Infant Crying pp 217-239 | Cite as

Aversiveness is in the Mind of the Beholder

Perception of Infant Crying by Adults
  • Ann D. Murray

Abstract

The research reported in this chapter grew out of a long-standing interest in the compelling nature of infant cries and their seemingly paradoxical capacity to elicit either nurturant or hostile responses from caregivers. On the one hand, the biological significance of the cry for survival seems obvious (Bowlby, 1969), but crying also may be maladaptive because it is so frequently cited as a major trigger for child abuse (Frodi & Lamb, 1980; Lester & Zeskind, 1982). Furthermore, if crying has adaptive significance, it seems odd that parental sensitivity and responsiveness to crying are reported to vary so widely between cultures (Mead & Newton, 1967). In primitive and traditional societies, the city is treated as an emergency signal and responded to immediately, but in Western cultures, responses are delayed, and infants may cry for several hours each day.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Ainsworth, M. Object relations, dependency, and attachment: A theoretical review of the mother-infant relationship. Child Development, 1969, 40, 969 - 1025.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bastian, J. Primate signalling systems and human language. In I. Devore (Ed.), Primate behavior: Field studies of monkeys and apes. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston, 1965.Google Scholar
  3. Bell, S., & Ainsworth, M. Infant crying and maternal responsiveness. Child Development, 1972, 43, 1171 - 1190.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bennett, S. Infant-caretaker interactions. American Academy of Child Psychiatry Journal, 1971, 10, 321 - 335.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Berman, P. Are women more responsive than men to the young? A review of developmental and situational variables. Psychological Bulletin, 1980, 88, 668 - 695.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bernal, J. Crying during the first 10 days of life and maternal responses. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 1972, 14, 362–372.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Boukydis, C. F. Z. An analog study of adult physiological and self-report responding to infant cries. Cry Research Newsletter, 1980, 2(2), 2–5.Google Scholar
  8. Bowlby, J. Attachment and loss: Vol. 1 Attachment. New York: Basic Books, 1969.Google Scholar
  9. Brazelton, T. B. Crying in infancy. Pediatrics, 1962, 29, 579–588.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Brown, C. Instruments in psychophysiology. In N. Greenfield & R. Steinbeck (Eds.), Handbook o f psychophysiology. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston, 1972.Google Scholar
  11. Cairns, R. Attachment behavior of mammals. Psychological Review, 1966, 73, 409–426.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Eibl-Eibesfelt, I. Ethology: The biology of behavior( 2nd ed. ). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1975.Google Scholar
  13. Friedman, S., Zahn-Waxler, C., & Radke-Yarrow, M. Perception of cries of full-term and preterm infants. Paper presented at the American Psychological Association Meetings, Montreal, Canada, September. 1980.Google Scholar
  14. Frodi, A., & Lamb, M. Sex differences in responsiveness to infants: A developmental study of psychophysiological and behavioral responses. Child Development, 1978, 49, 1182–1188.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Frodi, A., & Lamb, M. Child abuser’s responses to infant smiles. Child Development, 1980, 51, 238–241.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Frodi, A., Lamb, M., Leavitt, L., & Donovan, W. Fathers’ and mothers’ responses to infant smiles and cries. Infant Behavior and Development1978, 1, 187–198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Frodi, A., Lamb, M., Leavitt, L., Donovan, W., Neff, C., & Sherry, D. Fathers’ and mothers’ responses to the faces and cries of normal and premature infants. Developmental Psychology, 1978, 14, 490–498.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Harper, L. The young as a source of stimuli controlling caretaker behavior. Developmental Psychology, 1971, 4, 73–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Hinde, R. Biological bases of human social behavior. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1974.Google Scholar
  20. Hoffman, M. Developmental synthesis of affect and cognition and its implication for altruistic motivation. Developmental Psychology, 1975, 11, 607–622.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Hoffman, M. A three component model of empathy. Paper presented at a meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, New Orleans, 1977. (a)Google Scholar
  22. Hoffman, M. Sex differences in empathy and related behaviors. Psychological Bulletin, 1977, 84, 712–722. (b)PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Klaus, M., & Kennell, J. Maternal-infant bonding. St. Louis: C. V. Mosby, 1976.Google Scholar
  24. Kryter, K. The effects of noise on man. New York: Academic Press, 1971.Google Scholar
  25. Lenneberg, E., Rebelsky, F., & Nichols, I. The vocalization of infants born to deaf and hearing parents. Human Development, 1965, 8, 23–37.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Lessing, D. A proper marriage. St. Albans, England: Panther Books, 1976.Google Scholar
  27. Lester, B. M., & Zeskind, P. A biobehavioral perspective on crying in early infancy. In H. E. Fitzgerald, B. M. Lester, and M. W. Yogman (Eds.), Theory and research in behavioral pediatrics. New York: Plenum, 1982.Google Scholar
  28. Macfarlane, A. The psychology of childbirth. London: Fontana/Open Books, 1977.Google Scholar
  29. Mead, M., & Newton, N. Cultural patterning of perinatal behavior. In S. Richardson & A. Guttmacher (Eds.), Childbearing: Its social and psychological aspects. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins, 1967.Google Scholar
  30. Miller, J. The effects of noise on people. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 1974, 56, 729–764.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Moltz, H. The ontogeny of maternal behavior in some selected mammalian species. In H. Moltz (Ed.), The ontogeny of vertebrate behavior. New York: Academic Press, 1971.Google Scholar
  32. Moltz, H. Some mechanisms governing the induction, maintenance, and synchrony of maternal behavior in the laboratory rat. In. W. Montagna & W. Sadler (Eds.), Reproductive behavior. New York: Academic Press, 1974.Google Scholar
  33. Money, J., & Erhardt, A. Man and woman, boy and girl. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press, 1972.Google Scholar
  34. Moss, J., & Robson, K. The role of protest behavior in the development of mother-infant attachment. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Psychological Association, San Francisco, August September, 1968.Google Scholar
  35. Murray, A. Infant crying as an elicitor of parental behavior: An examination of two models. Psychological Bulletin, 1979, 86, 191–215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Noirot, E. The onset of maternal behavior in rats, hamsters, and mice: A selective review. In D. Lehrman, R. Hinde, & E. Shaw (Eds.), Advances in the study of behavior(Vol. 4 ). New York: Academic Press, 1972.Google Scholar
  37. Obrist, P. The cardiovascular-behavioral interaction—As it appears today. Psychophysiology, 1976, 13, 95–107.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Osgood, S., Suci, G., & Tannenbaum, P. The measurement of meaning. Urbana: University of Illinois Press, 1957.Google Scholar
  39. Ostwald, P. Soundmaking: The acoustic communication of emotion. Springfield, III.: Charles C Thomas, 1963.Google Scholar
  40. Parke, R. Parent-infant interaction: Progress, paradigms, and problems. In G. Sackett (Ed.), Observing behavior: Vol. 1. Theory and application in mental retardation. Baltimore: University Park Press, 1977.Google Scholar
  41. Piaget, J. The origins of intelligence in children. New York: International Universities Press, 1952.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Rosenblatt, J. Views on the onset and maintenance of maternal behavior in the rat. In L. Aronson, E. Tobach, D. Lehrman, & J. Rosenblatt (Eds.), Development and evolution of behavior. San Francisco: Freeman, 1970.Google Scholar
  43. Sagi, A., & Hoffman, M. Empathic distress in the newborn. Developmental Psychology, 1976, 13, 175–176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Shaver, K. An introduction to the attribution process. Cambridge, Mass.: Winthrop Publishers, 1975.Google Scholar
  45. Simner, M. Newborn’s response to the cry of another infant. Developmental Psychology, 1971, 5, 136–150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Wiesenfeld, A., & Klorman, R. The mother’s psychophysiological reactions to contrasting affective expressions by her own and an unfamiliar infant. Developmental Psychology, 1978, 14, 294–304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Wilson, E. O. Sociobiology: The new synthesis. Cambrige, Mass.: Belknap Press, 1975.Google Scholar
  48. Wolff, P. The natural history of crying and other vocalizations in early infancy. In B. M. Foss (Ed.), Determinants of infant behavior(Vol. 4 ). London: Methuen, 1969.Google Scholar
  49. Wordon, F., & Galambos, R. Auditory processing of biologically significant sounds. Neurosciences Research Program Bulletin, 1972, 10, 1–117.Google Scholar
  50. Zeskind, P. S., & Lester, B. M. Acoustic features and auditory perception of the cries of newborns with prenatal and perinatal complications. Child Development, 1978, 49, 580–582.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Press, New York 1985

Authors and Affiliations

  • Ann D. Murray
    • 1
  1. 1.High Risk Infant Development LaboratoryBoys Town National Institute for Communication Disorders in ChildrenOmahaUSA

Personalised recommendations