klopfer, eds.), Plenum Press, New York, pp. 325–364.Google Scholar
  71. Kamil, A. C., Jones, T. B., Pietrewicz, A. T., and Maudlin, J., 1977, Positive transfer from successive reversal training to learning set in Blue Jays, J. Comp. Physiol. 91: 79–86.Google Scholar
  72. Kamil, A. C., Balda, R. P., and Olson, D. J., 1994, Performance of four seed-caching corvid species in the radial-arm maze analog, J. Comp. Psychol.:Anim. Behay. Proc. 108: 385–393.Google Scholar
  73. Ketterson, E. D., Nolan, V. Jr., Cawthorn, M. J., Parker, P. G., and Ziegenfus, C., 1996, Phenotypic engineering: using hormones to explore the mechanistic and functional bases of phenotypic variation in nature, Ibis 138: 70–86.Google Scholar
  74. Koenig, W. D., and Stacy, P. B., 1990, Acorn Woodpeckers: group living and food storage under contrasting ecological conditions, in: Cooperative Breeding in Birds( P. B. Stacy and W. D. Koenig, eds.), Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, England, pp. 415–453.Google Scholar
  75. Konishi, M., and Nottebohm, F., 1969, Experimental studies in the ontogeny of avian vocalization, in: Bird Vocalization: their Relation to Current Problems in Biology and Psychology ( R. A. Hinde, ed.), Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, England, pp. 29–48.Google Scholar
  76. Krebs, J. R., and Davies, N., eds., 1984, Behavioural Ecology, Sinauer Associates Inc., Sunderland, Massachusetts.Google Scholar
  77. Krebs, J. R., Erichsen, T. J., Webber, M. I., and Charnov, E. L., 1977, Optimal prey selection in the Great Tit (Parus major), Anim. Behay. 25: 30–38.Google Scholar
  78. Krebs, J. R., Kacelnik, A., and Taylor, P., 1978, Test of optimal sampling by foraging Great Tits, Nature 275: 27–31.Google Scholar
  79. Krebs, J. R., Sherry, D. F., Healy, S. D., Perry, V. H., and Vaccarino, A. L., 1989, Hippocampal specialization of food-storing birds, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 86: 1388–1392.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  80. Krebs, J. R., Healy, S. D., and Shettleworth, S., 1990, Spatial memory of Paridae: Comparison of a storing and non-storing species, P. ater and P. major, Anim. Behay. 39: 1127–1138.Google Scholar
  81. Krushinskya, N. L., 1966, Some complex forms of feeding behavior of nutcracker, Nucifraga caryocatactes, after removal of old cortex, Z. Evoluz. Biochem. Fisiolog. II, 563–568.Google Scholar
  82. Lack, D., 1947, Darwin’s Finches, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, England.Google Scholar
  83. Ligon, J. D., 1978, Reproductive interdependence of Pinyon Jays and pinyon pines, Ecol. Monog. 48: 111–126.Google Scholar
  84. Ligon, J. D., and Ligon, S. H., 1983, Reciprocity in the Green Woodhoopoe (Phoeniculus purpureus), Anim. Behay. 31: 480–489.Google Scholar
  85. Ligon, J. D., and Ligon, S. H., 1988, Territorial quality: key determinant of fitness in the group-living Green Woodhoopoe, in: The Ecology of Social Behavior ( C. N. Slobodchkoff, ed.), Academic Press, San Diego, pp. 229–253.Google Scholar
  86. Ligon, J. D., and Martin, D. J., 1974, Pinyon seed assessment by the Pinyon Jay, Anim. Behay. 22: 421–429.Google Scholar
  87. Lima, S. L., 1984, Downy Woodpecker foraging behavior: efficient sampling in simple stochastic environments, Ecology 65: 166–174.Google Scholar
  88. Lorenz, K., 1941, Vergleichende bewegungsstudien bei Anatiden, J. Ornithol. 89: 194–294.Google Scholar
  89. MacArthur, R. H., and Pianka, E. R., 1966, On optimal use of a patchy environment. Am. Nat. 100: 603–609.Google Scholar
  90. Marler, P., 1969, Tonal quality of bird sounds, in: Bird Vocalizations ( R. A. Hinde, ed.), Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, England, pp. 5–18.Google Scholar
  91. Marzluff, J. M., and Balda, R. P., 1992, The Pinyon Jay: Behavioral Ecology of a Colonial and Cooperative Corvid, Academic Press, London.Google Scholar
  92. McDonald, M. V., 1989, Function of song in Scott’s Seaside Sparrow, Ammodramus maritimus penensulae, Anim. Behay. 38: 468–485.Google Scholar
  93. McGowan, K. J., and Woolfenden, G. E., 1989, A sentinel system in the Florida Scrub Jay, Anim. Behay. 37: 1000–1006.Google Scholar
  94. McLennon, D. A., and McPhail, J. D., 1989, Experimental investigations of the evolutionary significance of sexually dimorphic nuptial colouration in Gasterosteus aculeatus (L.): Temporal changes in the structure of the male mosaic signal, Canad. J. Zool. 67: 1767–1777.Google Scholar
  95. McLennon, D. A., and McPhail, J. D., 1990, Experimental investigations on the evolutionary significance of sexually dimorphic nuptial colouration in Gasterosteus aculeatus (L.): The relationship between male colours and female behaviour, Canad. J. Zool. 68: 482–492.Google Scholar
  96. Mewaldt, L. R., 1956, Nesting behavior of the Clark’s Nutcracker, Condor 58: 3–23.Google Scholar
  97. Morton, E. S., 1977, On the occurrence and significance of motivation-structural rules in some bird and mammal sounds, Am. Nat. 111: 855–869.Google Scholar
  98. Morse, D. H., and Fritz, R. S., 1987, The consequences of foraging for reproductive success, in: Foraging Behavior ( A. C. Kamil, J. R. Krebs, and H. R. Pulliam, eds.), Plenum Press, New York, pp. 443–455.Google Scholar
  99. Moss, C. J., 1988, Elephant Memories, Houghton Mifflin, Boston.Google Scholar
  100. Myberg, A. A., Jr., and Riggio, R. J., 1985, Acoustically mediated individual recognition by a coral reef fish (Pomacentrus partitus), Anim. Behay. 33: 411–416.Google Scholar
  101. Olson, D. J., 1991, Species differences in spatial memory among Clark’s Nutcrackers, Scrub Jays, and Pigeons, J. Exper. Psycho.: Anim. Behav. Proc. 104: 326–334.Google Scholar
  102. Olson, D. J., Kamil, A. C., Balda, R. P., and Nims, P. J., 1995, Performance of four seed-caching corvid species in operant tests of nonspatial and spatial memory, J. Comp. Psycho.: Anim. Behay Proc. 109: 173–181.Google Scholar
  103. Olton, D. S., and Collison, C., 1979, Intramaze cues and `odor trails“ fail to direct choice behavior on an elevated maze, Anim. Learn. Behay. 7: 221–223.Google Scholar
  104. Olton, D. S., and Samuelson, R. J., 1976, Remembrance of places passed: spatial memory in rats, J. Exper. Psycho. Anim. Behay. Proc. 2: 97–116.Google Scholar
  105. Pepperberg, I. M., 1990, Some cognitive capacities of an African Grey Parrot (Psittacus erithacus), in: Advances inthe Study of Behavior ( P. J. B. Slater, J. S. Rosenblatt, and C. Beer, eds.), Academic Press, New York, pp. 357–409.Google Scholar
  106. Peterson, A. T., 1993, Adaptive geographical variation in bill shape of Scrub Jays, Am. Nat. 142: 508–527.Google Scholar
  107. Pettifor, R. A., Perrins, C. M., and McCleery, R. H., 1988, Individual optimization clutch size in great tits, Nature 336: 160–152.Google Scholar
  108. Pulliam, H. R., 1980, Do Chipping Sparrows forage optimally?, Ardea 68: 75–82.Google Scholar
  109. Pulliam, H. R., 1981, Learning to forage optimally, in: Foraging Behavior: Ecological, Ethological, and Psychological Approaches ( A. C. Kamil, and T. D. Sargent, eds.), Garland Press, New York, pp. 379–388.Google Scholar
  110. Real, L. A., 1991, Animal choice behavior and the evolution of cognitive architecture, Science 253: 980–986.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  111. Roitblat, H. L., Beaver T., and Terrace, H., eds., 1984, AnimalCognition, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, New Jersey.Google Scholar
  112. Sherry, D. F., and Schacter, D. L., 1987, The evolution of multiple memory systems, Psycho. Rev. 94: 439–454.Google Scholar
  113. Sherry, D. F., and Vaccarino, A. L., 1989, Hippocampal aspiration disrupts cache recovery in Black-capped Chickadees, Behay. Neurosci. 103: 308–318.Google Scholar
  114. Sherry, D. F., Vaccarino, A. L., Buckenham, K., and Herz, R., 1989, The hippocampal complex of food-storing birds, Brain Behay. Evol. 34: 308–317.Google Scholar
  115. Sherry, D. F., Forbes, M. R. L., Khurgel, M., and Ivy, G. O., 1993, Females have a larger hippocampus than males in the brood-parasitic Brown-headed Cowbird, Proc. Natl. Mad. Sci. 90: 7839–7843.Google Scholar
  116. Shettleworth, S. J., 1984, Learning and behavioral ecology, in: Behavioral Ecology, 2nd edition, ( J. R. Krebs, and N. B. Davies, eds.) Sinauer Associates, Sunderland, Massachusetts, pp. 170–194.Google Scholar
  117. Shettleworth, S. J., 1993, Where is the comparison in comparative cognition? Psychol. Sci. 4: 179–184.Google Scholar
  118. Spector, D., 1992, Wood-warbler song systems: a review of paruline singing behavior, in: CurrentOrnithology, Volume 9, (D. M. Powers, ed.), pp. 199–238.Google Scholar
  119. Staicer, C. A. 1991, Characteristics, use, and significance of two singing behaviors in Grace’s Warbler (Dendrocia graciae), Auk 106: 49–63.Google Scholar
  120. Stoddard, P. K., 1996, Vocal recognition of neighbors by territorial passerines, in: Ecology and Evolution of Acoustic Communication in Birds, ( D. E. Kroodsma, and E. H. Miller, eds.) Cornell University Press, Ithaca, New York, pp. 356–374.Google Scholar
  121. Sydeman, W. J., Guntert, M., and Balda, R. P., 1988, Annual reproductive yield in the cooperative Pygmy Nuthatch (Sitta pygmaea), Auk 105: 70–77.Google Scholar
  122. Tinbergen, N., 1960, The Herring Gull’s World, Doubleday, Garden City, New York.Google Scholar
  123. Tinbergen, N., 1963, The shell menace. Nat. Hist. 72: 28–35.Google Scholar
  124. Tinbergen, N., Impekoven, M., and Frank, D., 1967, An experiment on spacing-out as a defense against predation, Behaviour 28: 307–321.Google Scholar
  125. Tomback, D. F., 1980, How nutcrackers find their seed stores, Condor 82: 10–19.Google Scholar
  126. Vander Wall, S. B., and Balda, R. P., 1977, Coadaptations of the Clark’s Nutcracker and the pinyon pine for efficient seed harvest and dispersal, Ecol. Monog. 47: 89–111.Google Scholar
  127. Vander Wall, S. B., and Balda, R. P., 1981, Ecology and evolution of food-storage behavior in conifer-seed caching corvids, Z. Tierpsychol. 56: 217–242.Google Scholar
  128. Vander Wall, S. B., and Hutchins, H. E., 1983, Dependence of Clark’s Nutcracker (Nucifraga columbiana) on conifer seeds during the postfledgling period, Can. Field Natl. 97: 89–111.Google Scholar
  129. Vaughan, J. W., 1988, Formation of equivalence sets in pigeons, J. Exper. Psychol.: Anim. Behay.Proces. 14: 36–42.Google Scholar
  130. Weary, D. M., and Krebs, J. R., 1992, Great Tits classify songs by individual voice characteristics, Anim. Behay. 43: 283–287.Google Scholar
  131. Welty, J. C., 1962, The Life of Birds, W. B. Saunders Co., New York, New York.Google Scholar
  132. Whiten, A., and Byrne, R. W., 1988, The Machiavellian intelligence hypotheses (edi-torial), in: Machiavellian Intelligence ( R. W. Byrne, and A. Whiten, eds.), Clarendon Press, Oxford, England, pp. 1–9.Google Scholar
  133. Williams, G. C., 1966, Adaptation and Natural Selection, Princeton University Press, Princeton, New Jersey.Google Scholar
  134. Woolfenden, G. E., and Fitzpatrick, J. W., 1984, The Florida Scrub Jay: Demography of a Cooperative-breeding Bird, Monogr. Pop. Biol. 20, Princeton University Press, Princeton, New Jersey.Google Scholar
  135. Wrege, P. H., and Emlen, S. T., 1994, Family structure influences mate choice in White-fronted Bee-eaters, Behay. Ecol. Evol. 35: 185–193.Google Scholar
  136. Wunderle, J. M., and Martinez, J. S., 1987, Spatial learning in the nectarivorous banana-quit: Juveniles versus adults, Anim. Behay. 35: 652–658.Google Scholar
  137. Wynne, C. D. L., 1995, Reinforcement accounts for transitive inference performance, Anim. Learn. Behay. 23: 207–217.Google Scholar
  138. Yoerg, S. I., and Kamil, A C, 1988, Diet choices of Blue Jays (Cyanocitta cristata) as a function of time spent foraging, J. Comp. Psychol. 102: 230–235.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 1996

Authors and Affiliations

  • Russell P. Balda
    • 1
  • Alan C. Kamil
    • 2
  • Peter A. Bednekoff
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Biological SciencesNorthern Arizona UniversityFlagstaffUSA
  2. 2.School of Biological SciencesUniversity of NebraskaLincolnUSA
  3. 3.Institute of Applied Mathematics and Centre for Biodiversity ResearchUniversity of British ColumbiaVancouverCanada

Personalised recommendations