Advertisement

Depression and Helplessness in Primates

  • Susan Mineka

Abstract

“Five-year-old Merlin had been among the first victims of the polio epidemic. Though he had been one of our favorite youngsters, playful and impish, we were almost glad when he died, for by that time he had become a pathetic wreck…emaciated, lethargic, and morose.” Two years earlier Merlin’s mother had died and when first seen several months after her death “he looked thin…and his eyes seemed enormous, as though he had not slept for a long time.” Merlin was adopted by his older sister, and to a lesser extent by his brother, and they in large part took good care of him. Nonetheless, “gradually…Merlin became more emaciated, his eyes sank deeper into their sockets…He became increasingly lethargic and played less and less frequently with other youngsters”…There was also a “marked deterioration of Merlin’s social responses…When he was four years old, Merlin was far more submissive than other youngsters of that age…As Merlin entered his sixth year his behavior was becoming rapidly more abnormal. Sometimes he hung upside down like a bat…remaining suspended motionless for several minutes at a time.

Keywords

Rhesus Monkey Attributional Style Inescapable Shock Uncontrollable Event Young Monkey 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Abramson, L. Y., and Seligman, M. E. P. Modeling psychopathology in the laboratory: History and rationale. In J. Maser and M. E. P. Seligman (Eds.), Psychopathology: Experimental models. San Francisco: Freeman, 1977.Google Scholar
  2. Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M. E. P., and Teasdale, J. D. Learned helplessness in humans: Critique and reformulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1978, 87, 49–74.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Akiskal, H. S., and McKinney, W. T. Overview of recent research in depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 1975, 32, 285–305.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Alloy, L., and Abramson, L. Judgment of contingency in depressed and nondepressed students: Sadder but wiser ? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 1979, 108, 441–485.Google Scholar
  5. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, ( 3rd ed. ). Washington, D. C.: American Psychiatric Association, 1979.Google Scholar
  6. Anderson, D. C., Cole, J., and McVaugh, W. Variations in unsignaled inescapable preshock as determinants of responses to punishment. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology Monograph Supplement, 1968, 65, 1–17.Google Scholar
  7. Anisman, H., de Catanzaro, D., and Remington, G. Escape performance following exposure to inescapable shock: Deficits in motor response maintenance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1978, 4, 197–218.Google Scholar
  8. Arieti, S., and Bemporad, J. Severe and mild depression. New York: Basic Books, 1978.Google Scholar
  9. Beck, A. T. Depression: Clinical, experimental, and theoretical aspects. New York: Hoeber, 1967.Google Scholar
  10. Beck, A., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., and Emery, G. Cognitive therapy of depression: A treatment manual. New York: Guilford, 1979.Google Scholar
  11. Bibring, E. The mechanism of depression. In P. Greenacre (Ed.), Affective disorders. New York: International Universities Press, 1953.Google Scholar
  12. Bowden, D. M., and McKinney, W. T. Behavioral effects of peer separation, isolation, and reunion on adolescent male rhesus monkeys. Developmental Psychobiology, 1972, 5, 353–362.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Bowlby, J. Grief and mourning in infancy and early childhood. Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 1960, 15, 9–52.Google Scholar
  14. Bowlby, J. Attachment and loss: Volume 1. Attachment. New York: Basic Books, 1969.Google Scholar
  15. Bowlby, J. Separation: Anxiety and anger. New York: Basic Books, 1973.Google Scholar
  16. Bowlby, J. Loss: Sadness and depression. New York: Basic Books, 1980.Google Scholar
  17. Bronowski, J., and Bellugi, U. Language, name and concept. Science, 1970, 168, 669–673.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Brown, G. W., and Harris, T. Social origins of depression: A study of psychiatric disorder in women. New York: The Free Press, 1978.Google Scholar
  19. Cantor, N., Smith, E., French, R., and Mezzich, J. Psychiatric diagnosis as prototype categorization. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1980, 89, 181–193.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Chappell, P. F., and Meier, G. W. Modification of the response to separation in the infant rhesus macaque through manipulation of the environment. Biological Psychiatry, 1975, 10, 643–657.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Coe, C., and Levine, S. Normal responses to mother-infant separation in non-human primates. In D. Klein and J. Rabkin (Eds.), Anxiety: New research and changing concepts. New York: Raven, 1981.Google Scholar
  22. Coe, C., Mendoza, S., Smotherman, W., and Levine, S. Mother-infant attachment in the squirrel monkey: Adrenal responses to separation. Behavioral Biology, 1978, 22, 256–263.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Darwin, C. R. The expression of emotions in man and animals. London: John Murray, 1872.Google Scholar
  24. Dolhinow, P. An experimental study of mother loss in the Indian langur monkey (Presbytis entellus). Folia primatologica, 1980, 33, 77–128.Google Scholar
  25. Engel, G. L. Anxiety and depression-withdrawal: The primary affects of unpleasure. International Journal of Psycho-Analysis, 1962, 43, 49–108.Google Scholar
  26. Erwin, J., Brandt, E. M., and Mitchell, G. D. Attachment formation and separation in heterosexually naive preadolescent rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Developmental Psychobiology, 1973, 6, 531–538.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Ferster, C. B. Behavioral approaches to depression. In R. J. Friedman and M. M. Katz (Eds.), The psychology of depression: Contemporary theory and research. Washington, D.C.: Winston-Wiley, 1974.Google Scholar
  28. Fogle, D. Learned helplessness and learned restlessness. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, and Practice, 1978, 15, 39–47.Google Scholar
  29. Gardner, R. A., and Gardner, B. T. Early signs of language in child and chimpanzee. Science, 1969, 165, 664–672.Google Scholar
  30. Gittelman-Klein, R. Definitional and methodological issues concerning depressive illness in children. In J. G. Schulterbrandt and A. Raskin (Eds.), Depression in childhood: Diagnosis, treatment, and conceptual models. New York: Raven Press, 1977.Google Scholar
  31. Glazer, H., and Weiss, J. Long-term interference effect: An alternative to “Learned helplessness.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1976, 2, 202–213.Google Scholar
  32. Gunnar, M. Changing a frightening toy into a pleasant toy by allowing the infant to control its actions. Developmental Psychology, 1978, 14, 157–162.Google Scholar
  33. Gunnar, M. Contingent stimulation: A review of its role in early development. In S. Levine and H. Ursin (Eds.), Coping and health. New York: Plenum, 1980. (a)Google Scholar
  34. Gunnar, M. Control, warning signals and distress in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 1980, 16, 281–289. (b)Google Scholar
  35. Gunnar, M., Gonzalez, C., Goodlin, B., and Levine, S. Behavioral and pituitary-adrenal responses during a prolonged separation period in infant rhesus macaques. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 1981, 6, 65–75.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Hanson, J. P., Larson, M. E., and Snowdon, C. T. The effects of control over high intensity noise on plasma Cortisol levels in rhesus monkeys. Behavioral Biology, 1976, 16, 333–340.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Harlow, H. F. The formation of learning sets. Psychological Review, 1949, 56, 51–65.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Harlow, H. F., and Zimmerman, R. R. Affectional responses in the infant monkey. Science, 1959, 130, 421–432.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Harlow, M. K. Nuclear family apparatus. Behavior Research Methods and Instrumentation, 1971, 3, 301–304.Google Scholar
  40. Hennessey, M., Kaplan, J., Mendoza, S., Lowe, E., and Levine, S. Separation distress and attachment in surrogate-reared squirrel monkeys. Physiology and Behavior, 1979, 23, 1017–1023.Google Scholar
  41. Hess, W. R. The functional organization of the diencephalon. New York: Grune and Stratton, 1954.Google Scholar
  42. Hinde, R. A., and Davies, L. Removing infant rhesus from mother for 13 days compared with removing mother from infant. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 1972, 13, 227–237.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Hinde, R. A., Leighton-Shapiro, M., and McGinnis, L. Effects of various types of separation experience on rhesus monkeys 5 months later. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 1978, 19, 199–211.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Hinde, R. A., and McGinnis, L. Some factors influencing the effects of temporary mother-infant separation: Some experiments with rhesus monkeys. Psychological Medicine, 1977, 7, 197–212.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Hinde, R. A., and Spencer-Booth, Y. Effects of brief separations from mothers on rhesus monkeys. Science, 1971, 173, 111–118.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Hollis, K., and Overmier, J. B. Effect of inescapable shock on the efficacy of punishment of appetitive instrumental responding by dogs. Psychological Reports, 1973, 33, 903–906.Google Scholar
  47. Hugdahl, K. The three-systems-model of fear and emotion-a critical examination. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1981, 19, 75–85.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Irwin, J., Suissa, A., Annisman, H. Differential effects of inescapable shock on escape performance and discrimination learning in a water escape task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1980, 6, 21–40.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  49. Jackson, J., Alexander, J., Maier, S. Learned helplessness, inactivity, and associative deficits: Effects of inescapable shock on response choice escape learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1980, 6, 1–20.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Jackson, J., Maier, S., and Rappaport, P. Exposure to inescapable shock produces both activity and associative deficits in the rat. Learning and Motivation, 1978, 9, 69–98.Google Scholar
  51. Jones, B. C., and Clark, D. L. Mother-infant separation in squirrel monkeys living in a group. Developmental Psychobiology, 1973, 6, 259–269.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. Kaplan, J. The effects of separation and reunion on the behavior of mother and infant squirrel monkeys. Developmental Psychobiology, 1970, 3, 43–52.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Kaufman, I. C. Mother-infant separation in monkeys: An experimental model. In J. P. Scott and E. Senay (Eds.), Separation and depression: Clinical and research aspects. Washington, D. C.: American Association for the Advancement of Science, 1973. ( Rep. No. 94 )Google Scholar
  54. Kaufman, I. C. On animal models. In R. J. Friedman and M. M. Katz (Eds.), The psychology of depression: Contemporary theory and research. Washington, D. C.: Winston, 1974.Google Scholar
  55. Kaufman, I. C. Developmental considerations of anxiety and depression: Psychobiological studies in monkeys. In T. Shapiro (Ed.), Psycho-analysis and contemporary science. New York: International Universities Press, 1977.Google Scholar
  56. Kaufman, I. C., and Rosenblum, L. A. The reaction to separation in infant monkeys: Anaclitic depression and conservation-withdrawal. Psychosomatic Medicine, 1967, 29, 648–675.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  57. Kaufman, I. C., and Rosenblum, L. A. The waning of the mother-infant bond in two species of macaque. In B. M. Foss (Ed.), Determinants of infant behavior, (Vol. 4 ). London: Methuen, 1969.Google Scholar
  58. Kraemer, G., and McKinney, W. Interaction of pharmacological agents which alter biogenic amine metabolism and depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 1979, 1, 33–54.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  59. Kubler-Ross, E. On death and dying. New York: Macmillan, 1969.Google Scholar
  60. Lang, P. Fear reduction and fear behavior: Problems in treating a construct. Research in Psychotherapy, 1968, 3, 90–102.Google Scholar
  61. Lang, P. J. The application of psychophysiological methods to the study of psychotherapy and behavior change. In A. Bergin and S. Garfield (Eds.), Handbook of psychotherapy and behavior change: An empirical analysis. New York: Wiley, 1971.Google Scholar
  62. Langer, E., and Rodin, J. The effects of choice and enhanced personal responsibility for the aged: A field experiment in an institutional setting. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1976, 34, 191–198.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  63. Lefkowitz, M. M., and Burton, N. Childhood depression: A critique of the concept. Psychological Bulletin, 1978, 85, 716–726.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  64. Leon, G. R., Kendall, P. C., and Garber, J. Depression in children: Parent, teacher, and child perspectives. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 1980, 8, 221–235.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  65. Levine, S., Coe, C., Smotherman, W., and Kaplan, J. Prolonged Cortisol elevation in the infant squirrel monkey after reunion with mother. Physiology and Behavior, 1978, 20, 7–10.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  66. Levis, D. Learned helplessness: A reply and an alternative S-R interpretation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 1976, 105, 47–65.Google Scholar
  67. Lewinsohn, P. A behavioral approach to depression. In R. Friedman and M. Katz (Eds.), The psychology of depression: Contemporary theory and research. Washington, D.C.: Winston, 1974.Google Scholar
  68. Lewis, J. K., Young, L. T., McKinney, W. T., and Kraemer, G. W. Mother- infant separation in rhesus monkeys as a model of human depression: A reconsideration. Archives of General Psychiatry, 1976, 33, 699–705.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  69. Maier, S. F., Drugan, R., Grau, J. W., Hyson, R., MacLennan, A. J., Moye, T., Madden, J., and Barchas, J. D. Learned helplessness, pain inhibition, and the endogenous opiates. In M. D. Zeiler and P. Harzem (Eds.), Advances in analysis of behavior, Vol. 3. New York: Wiley, in press.Google Scholar
  70. Maier, S. F., and Jackson, R. L. Learned helplessness: All of us were right (and wrong): Inescapable shock has multiple effects. In G. Bower (Ed.), Advances in learning and motivation, Vol. 13. New York: Academic, 1979.Google Scholar
  71. Maier, S., and Seligman, M. E. P. Learned helplessness: Theory and evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 1976, 105, 3–46.Google Scholar
  72. Maier, S. F., Seligman, M. E. P., and Solomon, R. L. Pavlovian fear conditioning and learned helplessness. In B. A. Campbell and R. M. Church (Eds.), Punishment and aversive behavior. New York: Apple- ton-Century-Crofts, 1969.Google Scholar
  73. McConville, B. J., Boag, L. C., and Purohitt, A. P. Three types of childhood depression. Canadian Psychiatric Association Journal, 1973, 18, 133–138.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  74. McKinney, W. T. Animal models in psychiatry. Perspectives in Biology and Medicine, 1974, 17, 529–541.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  75. McKinney, W. T., Suomi, S. J., and Harlow, H. F. Repetitive peer separation of juvenile-age rhesus monkeys. Archives of General Psychiatry, 1972, 27, 200–203.Google Scholar
  76. Mendels, J., and Frazer, A. Brain biogenic amine depletion and mood. Archives of General Psychiatry, 1974, 30, 447–451.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  77. Mendoza, S., Smotherman, W., Miner, M., Kaplan, J., and Levine, S. Pituitary-adrenal response to separation in mother and infant monkeys. Developmental Psychobiology, 1978, 11, 169–175.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  78. Miller, W., and Seligman, M. E. P. Depression and the perception of reinforcement. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1973, 82, 62–73.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  79. Mineka, S. The role of fear in theories of avoidance learning, flooding and extinction. Psychological Bulletin, 1979, 86, 985–1010.Google Scholar
  80. Mineka, S., and Kihlstrom, J. Unpredictable and uncontrollable events: A new perspective on experimental neurosis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1978, 87, 256–271.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  81. Mineka, S., and Suomi, S. J. Social separation in monkeys. Psychological Bulletin, 1978, 85, 1376–1400.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  82. Mineka, S., Suomi, S. J., and DeLizio, R. Multiple peer separation in adolescent monkeys: An opponent process interpretation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 1981, 110, 56–85.Google Scholar
  83. Overmier, J. B., Patterson, J., and Wielciwicz, R. Environmental contingencies as sources of stress in animals. In S. Levine and H. Ursin (Eds.), Coping and health. New York: Plenum, 1980.Google Scholar
  84. Overmier, J. B., and Seligman, M. E. P. Effects of inescapable shock upon subsequent escape and avoidance learning. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1967, 63, 23–33.Google Scholar
  85. Pavlov, I. P. Conditioned reflexes: An investigation of the physiological activity of the cerebral cortex. London: Oxford University Press, 1927.Google Scholar
  86. Plimpton, E. H. Environmental variables and the response to maternal loss. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, School of Graduate Studies, Downstate Medical Center, Brooklyn, New York, March 1981.Google Scholar
  87. Plimpton, E., and Rosenblum, L. Detachment-avoidance responses to mother following a separation: A comparative perspective. In H. Harlow, L. Rosenblum, and S. Suomi (Eds,), Advances in the study of primate social development. New York: Van Nostram, 1981.Google Scholar
  88. Premack, D. Language in chimpanzees? Science, 1971, 172, 808–822.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  89. Preston, D. G., Baker, R. P., and Seay, B. M. Mother-infant separation in the patas monkey. Developmental Psychology, 1970, 3, 298–306.Google Scholar
  90. Rachman, S. The meanings of fear. Harmondsworth: Penguin, 1974.Google Scholar
  91. Rachman, S. The passing of the two-stage theory of fear and avoidance: Fresh possibilities. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1976, 14, 125–131.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  92. Rachman, S. Fear and courage. San Francisco: Freeman, 1978.Google Scholar
  93. Reite, M., Short, R., Seiler, C., and Pauley, J. P. Attachment, loss, and depression. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 1981, 22, 141–169.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  94. Robertson, J., and Bowlby, J. Responses of young children to separation from their mothers, Le Courier: Centre International de l’Enfance, 1952, 2, 131–142.Google Scholar
  95. Rodin, J., and Langer, E. Long-term effects of a control-relevant intervention with the institutionalized aged. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1977, 35, 897–902.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  96. Rosenblum, L. Infants’ response to separation: A transactional perspective. In P. G. Turillazzi, L. Rosenblum, and S. Suomi (Eds.), Proceedings of the international meeting on normal and abnormal social development in primates, 1982, in press.Google Scholar
  97. Rosenblum, L. A., and Kaufman, I. C. Variations in infant development and response to maternal loss in monkeys. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 1968, 38, 418–426.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  98. Rosenblum, L., and Plimpton, E. Adaptation to separation: The infant’s efforts to cope with an altered environment. In M. Lewis and L. Rosenblum (Eds.), The uncommon child: Genesis of behavior. New York: Plenum, 1980.Google Scholar
  99. Rumbaugh, D. M. (Ed.) Language learning by a chimpanzee: The Lana project. New York: Academic Press, 1977.Google Scholar
  100. Rush, D., Mineka, S., and Suomi, S. The effects of control and lack of control on active and passive avoidance in rhesus monkeys. Behavior Research and Therapy, 1982, 20, in press.Google Scholar
  101. Schlottmann, R. S., and Seay, B. M. Mother-infant separation in the Java monkey (Macaca irus). Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1972, 79, 334–340.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  102. Schulz, R. Aging and control. In J. Garber and M. E. P. Seligman (Eds.), Human helplessness: Theory and applications. New York: Academic Press, 1980.Google Scholar
  103. Seay, B. M., Hansen, E. W., and Harlow, H. F. Mother-infant separation in monkeys. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 1962, 3, 123–132.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  104. Seay, B. M., and Harlow, H. F. Maternal separation in the rhesus monkey. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 1965, 140, 434–441.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  105. Seligman, M. E. P. Depression and learned helplessness. In R. J. Friedman and M. M. Katz (Eds.), The psychology of depression: Contemporary theory and research. Washington, D.C.: Winston-Wiley, 1974.Google Scholar
  106. Seligman, M. E. P. Helplessness: On depression, development, and death. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman, 1975.Google Scholar
  107. Seligman, M. E. P., Abramson, L., Semmel, A., and von Baeyer, C. Depressive attributional style. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1979, 88, 242–247.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  108. Seligman, M. E. P., Klein, D. C., and Miller, W. Depression. In H. Leitenberg (Ed.), Handbook of behavior therapy. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1976.Google Scholar
  109. Seligman, M. E. P., and Maier, S. F. Failure to escape traumatic shock. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1967, 74, 1–9.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  110. Seligman, M. E. P., Maier, S. F., and Solomon, R. L. Unpredictable and uncontrollable aversive events. In F. R. Brush (Ed.), Aversive conditioning and learning. New York: Academic Press, 1971.Google Scholar
  111. Sidowski, J. Psychopathological consequences of induced social helpless¬ness during infancy. In H. Kimmel (Ed.), Experimental Psychopathology: Recent research and theory. New York: Academic, 1971.Google Scholar
  112. Singh, M. E. Mother-infant separation in rhesus monkeys living in natural environments. Primates, 1975, 16, 471–476.Google Scholar
  113. Singh, S. D. Effects of infant-infant separation of young monkeys in a free-ranging natural environment. Primates, 1977, 18, 205–214.Google Scholar
  114. Spencer-Booth, Y., and Hinde, R. A. Effects of 6 days separation from mother on 18- to 32-week-old rhesus monkeys. Animal Behaviour, 1971, 19, 174–191.Google Scholar
  115. Spitz, R. A. Anaclitic depression. Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 1946, 2, 313–347.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  116. Stroebel, C. Biologic rhythm correlates of disturbed behavior in the monkey. Bibliotheca Primatologica, 1969, 9, 91–105.Google Scholar
  117. Suomi, S. J. Repetitive peer separation of young monkeys: Effects of vertical chamber confinement during separation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1973, 83, 1–10.Google Scholar
  118. Suomi, S. J., Collins, M. L., and Harlow, H. F. Effects of permanent separation from mother on infant monkeys. Developmental Psychology, 1973, 9, 376–384.Google Scholar
  119. Suomi, S. J., Collins, M. L., Harlow, H. F., and Ruppenthal, G. C. Effects of maternal and peer separations on young monkeys. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 1976, 17, 101–112.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  120. Suomi, S. J., Eisele, C. J., Grady, S. A., and Harlow, H. F. Depression in adult monkeys following separation from nuclear family environment. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1975, 84, 576–578.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  121. Suomi, S. J., and Harlow, H. F. Effects of differential removal from group on social development of rhesus monkeys. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 1975, 16, 149–164.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  122. Suomi, S. J., and Harlow, H. F. Production and alleviation of depressive behaviors in monkeys. In J. Maser and M. E. P. Seligman (Eds.), Psychopathology: Experimental models. San Francisco: Freeman, 1977.Google Scholar
  123. Suomi, S. J., Harlow, H. F., and Domek, C. J. Effect of repetitive infant- infant separation of young monkeys. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1970, 76, 161–172.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  124. Suomi, S., and Mineka, S. Short and long term effects of repetitive mother-infant separations on social development in rhesus monkeys. Submitted for publication.Google Scholar
  125. Suomi, S. J., Mineka, S., and Harlow, H. F. Social separation in monkeys as viewed from several different motivational perspectives. In P. Teitelbaum and E. Satinoff (Eds.), Handbook of behavioral neurobiology: Motivation. New York: Plenum Press, 1982, in press.Google Scholar
  126. Terrace, H. S. Nim: A chimpanzee who learned sign language. New York: Knopf, 1979.Google Scholar
  127. van Lawick-Goodall, J. In the shadow of man. New York: Houghton- Mifflin, 1971.Google Scholar
  128. Vogt, J., Coe, C., Lowe, E., and Levine, S. Behavioral and pituitary- adrenal response of adult squirrel monkeys to mother-infant separation. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 1980, 5, 181–190.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  129. Volpicelli, J., Alienor, A., and Seligman, M. E. P. Learned mastery in the rat. Paper presented at Eastern Psychological Association, Philadelphia, April, 1979.Google Scholar
  130. Watson, J., and Ramey, C. Reaction to response contingent stimulation in early infancy. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 1972, 18, 219–228.Google Scholar
  131. Wolff, C., Friedman, S., Hofer, M., and Mason, J. Relationship between psychological defenses and mean urinary 17-hydroxycorticosteroid excretion rates: A predictive study of parents of fatally ill children. Psychosomatic Medicine, 1964, 26, 576–591. (a)PubMedGoogle Scholar
  132. Wolff, C., Hofer, M., and Mason, J. Relationship between psychological defenses and mean urinary 17-hydroxycorticosteroid excretion rates: Methodologie and theoretical considerations. Psychosomatic Medicine, 1964, 26, 592–609. (b)PubMedGoogle Scholar
  133. Wortman, C. B., and Brehm, J. W. Responses to uncontrollable outcomes: An integration of reactance theory and the learned helplessness model. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology, Vol. 8. New York: Academic Press, 1975.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Press, New York 1982

Authors and Affiliations

  • Susan Mineka
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of Wisconsin-MadisonMadisonUSA

Personalised recommendations