Crossmodal Mental Imagery

Abstract

While researchers have long pondered over the nature (and even the very existence) of visual mental imagery, it is only in the last few decades or so that serious scientific research has been devoted to the study of this phenomenon in modalities other than vision. That said, the available empirical evidence now supports the view that mental imagery can occur in any sensory modality, though with widely varying degrees of vividness. It is at this point, then, that the question arises as to whether there might also be such a thing as crossmodal imagery. Crossmodal mental imagery has most commonly been reported under those conditions in which the presentation of a stimulus in one sensory modality results in the formation of a mental image in another modality. In this review, evidence supporting the existence of crossmodal mental imagery in neurologically normal adults is critically evaluated. Furthermore, similarities and differences with related phenomena such as crossmodal sensory forms of synaesthesia and crossmodal perceptual completion are also discussed.

Keywords

Crossmodal mental imagery Crossmodal perceptual completion Crossmodal sensory synaesthesia 

Notes

Acknowledgments

Thanks to Juan Botero, Fiona Macpherson, and Barry Smith for comments on an oral presentation of this chapter given at the British Academy Anglo-Colombian workshop held in Bogota, Colombia, on February, 2012. O.D. is funded by an FP7 Marie Curie grant.

References

  1. Abramson M, Goldinger SD (1997) What the reader’s eye tells the mind’s ear: silent reading activates inner speech. Percept Psychophys 59:1059–1068PubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Ahsen A (1997) Visual imagery and performance during multisensory experience, synaesthesia and phosphenes. J Mental Imagery 21(3–4):1–40Google Scholar
  3. Amedi A, Kriegstein K, Atteveldt NM, Beauchamp MS, Naumer MJ (2005) Functional imaging of human crossmodal identification and object recognition. Exp Brain Res 166:559–571PubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Auvray M, Deroy O (in press) Synesthesia. In: Matthen M (ed) Oxford Handbook of Philosophy of Perception. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  5. Auvray M, Spence C (2008) The multisensory perception of flavor. Conscious Cogn 17:1016–1031PubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Baars BJ (1996) When are images conscious? The curious disconnection between imagery and consciousness in the scientific literature. Conscious Cogn 5:261–264PubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Baddeley AD, Andrade J (2000) Working memory and the vividness of imagery. J Exp Psychol Gen 129:129–145Google Scholar
  8. Banissy MJ, Cohen Kadosh R, Maus GW, Walsh V, Ward J (2009) Prevalence, characteristics and a neurocognitive model of mirror-touch synaesthesia. Exp Brain Res 198:261–272PubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Banissy MJ, Ward J (2007) Mirror-touch synesthesia is linked with empathy. Nat Neurosci 10:815–816PubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Bargary G, Mitchell KJ (2008) Synesthesia and cortical connectivity. Trends Neurosci 31:335–342PubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Barnett KJ, Newell FN (2008) Synaesthesia is associated with enhanced, self-rated visual imagery. Conscious Cogn 17:1032–1039PubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Bensafi M, Rouby C (2007) Individual differences in odor imaging ability reflect differences in olfactory and emotional perception. Chem Senses 32:237–244PubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Betts GH (1909) Distribution and Function of Mental Imagery. Teachers College, Columbia University, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  14. Blakemore S-J, Bristow D, Bird G, Frith C, Ward J (2005) Somatosensory activations during the observation of touch and a case of vision-touch synaesthesia. Brain 128:1571–1583PubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Brang D, Ramachandran VS (2008) Psychopharmacology of synesthesia: the role of serotonin S2a receptor activation. Med Hypotheses 70:903–904PubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Brower D (1947) The experimental study of imagery: II. The relative predominance of various imagery modalities. J Gen Psychol 37:199–200PubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Busse L, Roberts KC, Crist RE, Weissman DH, Woldorff MG (2005) The spread of attention across modalities and space in a multisensory object. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 102:18751–18756PubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Calvert GA, Bullmore ET, Brammer MJ, Campbell R, Williams SC, McGuire PK, Woodruff PW, Iversen SD, David AS (1997) Activation of auditory cortex during silent lipreading. Science 276:593–596PubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Campbell R, MacSweeney M, Surguladze S, Calvert GA, McGuire P, Suckling J, Brammer MJ, David AS (2001) Cortical substrates for the perception of face actions: an fMRI study of the specificity of activation for seen speech and for meaningless lower-face acts (gurning). Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 12:233–243PubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Carlyon RP, Deeks J, Norris D, Butterfield S (2002) The continuity illusion and vowel identification. Acustica Acta Acustica 88:408–415Google Scholar
  21. Clark A (in press) Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. Behav Brain SciGoogle Scholar
  22. Chara PJ (1992) Some concluding thoughts on the debate about the Vividness of Visual Imagery Questionnaire. Percept Mot Skills 75:947–954PubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Cohen Kadosh R, Terhune DB (2012) Redefining synaesthesia? Br J Psychol 103(1):20–23PubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Cui X, Jeter CB, Yang D, Montague PR, Eagleman DM (2007) Vividness of mental imagery: individual variability can be measured objectively. Vision Res 47:474–478PubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Darling WG, Seitz RJ, Peltier S, Tellmann L, Butler AJ (2007) Visual cortex activation in kinesthetic guidance of reaching. Exp Brain Res 179:607–619PubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Daselaar SM, Porat Y, Huijbers W, Pennartz CMA (2010) Modality-specific and modality-independent components of the human imagery system. Neuroimage 52:677–685PubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Day S (2005) Some demographic and socio-cultural aspects of synesthesia. In: Robertson LC, Sagiv N (eds) Synesthesia: Perspectives from Cognitive Neuroscience. Oxford University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  28. Deroy O, Spence C (2011) Synaesthesia reclassified: borderline cases of crossmodally-induced experiences. Psychol BullGoogle Scholar
  29. De Volder AG, Toyamaa H, Kimura Y, Kiyosawa M, Nakanoe M, Vanlier A, Wanet-Defalquef M-C, Mishina M, Odaa K, Ishiwataa K, Senda M (2001) Auditory triggered mental imagery of shape involves visual association areas in early blind humans. Neuroimage 14:129–139PubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Djordjevic J, Zatorre RJ, Petrides M, Jones-Gotman M (2004) The mind’s nose: effects of odor and visual imagery on odor detection. Psychol Sci 15:143–148PubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Eardley AF, Pring L (2011) Exploring the impact of sucking sweets on flavour imagery. J Cogn Psychol 23(7):811–817. doi:10.1080/20445911.2011.572872Google Scholar
  32. Ellis H (1902) Mescal: a study of a divine plant. Popular Sci Monthly 61:52–71Google Scholar
  33. Fallgatter AJ, Mueller TJ, Strik WK (1997) Neurophysiological correlates of mental imagery in different sensory modalities. Int J Psychophysiol 25:145–153PubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Felleman DJ, Van Essen DC (1991) Distributed hierarchical processing in primate cerebral cortex. Cereb Cortex 1:1–47PubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Fiebelkorn IC, Foxe JJ, Molholm S (2010) Dual mechanisms for the cross-sensory spread of attention: how much do learned associations matter? Cereb Cortex 20:109–120PubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Finke RA, Kurtzman HS (1981) Mapping the visual field in mental imagery. J Exp Psychol Gen 110:501–517PubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Friston K (2005) A theory of cortical responses. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 29:815–836Google Scholar
  38. Friston K, Stephan K (2007) Free energy and the brain. Synthese 159:417–458PubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Gallace A, Ngo MK, Sulaitis J, Spence C (2012) Multisensory presence in virtual reality: possibilities & limitations. In: Ghinea G, Andres F, Gulliver S (eds) Multiple Sensorial Media Advances and Applications: New Developments in MulSeMedia. Information Science Reference, Hershey, PAGoogle Scholar
  40. Gallace A, Spence C (2011) To what extent do Gestalt grouping principles influence tactile perception? Psychol Bull 137:538–561PubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Galton F (1880a) Statistics of mental imagery. Mind 5:301–318Google Scholar
  42. Galton F (1880b) Visualised numerals. Nature 21:494–495Google Scholar
  43. Galton F (1880c) Mental imagery. Fortnightly Rev 28:312–324Google Scholar
  44. Galton F (1881) Visualized numerals. J Anthropol Inst 10:85–102Google Scholar
  45. Gates C, Hupé J-M (2011) Synesthesia, mirror touch, and ticker-tape associations: an examination of individual differences. Perception 40(Suppl):185–186Google Scholar
  46. Ghazanfar AA, Schroeder CE (2006) Is neocortex essentially multisensory? Trends Cogn Sci 10:278–285PubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Gilbert AN, Crouch M, Kemp SE (1998) Olfactory and visual mental imagery. J Mental Imagery 22:137–146Google Scholar
  48. Grossenbacher PG, Lovelace CT (2001) Mechanisms of synesthesia: cognitive and physiological constraints. Trends Cogn Sci 5:36–41PubMedGoogle Scholar
  49. Hagen MC, Franzén O, McGlone F, Essick G, Dancer C, Pardo JV (2002) Tactile motion activates the human MT/V5 complex. Eur J Neurosci 16:957–964PubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Halpern AR (1992) Musical aspects of auditory imagery. In: Reisberg D (ed) Auditory Imagery. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJGoogle Scholar
  51. Held R, Ostrovsky Y, de Gelder B, Gandhi T, Ganesh S, Mathur U, Sinha P (2011) The newly sighted fail to match seen with felt. Nat Neurosci 14:551–553PubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. Hertrich I, Dietrich S, Ackermann H (2011) Cross-modal interactions during perception of audiovisual speech and nonspeech signals: an fMRI study. J Cogn Neurosci 23:221–237PubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Hong SW, Blake R (2008) Early visual mechanisms do not contribute to synesthetic color experience. Vision Res 48:1018–1026PubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. Hubbard TL (2010) Auditory imagery: empirical findings. Psychol Bull 136:302–329PubMedGoogle Scholar
  55. Intons-Peterson MJ (1983) Imagery paradigms: how vulnerable are they to experimenters’ expectations? J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 9:394–412PubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Intons-Peterson MJ (1992) Components of auditory imagery. In: Reisberg D (ed) Auditory Imagery. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJGoogle Scholar
  57. Ishai A, Sagi D (1995) Common mechanisms of visual imagery and perception. Science 268:1772–1774PubMedGoogle Scholar
  58. James W (1890) Principles of Psychology (vol 2). Henry Holt, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  59. Kanizsa G, Gerbino W (1982) Amodal completion: seeing or thinking? In: Beck J (ed) Organization and Representation in Perception. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJGoogle Scholar
  60. Karwoski TF, Odbert HS, Osgood CE (1942) Studies in synesthetic thinking. II. The rôle of form in visual responses to music. J Gen Psychol 26:199–222Google Scholar
  61. Kilintari M, Raos V, Savaki H (2011) Grasping in the dark activates early visual cortices. Cereb Cortex 21:949–963PubMedGoogle Scholar
  62. Kitagawa N, Igarashi Y (2005) Tickle sensation induced by hearing a sound. Jpn J Psychon Sci 24:121–122Google Scholar
  63. Kitagawa N, Igarashi Y, Kashino M (2009) The tactile continuity illusion. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 35:1784–1790PubMedGoogle Scholar
  64. Klatzky RL, Lederman SJ (1987) The intelligent hand. In: Bower G (ed) The Psychology of Learning and Motivation (vol 21). Academic Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  65. Kosslyn SM (1994) Image and Brain: The Resolution of the Imagery Debate. MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  66. Kosslyn SM, Behrmann M, Jeannerod M (1995) The cognitive neuroscience of mental imagery. Neuropsychologia 33:1335–1344PubMedGoogle Scholar
  67. Kosslyn SM, Ganis G, Thompson WL (2003) Mental imagery: against the nihilistic hypothesis. Trends Cogn Sci 7:109–111PubMedGoogle Scholar
  68. Lacey S, Flueckiger P, Stilla R, Lava M, Sathian K (2010) Object familiarity modulates the relationship between visual object imagery and haptic shape perception. Neuroimage 49:1977–1990PubMedGoogle Scholar
  69. Lacey S, Sathian K (2012) Representation of object form in vision and touch. In: Murray MM, Wallace M (eds) The Neural Bases of Multisensory Processes. CRC, Boca Raton, FLGoogle Scholar
  70. Lawless H, Engen T (1977) Associations to odors: Interference, mnemonics, and verbal labelling. J Exp Psychol Hum Learn 3:52–59PubMedGoogle Scholar
  71. Lederman SJ, Klatzky RL, Chataway C, Summers CD (1990) Visual mediation and the haptic recognition of two-dimensional pictures of common objects. Percept Psychophys 47:54–64PubMedGoogle Scholar
  72. Leuba C (1940) Images as conditioned sensations. J Exp Psychol 26:345–351Google Scholar
  73. Liberman AM, Mattingly IG (1985) The motor theory of speech perception revised. Cognition 21:1–36PubMedGoogle Scholar
  74. Lindauer MS (1969) Imagery and sensory modality. Percept Mot Skills 29:203–215PubMedGoogle Scholar
  75. MacDougal R (1898) Music imagery: a confession of experience. Psychol Rev 5(5):463–476Google Scholar
  76. Marks DF (1977) Imagery and consciousness: a theoretical review from an individual differences perspective. J Mental Imagery 12:275–290Google Scholar
  77. Marsella AJ, Quijano WY (1974) A comparison of vividness of mental imagery across different sensory modalities in Filipinos and Caucasian Americans. J Cross Cult Psychol 5:451–465Google Scholar
  78. Newton GE (1890) Visualized images produced by music. Nature 41:417Google Scholar
  79. Nikolic D (2009) Is synaesthesia actually ideasthesia? An inquiry into the nature of the phenomenon. Proceedings of the Third International Congress on Synaesthesia, Science & Art, Granada, Spain, 26–29 April 2009Google Scholar
  80. Nikolic D, Jürgens UM, Rothen N, Meier B, Mroczko A (2011) Swimming-style synaesthesia. Cortex 47:874–879PubMedGoogle Scholar
  81. Novich S, Cheng S, Eagleman DM (2011) Is synaesthesia one condition or many? A large-scale analysis reveals subgroups. J Neuropsychol 5:353–371PubMedGoogle Scholar
  82. O’Callaghan C (2008) Seeing what you hear: cross-modal illusions and perception. Philos Issues 18:316–338Google Scholar
  83. Olivetti Belardinelli M, Matteo R, Del Gratta C, De Nicola A, Ferretti A, Tartaro A, Bonomo L, Romani GL (2004) Intermodal sensory image generation: an fMRI analysis. Eur J Cogn Psychol 16:729–752Google Scholar
  84. Olivetti Belardinelli M, Palmiero M, Sestieri C, Nardo D, Di Matteo R, Londei A et al (2009) An fMRI investigation on image generation in different sensory modalities: the influence of vividness. Acta Psychol 132:190–200Google Scholar
  85. Paivio A (1971) Imagery and Verbal Processes. Holt, Rinehart, & Winston, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  86. Paivio A (1986) Mental Representations: A Dual Coding Approach. Oxford University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  87. Pearson J, Clifford CWG, Tong F (2008) The functional impact of mental imagery on conscious perception. Curr Biol 18:982–986PubMedGoogle Scholar
  88. Pekkola J, Ojanen V, Autti T, Jääskeläinen JP, Möttönen R, Tarkiainen A, Sams M (2005) Primary auditory cortex activation by visual speech: an fMRI study at 3 T. Neuroreport 16:125–128PubMedGoogle Scholar
  89. Peirce CS (1935) Collected Papers of Charles Sanders Peirce (vol 6). Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  90. Perky CW (1910) An experimental study of imagination. Am J Psychol 21:422–452Google Scholar
  91. Pessoa L, De Weerd P (2003) Filling-in: From Perceptual Completion to Cortical Reorganization. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  92. Peynaud E (1996) The Taste of Wine: The Art and Science of Wine Appreciation (Trans. Michael Schuster). London: Macdonald OrbisGoogle Scholar
  93. Ptito M, Moesgaard SM, Gjedde A, Kupers R (2005) Cross-modal plasticity revealed by electrotactile stimulation of the tongue in the congenitally blind. Brain 128:606–614PubMedGoogle Scholar
  94. Pylyshyn ZW (1973) What the mind’s eye tells the mind’s brain: a critique of mental imagery. Psychol Bull 80:1–25Google Scholar
  95. Pylyshyn ZW (2002) Mental imagery: in search of a theory. Behav Brain Sci 25:157–182PubMedGoogle Scholar
  96. Pylyshyn ZW (2003) Return of the mental image: are there really pictures in the brain? Trends Cogn Sci 7:113–118PubMedGoogle Scholar
  97. Qureshy A, Kawashima R, Imran MB, Sugiura M, Goto R, Okada K et al (2000) Functional mapping of human brain in olfactory processing: a PET study. J Neurophysiol 84:1656–1666PubMedGoogle Scholar
  98. Rader CM, Tellegen A (1987) An investigation of synesthesia. J Pers Soc Psychol 52:981–987Google Scholar
  99. Redlich E, Bonvicini G (1907) Die mangelnde wahrnehmung (autoanästhesie) der blindheit bei cerebralen erkrankungen [The lack of perception/sense in the blind (auto-anaesthesia) accompanying cerebral disease]. Neurol Centralblatt 26:945–951Google Scholar
  100. Reisberg D, Pearson DG, Kosslyn SM (2003) Intuitions and introspections about imagery: the role of imagery experience in shaping an investigator’s theoretical views. Appl Cogn Psychol 17:147–160Google Scholar
  101. Riggs LA, Karwoski T (1934) Synaesthesia. Br J Psychol 25:29–41Google Scholar
  102. Roland PE, Gulyas B (1994) Visual imagery and visual representation. Trends Neurosci 17:281–286PubMedGoogle Scholar
  103. Royet JP, Koenig O, Gregoire M-C, Cinotti L, Lavenne F, Le Bars D, Costes N, Vigouroux M, Farget V, Sicard G, Holley A, Mauguiere F, Comar D, Froment JC (1999) Functional anatomy of perceptual and semantic processing for odors. J Cogn Neurosci 11:94–109PubMedGoogle Scholar
  104. Ryle G (1949) The Concept of Mind. Hutchinson, LondonGoogle Scholar
  105. Sartre J-P (1936) L’Imagination. Paris: Alcan (Imagination: A Psychological Critique. Translated from the French by F. Williams. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press; 1962) Original edition, 1936Google Scholar
  106. Sartre J-P (1940) L’Imaginaire. Paris: Gallimard (The Psychology of Imagination. Translated from the French by B. Frechtman, New York: Philosophical Library; 1948) Original edition, 1940Google Scholar
  107. Sathian K, Prather SC, Zhang M (2004) Visual cortical involvement in normal tactile perception. In: Calvert GA, Spence C, Stein BE (eds) The Handbook of Multisensory Processes. MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  108. Sathian K, Zangaladze A (2001) Feeling with the mind’s eye: the role of visual imagery in tactile perception. Optom Vis Sci 8:276–281Google Scholar
  109. Sathian K, Zangaladze A, Hoffman JM, Grafton ST (1997) Feeling with the mind’s eye. Neuroreport 8:3877–3881PubMedGoogle Scholar
  110. Sedman G (1966) A comparative study of pseudohallucinations, imagery and true hallucinations. Br J Psychiatry 112:9–17PubMedGoogle Scholar
  111. Segal SJ (1971) Processing of the stimulus in imagery and perception. In: Segal SJ (ed) Imagery: Current Cognitive Approaches. Academic, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  112. Segal SJ, Gordon P-E (1969) The Perky Effect Revisited: Blocking of Visual Signals by Imagery. Percept Mot Skills 28:791–797PubMedGoogle Scholar
  113. Shepard RN (1978) The mental image. Am Psychol 33:125–137Google Scholar
  114. Shepard RN, Metzler J (1971) Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science 171:701–703PubMedGoogle Scholar
  115. Shepard RN, Cooper LA (1982) Mental Images and their Transformations. MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  116. Simmons WK, Martin A, Barsalou LW (2005) Pictures of appetizing foods activate gustatory cortices for taste and reward. Cereb Cortex 15:1602–1608PubMedGoogle Scholar
  117. Simner J (2007) Beyond perception: synaesthesia as a psycholinguistic phenomenon. Trends Cogn Sci 11:23–29PubMedGoogle Scholar
  118. Simpson L, McKellar P (1955) Types of synaesthesia. J Ment Sci 101:141–147PubMedGoogle Scholar
  119. Slezak P (1995) The ‘philosophical’ case against visual imagery. In: Slezak P et al (eds) Perspectives on Cognitive Science: Theories, Experiments and Foundations. Ablex Publishing, Norwood, NJGoogle Scholar
  120. Smith BC (2007) The objectivity of taste and tasting. In: Smith BC (ed) Questions of Taste: The Philosophy of Wine. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  121. Sparks GG, Sparks CW, Gray K (1995) Media impact on fright reactions and belief in UFOs: the potential role of mental imagery. Commun Res 22:3–23Google Scholar
  122. Spence C (2008) Multisensory perception. In: Blumenthal H (ed) The Big Fat Duck Cook Book. Bloomsbury, LondonGoogle Scholar
  123. Spence C, Bayne T (in press) Is consciousness multisensory? In: Matthen M, Stokes D (eds) Perception and its Modalities. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  124. Spence, C, Piqueras-Fiszman B (in press) Dining in the dark: why, exactly, is the experience so popular? PsychologistGoogle Scholar
  125. Stein BE, Burr D, Costantinides C, Laurienti PJ, Meredith AM, Perrault TJ et al (2010) Semantic confusion regarding the development of multisensory integration: a practical solution. Eur J Neurosci 31:1713–1720PubMedGoogle Scholar
  126. Steven MS, Blakemore C (2004) Visual synaesthesia in the blind. Perception 33:855–868PubMedGoogle Scholar
  127. Stevenson RJ (2009a) The Psychology of Flavour. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  128. Stevenson RJ (2009b) Phenomenal and access consciousness in olfaction. Conscious Cogn 18:1004–1017PubMedGoogle Scholar
  129. Stevenson RJ, Boakes RA, Prescott J (1998) Changes in odor sweetness resulting from implicit learning of a simultaneous odor-sweetness association: an example of learned synaesthesia. Learn Motiv 29:113–132Google Scholar
  130. Stevenson RJ, Boakes RA (2004) Sweet and sour smells: Learned synaesthesia between the senses of taste and smell. In: Calvert GA, Spence C, Stein BE (eds) The Handbook of Multisensory Processing. MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  131. Stevenson RJ, Case TI (2005) Olfactory imagery: a review. Psychon Bull Rev 12:244–264PubMedGoogle Scholar
  132. Stevenson RJ, Tomiczek C (2007) Olfactory-induced synesthesias: a review and model. Psychol Bull 133:294–309PubMedGoogle Scholar
  133. Symons D (1993) The stuff that dreams aren’t made of: why wake-state and dream-state sensory experiences differ. Cognition 47:181–217PubMedGoogle Scholar
  134. Ullman S (1945) Romanticism and synaesthesia: a comparative study of sense transfer in Byron and Keats. Publ Modern Lang Assoc America 60:811–827Google Scholar
  135. Ullmann S (1957) The Principles of Semantics. Blackwell, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  136. Veldhuizen MG, Nachtigal D, Teulings L, Gitelman DR, Small DM (2010) The insular taste cortex contributes to odor quality coding. Front Hum Neurosci 4:58PubMedGoogle Scholar
  137. Verhagen JV, Engelen L (2006) The neurocognitive bases of human multimodal food perception: sensory integration. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 30:613–650PubMedGoogle Scholar
  138. von Kriegstein K, Kleinschmidt A, Sterzer P, Giraud A-L (2005) Interaction of face and voice areas during speaker recognition. J Cogn Neurosci 17:367–376Google Scholar
  139. Ward J (2004) Emotionally mediated synesthesia. Cogn Neuropsychol 21:761–772PubMedGoogle Scholar
  140. Ward J, Mattingley JB (2006) Synaesthesia: an overview of contemporary findings and controversies. Cortex 42:129–136PubMedGoogle Scholar
  141. Warren RM (1970) Perceptual restoration of missing speech sounds. Science 167:392–393PubMedGoogle Scholar
  142. Williams JM (1976) Synesthetic adjectives: a possible law of semantic change. Language 52:461–478Google Scholar
  143. Wolpin M, Weinstein C (1983) Visual imagery and olfactory stimulation. J Mental Imagery 7:63–73Google Scholar
  144. Zhang M, Weisser VD, Stilla R, Prather SC, Sathian K (2004) Multisensory cortical processing of object shape and its relation to mental imagery. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 4:251–259PubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Crossmodal Research Laboratory, Department of Experimental PsychologyUniversity of OxfordOxfordUK
  2. 2.Centre for the Study of the Senses, Institute of PhilosophyUniversity of LondonLondonUK

Personalised recommendations