Advertisement

Cognitive Phenomenology in the Study of Tibetan Meditation: Phenomenological Descriptions Versus Meditation Styles

  • Olga Louchakova-SchwartzEmail author
Chapter

Abstract

This chapter discusses the inclusion of phenomenological analysis in the cognitive psychological research of meditation. Different meditation styles involve the specific changes of the mind, such as a long-lasting, vivid, and stable mental imagery in some types of Tibetan meditation. Comparative phenomenological analysis of the Deity Yoga, mandala, Vipashyana, and Rig-pa types of Tibetan meditation was included in designing a cognitive experiment. Results indicate the increase of visual working memory due to the practice of Deity Yoga, suggesting access to the heightened visual processing resources (Kozhevnikov, Louchakova, Josipovic, & Motes. Psychological Science 20(5):645–653, 2009). The phenomenological part of the design, reported here for the first time, included a new methodology termed phenomenological-cognitive mapping (PCM). PCM linked the comparative phenomenological analysis of meditation to the psychological parameters of cognitive testing. PCM proved to be crucial in designing a successful experiment that led to novel findings. In the absence of PCM, isolating the meditation style that causes the optimization of visual-spatial processing and finding at what stage of meditation this happens would not be possible. This chapter argues in favor of including a detailed phenomenological analysis of experience in the cognitive research of meditation, as opposed to using only general classifications such as meditation styles.

Keywords

phenomenology visual cognition consciousness Tibetan meditation meditation styles non-dual consciousness open awareness non-conceptual state 

References

  1. Aftanas, L. I., & Golocheikine, S. A. (2001). Human anterior and frontal midline theta and lower alpha reflect emotionally positive state and internalized attention: High resolution EEG investigation of meditation. Neuroscience Letters, 310(1), 57–60.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Aftanas, L. I., & Golocheikine, S. A. (2002). Non-linear dynamic complexity of the human EEG during meditation. Neuroscience Letters, 330, 143–146.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Ales Bello, A. (2009). The divine in Husserl and other explorations. Analecta Husserliana: Vol. 98. New York, NY: Springer.Google Scholar
  4. Baars, B. J. (1999) Attention vs. consciousness in the visual brain: Differences in conception, behavior, neuroanatomy, and physiology. Journal of General Psychology, 126 (3), 224–233.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bader, J. (1990). Meditation in Shankara’s Vedanta. New Delhi, India: Aditya Prakashan.Google Scholar
  6. Baerentsen, K. B., Stødkilde-Jørgensen, H., Sommerlund, B., Hartmann, T., Damsgaard-Madsen, J.,… Green, A. C. (2010). An investigation of brain processes supporting meditation. Cognitive Processing, 11(1), 57–84.Google Scholar
  7. Banquet, J. P., & Lesèvre, N. (1980). Event-related potentials in altered states of consciousness. Progress in Brain Research, 54, 447–453.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Barinaga, M. (2003). Buddhism and neuroscience. Studying a well trained mind. Science, 302, 5642–5644.Google Scholar
  9. Bechtel, W., & Stuffebeam, R. S. (2001). Epistemic issues in procuring evidence about the brain. In W. Bechtel, R. Stufflebeam, J. Mundale, & P. Mandik (Eds.), Philosophy and the neurosciences: A reader (pp. 55–81). Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
  10. Benchenane, K., Tiesinga, P. H., & Battaglia, F. P. (2011). Oscillations in the prefrontal cortex: A gateway to memory and attention. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. Advance online publication. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2011.01.004Google Scholar
  11. Biswal, B. B., Mennes, M., Zuo, X. N., Gohel, S., Kelly, C., Smith, S. M.,...Milham, M. P. (2010).Toward discovery science of human brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107(10), 4734–4739.Google Scholar
  12. Brefczynski-Lewis, J. A., Lutz, A., Schaefer, H. S., Levinson, D. B., & Davidson, R. (2007). Neural correlates of attentional expertise in long-term meditation practitioners. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(27), 11483–11488.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Cahn, B. R., Delorme, A., & Polish, J. (2010). Occipital gamma activation during Vipassana meditation. Cognitive Process, 11, 39–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Cahn, B. R., & Polich, J. (2006). Meditation states and traits: EEG, ERP, and neuroimaging studies. Psychological Bulletin, 132(2), 180–211.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Camic, P. J., Rhodes, J., & Yardley, L. (Eds.), Qualitative research in psychology: Expanding perspectives in methodology and design (pp. 243–273). Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
  16. Carruthers, P., & Veillet, B. (2012). The case against cognitive phenomenology. In T. Bayne & M. Montague (Eds.), Cognitive phenomenology (pp. 35–55). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  17. Chan, D., & Woolacott, M. (2007). Effects of level of meditation experience on attentional focus: Is the efficiency of executive or orientation networks improved? The Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, 13(6), 651–657.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Chen, T. L., Babiloni, C., Ferretti, A., Perrucci, M. G., Romani, G. L., Rissini, P. M.,...Del Gratta, C. (2010). Effects of somatosensory stimulation and attention on human somatosensory cortex: an fMRI study. Neuroimage, 53(1), 181–188.Google Scholar
  19. Chiesa, A. (2010). Vipassana meditation: Systematic review of current evidence. Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, 16(1), 37–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Cowan, N. (2001). The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behavioral Brain Science, 24, 87–118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Craig, A. D. (2010). The sentient self. Brain Structure and Function, 214(5–6), 563–77.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Davanger, S., Ellingsen, O. Holen, A., & Higdahl, K. (2010). Meditation-specific prefrontal cortical activation during acem meditation: An fMRI study. Perception & Motor Skills, 111(1), 291–306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Davidson, R. J., Goleman, D. J., & Schwartz, G. E. (1976). Attentional and affective concomitants of meditation: A cross-sectional study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 85(2), 235–238.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Dehaene, S., & Naccache, L. (2001). Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: Basic evidence and a workspace framework. Cognition, 79(1–2), 1–37.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Dillon, M. C. 1997. Merleau-Ponty’s ontology. 2nd ed., Northwestern University studies in phenomenology and existential philosophy. Evanston, Ill: Northwestern University Press.Google Scholar
  26. Doucet, G., Naveau, M., Petit, L., Delcroix, N., Zago, L., Crivello, F.,...Joliot, M. (2011). Brain activity at rest: A multi-scale hierarchical functional organization. Journal of Neurophysiology. Advance online publication. doi:10.1152/jn.00895.2010Google Scholar
  27. Erfani, A., & Erfanian, A. (2004). The effects of mental practice and concentration skills on EEG brain dynamics during motor imagery using independent component analysis. Conference Proceedings of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society, 1, 239–42.Google Scholar
  28. Fan, J., Byrne, J., Worden, M. S., Guise, K. G., McCandliss, B. D., Fossella, J., & Posner, M. I. (2007). The relation of brain oscillations to attentional networks. The Journal of Neuroscience, 27(23), 6197–6206.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Fasching, W. (2008). Consciousness, self-consciousness, and meditation. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 7, 463–483.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Fell, J., Axmacher, N., & Haupt, S. (2010). From alpha to gamma: Electrophysiological correlates of meditation-related states of consciousness. Medical Hypothesis, 75(2), 218–224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Finlay, L. (2008). A dance between the reduction and reflexivity: Explicating the “Phenomenological Psychological Attitude.” Journal of Phenomenological Psychology, 39, 1–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Forman, R. K. C. (1990). Introduction: Mysticism, constructivism and forgetting. In R. K. C. Forman (Ed.), The problem of pure consciousness (pp. 3–49). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  33. Freeman, W. J. (2009). Vortices in brain activity: Their mechanism and significance for perception. Neural Networks, 22, 491–501.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Frie, R. (Ed.). (2003). Understanding experience: Psychotherapy and postmodernism. New York: NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
  35. Fujiki, M., Akiyoshi, J., Yoshida, T., Tabata, M., Kabsawa, H., Kobayashi, H., & Shimomura, T. (2008). Functional brain mapping during recitation of Buddhist scriptures and repetition of the Namu Amida Butsu; A study in experienced Japanese monks. Turkish Neurosurgery, 18(2), 134–141.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Gallagher, S. (1997). Mutual enlightenment: Recent phenomenology in cognitive science. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 4(3), 195–214.Google Scholar
  37. Goleman, D. (1988). The meditative mind: The varieties of meditative experience. New York, NY: Penguin, Putman.Google Scholar
  38. Green, R., & Turner, G. (2010). Growing evidence for the influence of meditation on brain and behavior. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 20(2), 306–311.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Guenther, H. (1992). Meditation differently. Delhi, India: Motilal Banarsidas.Google Scholar
  40. Huang, H. Y., & Lo, P. C. (2009). EEG dynamics of experienced Zen meditation practitioners probed by complexity index and spectral measure. Journal of Medical Engineering and Technology, 33(4), 314–321.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Husserl, E. (1980). Ideas pertaining to a pure phenomenology and to a phenomenological philosophy. Collected works Edmund Husserl. The Hague, Boston, Hingham, MA, USA: M. Nijhoff.Google Scholar
  42. Jang, J. H., Jung, W. H., Kang, D. H., Byun, M. S., Kwon, S. J., Choi, C. H., & Kwon, J. S. (2011). Increased default mode network connectivity associated with meditation. Neuroscience Letters, 487(3), 358–62.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Jha, A. P., Krompinger, J., & Baime, M. J. (2007) Mindfulness training modifies subsystems of attention. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 7(2), 109–119.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Josipovic, Z. (2010). Duality and nonduality in meditation research. Consciousness and Cognition, 19(4), 1119–1121.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Kinsbourne, M. (2006). From unilateral neglect to the brain basis of consciousness. Cortex, 42, 869–874.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Kosslyn, S. M. (1995). Mental imagery. In S. M. Kosslyn & D. N. Osherson (Eds.), Visual cognition: An invitation to cognitive science (Vol. 2) (pp. 267–296). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
  47. Kosslyn, S. M., Seger, C., Pani, J. R., & Hillger, L. A. (1990). When is imagery used in everyday life? A diary study. Journal of Mental Imagery, 14(3&4), 131–152.Google Scholar
  48. Kozhevnikov, M., Louchakova, O., Josipovic, Z., & Motes, M. A. (2009). The enhancement of visuospatial processing efficiency through Buddhist deity meditation. Psychological Science, 20(5), 645–653.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Kravitz, D. J., Saleem, K. S., Baker, C. I., & Mishkin, M. (2011). A new neural framework for visuospatial processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 217–230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Kungurtsev, I., & Louchakova, O. (1994). The unknown Russian mysticism: Pagan sorcery, Christian Yoga, and other esoteric practices in the former Soviet Union. Gnosis, 32, 20–27.Google Scholar
  51. Lazar, S. W., Kerr, C. E., Wasserman, R. H., Gray, J. R., Greve, D. N., Treadway, M. T.,...Fischl, B. (2005). Meditation experience is associated with increased cortical thickness. Neuroreport, 16(17), 1893–1897.Google Scholar
  52. Leisman, G., & Koch, P. (2009). Networks of conscious experience: Computational neuroscience in understanding life, death, and consciousness. Reviews in the Neurosciences, 20, 151–176.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Lindberg, D. A. (2005). Integrative review of research related to meditation, spirituality, and the elderly. Geriatric Nursing, 26(6), 372–377.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Lo, P. C., Huang, M. L., & Chang, K. M. (2003). EEG alpha blocking correlated with perception of inner light during Zen meditation. The American Journal of Chinese Medicine, 31(4), 629–642.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Longchenpa. (1975). Kindly bent to ease us. Part one: Mind (H. V. Guenther, Trans.) Berkeley, CA: Dharma Publishing.Google Scholar
  56. Lou, H. C., Kjaer, T. W., Friberg, L., Wildschiodtz, G., Holm, S., & Nowak, M. (1999). A 15O-­H2O PET study of meditation and the resting state of normal consciousness. Human Brain Mapping, 7, 98–105.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Louchakova, O., & Warner, A. (2003). Via Kundalini: Psychosomatic excursions in transpersonal psychology. The Humanistic Psychologist, 31(2–3), 115–158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Louchakova, O. (2005). Ontopoiesis and union in the prayer of the heart: Contributions to psychotherapy and learning. In A. T. Tymieniecka (Ed.), Analecta Husserliana: The yearbook of phenomenological research (Vol. 91). Logos of phenomenology and phenomenology of the logos. Book four: The logos of scientific interrogation. Participating in nature –life- sharing in life (pp. 289–311). Dordrecht, UK: Kluwer.Google Scholar
  59. Louchakova, O. (2007a). Ontopoiesis and spiritual emergence: Bridging Tymieniecka’s phenomenology of life and transpersonal psychology. In A.-T. Tymieniecka (Ed.), Analecta Husserliana: Phenomenology of life- from the animal soul to the human mind (Vol. 94) (pp. 43–68). Dordrecht, UK: Kluwer.Google Scholar
  60. Louchakova, O. (2007b). Prayer of the Heart, ego-transcendence and adult development. Existential Psychoanalysis, 18(2), 261–287.Google Scholar
  61. Louchakova, O. (2007c). Spiritual heart and direct knowing in the Prayer of the Heart. Existential Psychoanalysis, 18(1), 81–102.Google Scholar
  62. Louchakova, O. (2008). Reconstitution of the self during the study of Advaita Vedanta (Traditional Indian teachings of non-dual consciousness): Phenomenological investigations. In Toward a science of consciousness, April 8–12, 2008, Tucson AZ, Consciousness Research Abstracts (315), p. 194.Google Scholar
  63. Louchakova-Schwartz, O. (2011). Self and world: Vedanta, Sufism and Presocratics in phenomenological view. In A.-T. Tymieniecka (Ed.), Phenomenology/ontopoiesis retrieving cosmic horizons of antiquity (pp. 423–438). Dordrecht, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Louchakova-Schwartz, O. (2012). Intuition of life in Tymieniecka’s Phenomenology with a reference to intuition of Sat in Śaṅkara’s Advaita Vedanta. Culture & Philosophy: A Journal for Phenomenological Inquiry, 2011, 40–60.Google Scholar
  65. Luck, S. J., & Vogel, E. K. (1997). The capacity of visual working memory for features and ­conjunctions. Nature, 390, 279–281.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Luft, Sebastian. 2011. Subjectivity and lifeworld in transcendental phenomenology. Evanston, Ill: Northwestern University Press.Google Scholar
  67. Lundervold, A. (2010). On consciousness, resting state fMRI, and neurodynamics. Nonlinear Biomedical Physics, 4 (Suppl. 1), S1–S9.Google Scholar
  68. Lutz, A., Brefczynski-Lewis, J., Johnstone, T., & Davidson, R. J. (2008). Regulation of the neural circuitry of emotion by compassion meditation: Effects of meditative expertise. Public Library of Science One, 3(3), e1897.Google Scholar
  69. Lutz, A., Dunne, J. D., & Davidson, R. J. (2007). Meditation and the neuroscience of consciousness: An introduction. In P. Zelazo, M. Moscovitch, & E. Thompson (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of consciousness (pp. 499–554). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Lutz, A., Greischar, L. L., Rawlings, N. B., Richard, M., Davidson, R. J. (2004). Long-term meditators self-induce high-amplitude gamma synchrony during mental practice. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 101(46), 16369–16373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Lutz, A., Lachaux, J. P., Martinerie, J., & Varela, F. J. (2002). Guiding the study of brain dynamics by using first-person data: Synchrony patterns correlate with ongoing conscious states during a simple visual task. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 99(3), 1586–1891.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Lutz, A., Slagter, H. A., Dunne, J. D., & Davidson, R. J. (2008). Attention regulation and monitoring in meditation. Trends in Cognitive Science, 12(4), 163–169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Lutz, A., & Thompson, E. (2003). Neurophenomenology: Integrating subjective experience and brain dynamics in the neuroscience of consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 10 (9–10), 31–52.Google Scholar
  74. Marbach, E. (2006). No heterophenomenology without autophenomenology: Variations on a theme of mine. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 6(1–2), 75–87.Google Scholar
  75. Marti S., Sackur, J., Sigman, M., & Dehaene, S. (2010). Mapping introspection’s blind spot: reconstruction of dual-task phenomenology using quantified introspection. Cognition, 115(2), 303–313.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Mayo, K. R. (2009). Support from neurobiology for spiritual techniques for anxiety: A brief review. Journal of Health Care Chaplaincy, 16(1–2), 53–7.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  77. Mennes, M., Zuo, X. N., Kelly, C., Di Martino, A., Zang, Y. F., Biswal, B.,...Milham, M. P. (2011). Linking inter-individual differences in neural activation and behavior to intrinsic brain dynamics. Neuroimage, 54(4), 2950–2959.Google Scholar
  78. Merleau-Ponty, M. (1962). Phenomenology of perception (C. Smith, Trans.). London, UK: Routledge and Kegan Paul. (Original work published 1945)Google Scholar
  79. Metzler, J., & Shepard, R. N. (1974). Transformational studies of the internal representation of three-dimensional objects. In R. Solso (Ed.), Theories in cognitive psychology: The Loyola Symposium (pp. 147–201). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  80. Mitchel, D. J., & Cusack, R. (2011). The temporal evolution of electromagnetic markers sensitive to the capacity limits of visual short-term memory. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 5, 18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Morey, C. C., Cowan, N., Morey, R. D., & Rouder, J. N. (2011). Flexible attention allocation to visual and auditory working memory tasks: Manipulating reward induces a trade-off. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 3(2), 458–472.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. Murphy, N. (1999). Introduction. In R. Russel, N. Murphy, T. Meyering, & M. Arbib (Eds.), Neuroscience and the person. Scientific perspectives on divine action (pp. i-xxxv). Berkeley, CA: Center for Theology and Natural Science.Google Scholar
  83. Nagel, T. (1971). Brain bisection and the unity of consciousness. Synthese, 22, 396–413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. Neumann, N.-U., & Frasch, K. (2006). The neurobiological dimension of meditation–Results from neuroimaging studies. Psychotherapie Psychosomatik Medizinische Psychologie, 56, 488–492.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. Neumann, N.-U., & Karel, F. (2006). The neurobiological dimension of meditation - results from neuroimaging studies. Psychotherapie Psychosomatik Medizinische Psychologie, 5(12), 488–492.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  86. Newberg, A. B. (2006). Religious and spiritual practices: A neurochemical perspective. In P. NcNamara (Ed.), Where God and science meet: How brain and evolutionary studies alter our understanding of religion (pp. 15–32). Westport, CT: Praeger.Google Scholar
  87. Newberg, A. B. (2010). Neurobiology of meditation. In D. Monty & B. Beitman (Eds.), Integrative psychiatry (pp. 339–358). Oxford, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  88. Newberg, A., Alavi, A., Baime, M., Pourdehnad, M., Santanna, J., & d’Aquili, E. (2001). The measurement of regional cerebral blood flow during the complex cognitive task of meditation: a preliminary SPECT study. Psychiatry Research, 106, 113–122.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  89. Newberg, A., Pourdehnad, M., Alavi, A., & d’Aquili, E. G. (2003). Cerebral blood flow during meditative prayer: Preliminary findings and methodological issues. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 97, 625–630.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  90. Overgaard, M. (2004). On the naturalising of phenomenology. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 3, 365–379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  91. Pagnoni, G., Cekic, M., & Guo, Y. (2008). Thinking about not-thinking: Neural correlates of conceptual processing during Zen meditation. Public Library of Science One, 3(9), e3083.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  92. Pekala, R., Wenger, C., & Levine, R. (1985). Individual differences in phenomenological experience: States of consciousness as a function of absorption. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48(1), 125–32.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  93. Petitot, J., Varela, F. J., Pachoud, B., & Roy J-M. (Eds.). (1999). Naturalizing phenomenology: Issues in contemporary phenomenology and cognitive science. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
  94. Pfeiffer, W. M. (1966). Concentrative self-relaxation by exercises derived from Buddhistic respiration-­meditation and from respiration therapy. Zeitschrift für Psychotherapie und Medizinische Psychologie, 16(5), 172–81.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  95. Posner, M. I., & Dehaene, S. (1994). Attentional networks. Trends in Neuroscience, 17, 75–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  96. Posner, M. I., & Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attentional system of the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 25–42.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  97. Raffone, A., & Pantani, M. (2010). A global workspace model for phenomenal and access ­consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition, 19(2), 580–596.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  98. Raffone, A., & Srinivasan, N. (2009). An adaptive workspace hypothesis about the neural correlates of consciousness: Insights from neuroscience and meditation studies. Progressive Brain Research, 176, 161–180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  99. Raffone, A., & Srinivasan, N. (2010). The exploration of meditation in the neuroscience of attention and consciousness. Cognitive Processing, 11(1), 1–7.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  100. Rosenzweig, M. R. (2003). Effects of differential experience on the brain and behavior. Developmental Neuropsychology, 24(2&3), 523–540.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  101. Rubik, B. (2011). Neurofeedback-enhanced gamma brainwaves from the prefrontal cortical region of meditators and non-meditators and associated subjective experiences. Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, 17(2), 109–115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  102. Schore, A. (2010). The right brain implicit self: A central mechanism of the psychotherapy change process. In J. Petrucelli (Ed.), Knowing, not-knowing and sort of knowing: Psychoanalysis and the experience of uncertainty (pp. 177–202). London, UK: Karnac.Google Scholar
  103. Schwartz, G. E., Davidson, R. J., & Goleman, D. J. (1978). Patterning of cognitive and somatic processes in the self-regulation of anxiety: Effects of meditation versus exercise. Psychosomatic Medicine, 40(4), 321–8.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  104. Sells, M. A. (1994). Mystical languages of the unsaying. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  105. Slagter, H. A., Davidson, R. J., & Lutz, A. (2011). Mental training as a tool in the neuroscientific study of brain and cognitive plasticity. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10, 5–17.Google Scholar
  106. Stcherbatskoi (Stcherbatsky, Sherbastky), T. (1903). Teoriya poznaniya i logika po ucheniyu ­pozdneishikh buddistov [Theory of knowledge and Logic According to Later Buddhists]. St. Petersburg, Russia: Izd. Fak. Vost Yazykov Imp. Sankt-Peterburgskogo Universiteta. No. 14.Google Scholar
  107. Taylor, J. G., & Fragopanagos, N. (2007). Resolving some confusions over attention and ­consciousness. Neural Networks, 20(9), 993–1003.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  108. Tei, S., Faber, T. L., Lehmann, D., Tsujiuchi, T., Kumano, H., Pascual-Marqui, R. D.,...Kochi, K. (2009). Meditators and non-meditators: EEG source imaging during resting. Brain Topography, 22(3), 158–165.Google Scholar
  109. Thomas, N. J. T. (2010). Mental imagery. In E. N. Zalta (Ed.), The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy. Retrieved from http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/fall2010/entries/mental-imagery/
  110. Toadvine, T. (2002). Leaving Husserl’s cave? The philosopher’s shadow revisited. In T. Toadvine, & L. Embree (Eds.), Merleau-Ponty’s readings of Husserl (pp. 71–94). Netherlands: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  111. Travis, F., Arenander, A., & DuBois, D. (2004). Psychological and physiological characteristics of a proposed object-referral/self-referral continuum of self-awareness. Consciousness and Cognition, 13, 401–420.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  112. Travis, T., & Shear, J. (2010). Focused attention, open monitoring and automatic self-transcending: Categories to organize meditations from Vedic, Buddhist and Chinese traditions. Consciousness and Cognition, 19(4), 1110–1118.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  113. Travis, F., & Wallace, R. K. (1999). Autonomic and EEG patterns during eyes-closed rest and transcendental meditation (TM) practice: The basis for a neural model of TM practice. Conscious and Cognition, 8(3), 302–318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  114. Van den Heuvel, M. P., & Hulshoff Pol, H. E. (2010). Exploring the brain network: A review on resting-state fMRI functional connectivity. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 20(8), 519–534.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  115. Varela, F. J., Thompson, E., & Rosch, E. (1991). The embodied mind: Cognitive science and human experience. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
  116. Vogel, A. C., Power, J. D., Petersen, S. E., & Schlaggar, B. L. (2010). Development of the brain’s functional network architecture. Neuropsychology Review, 20(4), 362–375.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  117. Wachholtz, A. B., & Pargament, K. I. (2005). Is spirituality a critical ingredient of meditation? Comparing the effects of spiritual meditation, secular meditation, and relaxation on spiritual, psychological, cardiac, and pain outcomes. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 28(4), 369–384.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  118. Wallace, B. A. (1999). The Buddhist tradition of Samatha. Methods of refining and examining consciousness. In F. Varela, & J. Shear (Eds.), The view from within. First-person approaches to the study of consciousness (pp. 175–187). Thorverton, UK: Imprint Academic.Google Scholar
  119. Wang, D. J., Rao, H., Korczykowski, M., Wintering, N., Pluta, J., Khalsa, D. S., & Newberg, A. B. (2011). Cerebral blood flow changes associated with different meditation practices and perceived depth of meditation. Psychiatry Research, 191(1), 60–67.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  120. Wangual, T. (1993). Wonders of the natural mind. The essence of Dozgchen in the native Bon tradition of Tibet. Barryton, NY: Station Hill.Google Scholar
  121. Wenk-Sormaz, H. (2005). Meditation can reduce habitual responding. Alternative Therapies in Health and Medicine, 11(2), 42–58.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  122. Wildman, W. J., & Brothers, L. A. (1999). A neurophysical-semiotic model of religious experience. In R. Russel, N. Murphy, T. Meyering, & M. Arbib (Eds.), Neuroscience and the person. Scientific perspectives on divine action (pp. 347–417). Berkeley, CA: Center for Theology and Natural Science.Google Scholar
  123. Wittig, A. F. (2001). Introduction to psychology. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
  124. Xiong, G. L., & Doraiswamy, P. M. (2009). Does meditation enhance cognition and brain plasticity? Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1172, 63–69.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  125. Zahavi, D. (2002). Anonymity and first-personal givenness: An attempt at reconciliation. In D. Carr & C. Lotz (Eds.), Subjektivität – Verantwortung – Wahrheit (pp. 75–89). Frankfurt am Main, Germany: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
  126. Zahavi, D. (2005). Subjectivity and selfhood. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
  127. Zahavi, D. (2010). Naturalized phenomenology. In S. Gallagher & D. Schmicking (Eds.), Handbook of phenomenology and cognitive science (pp. 3–19). Dordrecht, UK, New York, NY: Springer.Google Scholar
  128. Zanto, T. P., Rubens, M. T., Thangavel, A., & Gazzaley, A. (2011). Causal role of the prefrontal cortex in top-down modulation of visual processing and working memory. Nature Neuroscience, 14, 656661.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  129. Zeman, A. (2001). Consciousness. Brain, 124(7), 1263–89.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Sofia University (Formerly Institute of Transpersonal Psychology)Palo AltoUSA

Personalised recommendations