Neurophysiology of Consciousness pp 269-306

Part of the Contemporary Neuroscientists book series (CN)

Unconscious cerebral initiative and the role of conscious will in voluntary action

  • Benjamin Libet
Chapter

Abstract

Voluntary acts are preceded by electrophysiological “ readiness potentials” (RPs). With spontaneous acts involving no preplanning, the main negative RP shift begins at about -550 ms. Such RPs were used to indicate the minimum onset times for the cerebral activity that precedes a fully endogenous voluntary act. The time of conscious intention to act was obtained from the subject’s recall of the spatial clock position of a revolving spot at the time of his initial awareness of intending or wanting to move (W). W occurred at about -200 ms. Control experiments, in which a skin stimulus was timed (S), helped evaluate each subject’s error in reporting the clock times for awareness of any perceived event.

For spontaneous voluntary acts, RP onset preceded the uncorrected Ws by about 350 ms and the Ws corrected for S by about 400 ms. The direction of this difference was consistent and significant throughout, regardless ofwhich of several measures of RP onset or W were used. It was concluded that cerebral initiation of a spontaneous voluntary act begins unconsciously. However, it was found that the final decision to act could still be consciously controlled during the 150 ms or so remaining after the specific conscious intention appears. Subjects can in fact “veto” motor performance during a 100—200-ms period before a prearranged time to act.

The role of conscious will would be not to initiate a specific voluntary act but rather to select and control volitional outcome. It is proposed that conscious will can function in a permissive fashion, either to permit or to prevent the motor implementation of the intention to act that arises unconsciously. Alternatively, there may be the need for a conscious activation or triggering, without which the final motor output would not follow the unconscious cerebral initiating and preparatory processes.

Keywords

conscious volition event-related chronometry free will mental timing motor organization readiness potentials unconscious processes voluntary action 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Ach, N. (1935) Analyse des Willens. Urban & Schwarzenberg. [ES]Google Scholar
  2. Akert, K., Peper, K. & Sandri, C. (1975) Structural organization of motor end plate and central synapses. In: Cholinergic mechanisms, ed. P. G. Waser. Raven Press. [JCE]Google Scholar
  3. Allan, L. G. (1978) The attention switching model: Implications for research in schizophrenia. Journal of Psychiatric Research 14:195–202. [taBL]PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Arezzo, J. & Vaughan, H. G., Jr. (1975) Cortical potentials associated with voluntary movements in the monkey. Brain Research 88:99–104. [CCW]PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bauer, H. & Nirnberger, G. (1981) Concept identification as a function of preceding negative or positive spontaneous shifts in slow brain potentials. Psychophysiology 18:466–69. [JSS]PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Beloff, J. (1962) The existence of mind. MacGibbon and Kee. [taBL]Google Scholar
  7. Bolanowski, S. J., Jr. & Doty, R. W. (1982) Monocular loss, binocular maintenance of perception in Ganzfeld. Abstracts, Society for Neuroscience 8:675. [RWD]Google Scholar
  8. Boring, E. G. (1957) A history of experimental psychology. Appleton-Century-Crofts. [taBL]Google Scholar
  9. Born, J., Whipple, S. C. & Stamm, J. (1982) Spontaneous cortical slowpotential shifts and choice reaction time performance. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 54:668–76. [taBL, JSS]PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Breitmeyer, R. & Ganz, L. (1976) Implications of sustained and transient channels for theories of visual pattern masking, saccadic suppression, and information processing. Psychological Review 87: 52–69 [BB]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Bridgeman, B. (1971) Metacontrast and lateral inhibition. Psychological Review 78: 528–39. [BB]PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. (1978) Distributed sensory coding applied to simulations of iconic storage and metaconstrast. Bulletin of Mathematical Biology 40: 605–23. [BB]Google Scholar
  13. Brinkman, C. & Porter, R. (1979) Supplementary motor area in the monkey: Activity of neurons during performance of a learned motor task. Journal of Neurophysiology 42: 681–709. [JCE]PubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Broadbent, D. E. (1971) Decision and stress. Academic Press. [GU]Google Scholar
  15. Campion, J. & Latto, R. (1985) What is a blindsight? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8: 755–757. [RL]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Campion, J., Latto, R. & Smith, Y. M. (1983) Is blindsight an effect of scattered light, spared cortex, and near-threshold vision? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6: 423–86. [RWD, RL]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Cheesman, J. & Merikle, P. M. (1984) Priming with and without awareness. Perception ir Psychophysics 36: 387–95. [PMM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. press) Word recognition and consciousness. In: Reading research: Advances in theory and practice, vol. 5, ed. D. Besner, T. G. Waller & G. E. MacKinnon. Academic Press. [PMM Google Scholar
  19. Churchland, P. S. (1981a) The timing of sensations: Reply to Libet. Philosophy of Science 48: 492–97. [CCW]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. (1981b) On the alleged backwards referral of experiences and its relevance to the mind-body problem. Philosophy of Science 48: 165–81. [CCW]Google Scholar
  21. Creutzfeldt, O. D. & Rager, G. (1978) In: Cerebral correlates of conscious experience, ed. P. A. Buser & A. Rougeul-Buser. Elsevier/North Holland. [taBL]Google Scholar
  22. Deecke, L., Bashore, T., Brunia, C. H. M., Grünewald-Zuberbier, E., Grünewald, G. & Kristeva, R. (1984) Movement-associated potentials and motor control. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 425: 398–428. [CCW]PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Deecke, L., Grö zinger, B. & Kornhuber, H. H. (1976) Voluntary finger movement in man: Cerebral potentials and theory. Biological Cybernetics 23: 99–119. [taRL, CCW]PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Deecke, L. & Kornhuber, H. H. (1978) An electrical sign of participation of the mesial “ supplementary” motor cortex in human voluntary finger movement. Brain Research 159: 473–76. [taBL]PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Dennett, D. C. (I978) Brainstorms: Philosophical essays on mind and psychology. Bradford Books. [CCW]Google Scholar
  26. (1984) Elbow room: The varieties of free will worth wanting. Bradford/MIT Press. [CCW]Google Scholar
  27. Doty, R. W. (1975) consciousness from neurons. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis 35: 791–804. [RWD]PubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Eccles, J. C. (1980) The human psyche. Springer. [taBL]Google Scholar
  29. (1982a) The initiation of voluntary movements by the supplementary motor areas. Archiv für Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten 231: 423–41. [taBL]Google Scholar
  30. (1982b) How the self acts on the brain. Psychoneuroendocrinohgy 7: 271–83. [JCE]Google Scholar
  31. Efron, R. (1973) An invariant characteristic of perceptual systems in the time domain. In: Attention and performance, vol. 4, ed. S. Kornblum. Academic Press. [taBL]Google Scholar
  32. Fitts, P. M. & Deininger, R. L. (1954) S-R compatibility and information reduction. Journal of Experimental Psychology 48: 483–92. [GBR]PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Flachsbart-Kraft, F. (1930) Messung von Hemmungszeiten. Zeitschrift für Psychologie 117: 73–145. [ES]Google Scholar
  34. Fodor, J. (1983) The modularity of mind. Bradford/MIT Press. [CCW]Google Scholar
  35. Freud, S. (1925) Collected papers. Hogarth Press. [CCW]Google Scholar
  36. (1915/1955) The unconscious. Standard Edition. Hogarth Press. [taBL].Google Scholar
  37. Gemba, H., Sasaki, S. & Hashimoto, S. (1980) Distribution of premovement slow cortical potentials associated with self-paced hand movements in monkeys. Neuroscience Letters 20: 159–63. [CCW]PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Gilden, L., Vaughan, H. G., Jr. & Costa, L. D. (1966) Summated human EEG potentials with voluntary movement. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 20: 433–38. [taBL]PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Goldberg, G., Kwan, H. C, Borrett, D. & Murphy, J. T. (1984) Differential topography of the movement-associated scalp potential with internal vs. external dependence of movement timing. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation 65: 630. [rBL]Google Scholar
  40. Green, D. M. & Swets, J. (1966) Signal detection theory and psychophysics. Wiley. [LEM]Google Scholar
  41. Grey Walter, W., Cooper, R., Aldridge, V. J., McCallum, W. C. & Winter, A. L. (1964) Contingent negative variation: An electric sign of sensorimotor association and expectancy in the human brain. Nature 203: 380–84. [RL]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Grözinger, B., Kornhuber, H. H. & Kriebel, J. (1977) Human cerebral potentials preceding speech production, phonation and movements of the mouth and tongue with reference to respiratory and extracerebral potentials. In: Progress in clinical neurophysiology, vol. 3, Language and hemispheric specialization in man: Cerebral event-related potentials, ed. J. E. Desmedt. Karger. [rBL].Google Scholar
  43. Grünewald-Zuberbier, E., Grünewald, G. & Jung, R. (1978) Slow potentials of the human precentral and parietal cortex during goal-directed. movements (Zielbewegungspotentiale). Journal of Physiology (London) 284:181–82P. [RJ]Google Scholar
  44. Haber, R. N. & Hershenson, M. (1980) The psychology of visual perception, 2d ed. Holt, Rinehart & Winston. [RL]Google Scholar
  45. Hammer, A. (1914) Untersuchung der Hemmung einer vorbereiteten Willenshandlung. Psychologische Studien 9: 321–65. [ES]Google Scholar
  46. Hamad, S. (1982) Consciousness: An afterthought. Cognition and Brain Theory 5: 29–47. [BGB, tarBL]Google Scholar
  47. (unpublished) Conscious events cannot be localized in real time. [taBL]Google Scholar
  48. Hashimoto, S., Gemba, H. & Sasaki, K. (1980) Premovement slow cortical potentials and required muscle force in self-paced hand movements in the monkey. Brain Research 197: 415–23. [CCW]PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Hebb, D. O. (1980) Essay on mind. Erlbaum. [CHV]Google Scholar
  50. Hillyard, S. A., Simpson, G. V., Woods, D. L., Van Voorhis, S. & Munte, T. F. (1984) Event-related brain potentials and selective attention to different modalities. In: Cortical integration, ed. F. Reinoso-Suarez & C. Ajmone-Marsan. Raven Press. [RL]Google Scholar
  51. Hofstadler, D. R. (1979) Gödel, Escher, Bach An eternal golden braid. Basic Books. [CCW]Google Scholar
  52. Holender, D. (1986) Semantic activation without conscious identification in dichotic listening, parafoveal vision, and visual masking: A survey and appraisal. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, in press. [RL]Google Scholar
  53. Hook, S., ed. (1960). Dimensions of mind. New York University Press. [taBL]Google Scholar
  54. Hunzelmann, N. & Spillmann, L. (1984) Movement adaptation in the peripheral retina. Vision Research 24: 1765–69. [RWD]PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Jack, J. J. B., Redman, S. J. & Wong, K. (1981) The components of synaptic potentials evoked in cat spinal motoneurones by impulses in single group la afferents. Journal of Physiology 321: 65–96. [JCE]PubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Jacobson, E. (1930a) Electrical measurements of neuromuscular states during mental activities. 1. Imagination of movement involving skeletal muscle. American Journal of Physiology 91: 567–608. [CHV]Google Scholar
  57. (1930b) Electrical measurements of neuromuscular states during mental activities. 2. Imagination and recollection of various muscular acts. American Journal of Physiology 94: 22–34. [CHV]Google Scholar
  58. James, W. (1890) The principles of psychology. Holt. Dover repr., 1950. [BGB, ES]Google Scholar
  59. Jonides, J. (1981) Voluntary versus automatic control over the mind’s eye’s movement. In: Attention and performance, vol. 9, ed. J. B. Long & A. D. Baddeley. Erlbaum [BGB]Google Scholar
  60. Jung, R. (1981) Perception and action. In: Advances in physiological sciences. Regulatory functions of the CNS: Motion and organization principles, ed. J. Szentágothai, M. Palkovits, & J. Hámori. Pergamon Press; Adadémiai Kiadó. [RJ]Google Scholar
  61. (1982) Postural support of goal-directed movements: The preparation and guidance of voluntary action in man. Acta Biologica Academiae Scientiarum Hungaricae 33: 201–13. [RJ]Google Scholar
  62. (1984) Electrophysiological cues of the language dominant hemisphere in man: Slow brain potentials during language processing and writing. Experimental Brain Research Suppl. 9: 430–50. [RJ]Google Scholar
  63. Jung, R., Altenmüller, E. & Natsch, B. (1984) Zur Hemisphä rendominanz für Sprache and Rechnen: Elektrophysiologische Korrelate einer Linksdominanz bei Linkshä ndern. Neuropsychologia 22: 755–75. [RJ]PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Jung, R., Hufschmidt, A. & Moschallski, W. (1982) Langsame Hirnpotentiale beim Schreiben: Die Wechselwirking von Schreibhand und Sprachdominanz bei Rechtshä ndern. Archiv für Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten 232: 305–24. [RJ]PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Kaufmann, W. (1961) The faith of a heretic. Doubleday. [taBL]Google Scholar
  66. Korn, H. & Faber, D. S. (I985) Regulation and significance of probabilistic release mechanisms at central synapses. In: New insights into synaptic function, ed. G. M. Edelman, W. E. Gall & W. M. Cowan, Neurosciences Research Foundation. Wiley. In press. [JCE]Google Scholar
  67. Komhuber, H. H. & Deecke, L. (1965) Hirnpotentialä nderungen bei Willkürbewegungen und passiven Bewegungen des Menschen; Bereitschaftspotential und reafferente Potentiale. Pfl#x00FC;gers Archiv für Gesamte Physiologie 284:1–17. [RWD, RJ, taBL, RN]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Krech, D. (1969) Does behavior really need a brain? In: William James. Unfinished business, ed. R. B. McLeod. American Psychological Association. [rBL]Google Scholar
  69. Küpfmüller, K. (1971) Grundlagen der Informationstheorie und der Kybernetik. In: Physiologie des Menschen, vol. 10, Allgemeine Neurophysiologie, ed. O. H.A Gauer, K. Kramer, & R. Jung. Urban & Schwarzenberg. [RJ]Google Scholar
  70. Laszlo, J. (1966) The performance of a simple motor task with kinaesthetic sense loss. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 18:1–8. [rBL, CHV]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Latto, R. & Campion, J. (1986) Approaches to consciousness: Psychophysics or philosophy? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, in press. [RL]Google Scholar
  72. Leibniz, G. W. F. von (1704/1916). New essays concerning human understanding. Macmillan. [LEM]Google Scholar
  73. Libet, B. (1965) Cortical activation in conscious and unconscious experience. Perspectives in Biology and Medicine 9: 77–86. [tarBL]PubMedGoogle Scholar
  74. (1966) Brain stimulation and the threshold of conscious experience. In: Brain and conscious experience, ed. J. C. Eccles. Springer. [tarBL, CCW]Google Scholar
  75. (1973) Electrical stimulation of cortex in human subjects, and conscious sensory aspects. In: Handbook of sensory physiology, ed. A. Iggo. Springer. [JCE, tarBL, CCW]Google Scholar
  76. (1978) Neuronal vs. subjective timing for a conscious sensory experience. In: Cerebral correlates of conscious experience, ed. P. A. Buser & A. Rougeul-Buser. Elsevier/North Holland Biomé dical Press. [rBL]Google Scholar
  77. (1979) Can a theory based on some cell properties define the timing of mental activities? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2: 270–71. [rBL]Google Scholar
  78. (1981a) The experimental evidence for subjective referral of a sensory experience backwards in time. Philosophy of Science 48:182–97. [tarBL, CCW]Google Scholar
  79. (1981b) ERPs and conscious awareness; neurons and glia as generators. In: Electrophysiological approaches to human cognitive processing. NRP Bulletin, vol. 20, ed. R. Galambos & S. A. Hillyard, MIT Press. [tarBL]Google Scholar
  80. (1982) Brain stimulation in the study of neuronal functions for conscious sensory experiences. Human Neurobiology 1: 235–42. [tarBL]Google Scholar
  81. Libet, B., Gleason, C. A., Wright, E. W. & Pearl, D. K. (1983) Time of conscious intention to act in relation to onset of cerebral activities (readiness-potential); the unconscious initiation of a freely voluntary act. Brain 106: 623–42. [BGB, tarBL, GU, CCW]PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. Libet, B., Wright, E. W., Jr., Feinstem, B. & Pearl, D. K. (1979) Subjective referral of the timing for a conscious sensory experience. A functional role for the somatosensory specific projection system in man. Brain 102: 193–224. [RL, tarBL, CCW]PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. Libet, B., Wright, E. W., Jr. & Gleason, C. A. (1982) Readiness-potentials preceding unrestricted “ spontaneous” vs. pre-planned voluntary acts. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 54: 322–35. [tarBL, JSS]PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. (1983) Preparation-or intention-to-act, in relation to pre-event potentials recorded at the vertex. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 56:367–72. [RL, tarBL]Google Scholar
  85. McCloskey, D. I., Colebatch, J. G., Potter, E. K. & Burke, D. (1983) Judgements about onset of rapid voluntary movements in man. Journal of Neurophysiology 49: 851–63. [taBL, CCW]PubMedGoogle Scholar
  86. MacKay, D. M. (1951) Mindlike behaviour in artefacts. British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 2: 105–121. [DMM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  87. (1965) From mechanism to mind. In: Brain and mind, ed. J. R. Smythies. Routledge and Kegan Paul. [DMM]Google Scholar
  88. (1966) Cerebral organizaton and the conscious control of action. In: Brain and conscious experience, ed. J. C. Eceles. Springer. fDMM]Google Scholar
  89. (1980) The interdependence of mind and brain. Neuroscience 5:1389–91. [DMM]Google Scholar
  90. (1982) Ourselves and our brains: Duality without dualism. Psychoneuroendocrinology 7:285–94. [DMM]Google Scholar
  91. Margenau, H. (1984) The miracle of existence. Ox Bow Press. [JCE, taBL, RJN]Google Scholar
  92. Melzack, R. & Bromage, P. R. (1973) Experimental phantom limbs. Experimental Neurology 39:261–69. [rBL, CHV]PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  93. Milgram, S. (1974) Obedience to authority: An experimental view. Harper & Row. [RL]Google Scholar
  94. Nagel, T. (1979). Mortal questions. Cambridge University Press. [taBL]Google Scholar
  95. Naäätänen, R. (1971) Non-aging fore-periods and simple reaction time. Acta Psychologica 35: 316–27. [RN]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  96. (in press) Selective attention and stimulus processing: Reflections in evoked potentials, magnetoencephalograms, and regional cerebral blood flow. In: Attention and performance, vol. 11, ed. M. I. Posner & O. S. Marin. Erlbaum. [RN]Google Scholar
  97. Näätänen, R., Gaillard, A. W. K. & Mä ntysalo, S. (1978) Early selective-attention effect on evoked potential reinterpreted. Acta Psychologica 42: 313–29. [RN]PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  98. (1980) Brain potential correlates of voluntary and involuntary attention. In: Motivation, motor and sensory processes of the brain: Electrical potentials, behaviour and clinical use, ed. H. H. Kornhuber & L. Deecke. Elsevier. [RN]Google Scholar
  99. Näätänen, R. & Koskinen, P. (1975) Simple reaction times with very small imperative-stimulus probabilities. Acta Psychologica 39: 43–50. [RN]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  100. Näätänen, R. & Merisalo, A. (1977) Expectancy and preparation in simple reaction time. In: Attention and performance, vol. 6, ed. S. Domic. Erlbaum. [RN]Google Scholar
  101. Nääntänen, R., Sams. M. & Alho, K. (in press) The mismatch negativity: The ERP sign of a cerebral mismatch process. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, Supplement. [RN]Google Scholar
  102. Näätänen, R., Simpson, M. & Loveless, N. E. (1982) Stimulus deviance and evoked potentials. Biological Psychology 14: 53–98. [RN]PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  103. Nisbett, R. E. & Wilson, R. E. (1977) Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review 84: 231–59. [CHV]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  104. Norman, D. A. & Shallice, R. (1980) Attention to action: Willed and automatic control of behavior. Technical report, Center for Human Information Processing, University of California, San Diego. [BB]Google Scholar
  105. Oshima, T. (1983) Intracortical organization of arousal as a model of dynamic neuronal processes that may involve a set for movements. In: Motor control mechanisms in health and disease, ed. J. E. Desmedt. Raven Press. [JCE]Google Scholar
  106. Papakostopoulos, D. & Cooper, R. (1973) The contingent negative variation and the excitability of the spinal monosynaptic reflex. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 36: 1003–10. [CHV]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  107. Penfield, W. (1958) The excitable cortex in conscious man. Liverpool University Press. [RL, taBL]Google Scholar
  108. Popper, K. R. & Eceles, J. C. (1977) The self and its brain. Springer [taBL, RJN, CCW]Google Scholar
  109. Posner, M. I. (1980) The orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 32: 3–25. [MDR]PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  110. Pylyshyn, Z. (1973) What the mind’s eye tells the mind’s brain: A critique of mental imagery. Psychological Bulletin 80: 1–24. [CHV]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  111. Reason, J. (1979) Actions not as planned: The effects of automatisation. In: Aspects of consciousness, vol. 1, ed. G. Underwood & R. Stevens. Academic Press. [GU]Google Scholar
  112. Remington, R. (1980) Attention and saccadic eye movements. Journal of Experiental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 6: 726–44. [BGB]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  113. Roland, P. E. (1978) Sensory feedback to the cerebral cortex during voluntary movement in man. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1: 129–71. [RWD]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  114. Rollman, G. B. (1974) Electrocutaneous stimulation. In: Cutaneous communication systems and devices, ed. F. A. Geldard. Psychonomic Society. [GBR]Google Scholar
  115. Rozhkova, G. I., Nickolayev, P. P. & Shchadrin, V. E. (1982) Perception of stabilized retinal stimuli in dichoptic viewing conditions. Vision Research 22: 293–302. [RWD]PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  116. Schreiber, H., Lang, M., Lang, W, Kornhuber, A., Heise, B., Keidel, M., Deecke, L. & Kornhuber, H. H. (1983) Frontal hemispheric differences of the Bereitschaftspotential associated with writing and drawing. Human Neurobiology 2: 197–202. [RJ]PubMedGoogle Scholar
  117. Shibasaki, H., Barrett, G., Halliday, E. & Halliday, A. M. (1980) Components of the movement-related cortical potential and their scalp topography. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 49: 213–26. [taBL]PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  118. Skinner, B. F. (1974) About behaviorism. Alfred A. Knopf. [CHV]Google Scholar
  119. Sperry, R. W. (1980) Mind-brain interaction: Mentalism, yes; dualism, no. Neuroscience 5: 195–206. [tarBL]PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  120. Stamm, J. S. & Gillespie, O. (1980) Task acquisition with feedback of steady potential shifts from monkeys prefrontal cortex. In: Motivation, motor and sensory processes of the brain: Electrical potentials, behavior and clinical use, ed. H. H. Kornhuber & L. Deecke. Elsevier. [JSS]Google Scholar
  121. Sternberg, S. & Knoll, R. L. (1973) The perception of temporal order: Fundamental issues and a general model. In: Attention and performance, vol. 4, ed. S. Kornblum. Academic Press. [taBL, CCW]Google Scholar
  122. Swets, J. A., Tanner, W. P. & Birdsall, T. G. (1961). Decision processes in perception. Psychological Review 68:301–40. [LEM]PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  123. Thorpe, W. H. (1974) Animal nature and human nature. Methuen. [taBL]Google Scholar
  124. Underwood, G. (1982) Attention and awareness in cognitive and motor skills. In: Aspects of consciousness, vol. 3, ed. G. Underwood. Academic Press. [GU]Google Scholar
  125. Uttal, W. R. (1971) The psychobiological silly season, or what happens when neurophysiological data become psychological theories. Journal of General Psychology 84:151–66. [BB]Google Scholar
  126. Vanderwolf, C. H. (1983a) The influence of psychological concepts on brainbehavior research. In: Behavioral approaches to brain research, ed. T. E. Robinson. Oxford University Press. [CHV]Google Scholar
  127. (1983b) The role of the cerebral cortex and ascending activating systems in the control of behavior. In: Handbook of behavioral neurobiology, vol. 6, Motivation}, ed. E. Satinoff & P. Teitelbaum. Plenum Press. [CHV]Google Scholar
  128. Vanderwolf, C. H. & Robinson, T. E. (1981) Reticulo-cortical activity and behavior: A critique of the arousal theory and a new synthesis. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4: 459–514. [CHV]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  129. Vaughan, H. G., Jr. (1982) The neural origins of human event-related potentials. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 388: 125–38. [CCW]PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  130. Vaughan, H. G., Jr., Costa, L. D. & Ritter, W. (1968) Topography of the human motor potential. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 25: 1–10. [taBL]PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  131. Wagstaff, G. F. (1981). Hypnosis, compliance and belief. St. Martin’s Press. [RL]Google Scholar
  132. Wasserman, G. S. (1978) Limulus psychophysics: Temporal summation in the ventral eye. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 107: 276–86. [GSW]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  133. Wasserman, G. S. &: Kong, K.-L. (1979) Absolute timing of mental activities. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2: 243–304. [GSW]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  134. Weisstein, N. (1972) Metacontrast. In: Handbook of sensory physiology, vol. 7(4). Visual Psychophysics, ed. D. Jameson & L. Hurvich. Springer. [BB]Google Scholar
  135. Whyte, L. L. (1960) The unconscious before Freud. Basic Books. [taBL]Google Scholar
  136. Wood, C. C. & Allison, T. (1981) Interpretation of evoked potentials: A neurophysiological perspective. Canadian Journal of Psychology 35: 113–35. [CCW]PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  137. Woodworth, R. S. (1938) Experimental psychology. Holt. [ES]Google Scholar
  138. Wundt, W. (1874/1904). Principles of physiological psychology, vol. 2. Macmillan. [BGB]Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 1993

Authors and Affiliations

  • Benjamin Libet
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Physiology, School of MedicineUniversity of CaliforniaSan FranciscoUSA

Personalised recommendations