Advertisement

Comparing the Complex Cognition of Birds and Primates

  • Nathan J. Emery
  • Nicola S. Clayton
Chapter
Part of the Developments in Primatology: Progress and Prospects book series (DIPR)

Abstract

At first glance, birds and non-human primates (hereafter primates) seem very different. Birds have beaks, feathers, produce offspring that gestate in shells, and can fly. Primates are covered in hair, have forward facing eyes and grasping hands, and while some are arboreal, none of them can fly. Although there are vast morphological differences between the two groups, there is growing evidence of strong similarities in their mental abilities, particularly in the realm of advanced cognitive processing. This suggestion is initially surprising considering the vast difference between the size and structure of avian and primate brains. Even more surprising is the fact that a number of recent experiments have failed to demonstrate some complex cognitive abilities in primates (including chimpanzees, gorillas, and orangutans) that have been convincingly demonstrated in some species of birds, such as the corvids (jays, crows, ravens, and magpies) and parrots.

Keywords

Retention Interval Alarm Call Complex Cognition Mental Time Travel Artificial Fruit 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Akins, C. K., and Zentall, T. R, 1996, Imitative learning in male Japanese quail (Coturnix japonica) using the two-action method, J. Comp. Psychol. 110: 316–320Google Scholar
  2. Balda, R. P., and Kamil, A. C, 1989, A comparative study of cache recovery by three corvid species, Anim. Behav. 38: 486–495Google Scholar
  3. Balda, R P., Kamil, A. C, and Bednekoff, P. A., 1997, Predicting cognitive capacity from natural history: Examples from species of corvids, in: Current Ornithology, Volume 13, V. Nolan and E. D. Ketterson, eds., Plenum Press, New York, pp. 33–66Google Scholar
  4. Basil, J. A., Kamil, A. C, Balda, R A., and Fite, K. V., 1996, Differences in hippocampal volume in food-storing corvids, Brain Behav. Evol. 47: 156–164Google Scholar
  5. Baylis, J. R, 1982, Avian vocal mimicry: Its function and evolution, in: Acoustic Communication in Birds, D. E. Kroodsma, E. H. Miller, and H. Ouellet, eds., Academic Press, London, pp. 51–83Google Scholar
  6. Bednekoff, P. A., and Balda, R A., 1996a, Observational spatial memory in Clark’s nutcrackers and Mexican jays, Anim. Behav. 52: 833–839Google Scholar
  7. Bednekoff, P. A., and Balda, R A., 1996b, Social caching and observational spatial memory in Pinyon jays, Behaviour 133: 807–826Google Scholar
  8. Berthold, P., Heilbig, A. J., Mohr, G., and Querner, U., 1992, Rapid microevolution of migratory behaviour in a wild bird species. Nature 360: 668–670Google Scholar
  9. Birch, H. G., 1945, The relation of previous experience to insightful problem solving, J. Comp. Psych. 38: 367–383Google Scholar
  10. Bischof, N., 1978, On the phylogeny of human morality, in: Morality as a Biological Phenomenon, G. Stent, eds.,Berlin, Abakon, pp. 53–74Google Scholar
  11. Bischof-Koehler, D., 1985, Zur Phylogenese menschlicher motivation [On the phylogeny of human motivation], in: Emotion una 1 Reflexivitaet, L. H. Eckensberger and E. D. Lantermann, eds., Vienna, Urban & Schwarzenberg, pp. 3–47Google Scholar
  12. Boesch, C., and Boesch, H., 1983, Optimization of nut-cracking with natural hammers by wild chimpanzees, Behaviour 83: 265–286Google Scholar
  13. Brockmann, H. J., and Barnard, C. J., 1979, Kleptoparasitism in birds, Anim. Behav. 27: 487–514Google Scholar
  14. Bugnyar, T., and Huber, L., 1997, Push or pull: An experimental study on imitation in marmosets, Anim. Behav. 54: 817–831Google Scholar
  15. Bugnyar, T., and Kotrschal, K., 2001, Do ravens manipulate others’ attention in order to prevent or achieve social learning opportunities? Adv. Ethol. 36: 106Google Scholar
  16. Bugnyar, T., and Kotrschal, K., 2002, Observational learning and the raiding of food caches in ravens, Corvus comx: Is it “tactical” deception? Anim. Behav. 64: 185–195Google Scholar
  17. Byrne, R. W., 1995, The Thinking Ape: Evolutionary Origins of Intelligence, Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  18. Byrne, R. W., 1996, Relating brain size to intelligence in primates, in: Modelling the Early Human Mind, P. A. Mellars and K. R Gibson, eds., MacDonald Institute for Archaeological Research, Cambridge, pp. 49–56Google Scholar
  19. Byrne, R W., and Whiten, A., eds., 1998, Machiavellian Intelligence: Social Expertise and the Evolution of Intellect in Monkeys, Apes and Humans, Clarendon Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
  20. Campbell, F. M., Heyes, C. M., and Goldsmith, A. R, 1999, Stimulus learning and response learning by observation in the European starling, in a two-object/two action test, Anim. Behav. 58: 151–158Google Scholar
  21. Catchpole, C. K., and Slater, P. J. B., 1995, Bird Song: Themes and Variations, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar
  22. Chappell, J., and Kacelnik, A., 2002, Tool selectivity in a non-primate, the New Caledonian crow (Corvus moneduloides), Animal Cognition 5: 71–78Google Scholar
  23. Cheney, D. L., and Seyfarth, R M., 1985, Vervet monkey alarm calls: Manipulation through shared information? Behaviour 93: 150–166Google Scholar
  24. Cheney, D. L., and Seyfarth, R M., 1990, How Monkeys See the World: Inside the Mind of Another Species, University of Chicago Press, Chicago.Google Scholar
  25. Clayton, D. A., 1978, Socially facilitated behavior, Q. Rev. Biol. 53: 373–391Google Scholar
  26. Clayton, N. S., 1998, Memory and the hippocampus in food-storing birds: A comparative approach, Neuropharmacol. 37: 441–452Google Scholar
  27. Clayton, N. S., and Dickinson, A., 1998, Episodic-like memory during cache recovery by scrub jays, Nature 395: 272–278Google Scholar
  28. Clayton, N. S., and Dickinson, A., 1999a, Scrub jays (Aphelocoma coerulescens) remember the relative time of caching as well as the location and content of their caches, J. Comp. Psychol. 113: 403–416Google Scholar
  29. Clayton, N. S., and Dickinson, A., 1999b, Memory for the contents of caches by Scrub jays, J. Exp. Psychol.: Anim. Behav. Proc. 25: 82–91Google Scholar
  30. Clayton, N. S., and Jolliffe, A., 1996, Marsh tits (Parus palustris) use tools to store food., Ibis 138: 554Google Scholar
  31. Clayton, N. S., Griffiths, D. P., and Bennett, A. T. D., 1994, Storage of stones by jays Garrulus glandarius, Ibis 136: 331–334Google Scholar
  32. Clayton, N. S., Yu, K., and Dickinson, A., 2001a, Scrub jays (Aphelocoma coerulescens) can form an integrated memory for multiple features of caching episodes, J. Exp. Psychol.: Anim. Behav. Proc. 27: 17–29Google Scholar
  33. Clayton, N. S., Griffiths, D. P., Emery, N. J., and Dickinson, A., 2001b, Elements of episodic-like memory in animals, Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. Lond.: B. 356: 1483–1491Google Scholar
  34. Cristol, D., and Switzer, P. V., 1999, Avian prey-dropping behavior. II. American crows and walnuts, Behav. Ecol. 10: 220–226Google Scholar
  35. Custance, D., Whiten, A., and Freedman, T., 1999, Social learning of an artificial fruit task in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella), J. Comp. Psychol. 113: 13–23Google Scholar
  36. Dawkins, M. S., 2002, What are birds looking at> Head movements and eye use in chickens, Anim. Behav. 63: 991–998Google Scholar
  37. Dawson, B. V., and Foss, B. M., 1965, Observational learning in budgerigars, Anim. Behav. 13: 470–474Google Scholar
  38. Deacon, T., 1990, Rethinking mammalian brain evolution, Am. Zool. 30: 629–705Google Scholar
  39. D’Eath, R. B., 1998, Can video images imitate real stimuli in animal behaviour experiments? Biol. Rev. 73: 267–292Google Scholar
  40. De Vore, I., ed., 1965, Primate Behavior: Field Studies of Monkeys and Apes, Holt Reinhart Winston, New York.Google Scholar
  41. de Waal, F. B. M., ed., 2001, Tree of Origin: What Primate Behavior can tell us about Human Social Evolution, Harvard University Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar
  42. Durstewitz, D., Kroner, S., and Gunturkun, O., 1999, Dopamine innervation of the avian telencephalon, Prog. Neurobiol. 59: 161–195Google Scholar
  43. Emery, N. J., 2000, The eye have it: The neuroethology, evolution and function of social gaze, Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 24: 581–604Google Scholar
  44. Emery, N. J., and Clayton, N. S., 2001, Effects of experience and social context on prospective caching strategies by scrub jays, Nature 414: 443–446Google Scholar
  45. Emery, N. J., Lorincz, E. N., Perrett, D. I., Oram, M. W., and Baker, C. I., 1997, Gaze following and joint attention in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatto),. J. Comp. Psychol. 111: 286–293Google Scholar
  46. Epstein, R., Lanza, R P., and Skinner, B. F., 1981, “Self-awareness” in the pigeon, Science 212: 695–696Google Scholar
  47. Evans, C. S., Evans, L., and Marler, P., 1993, On the meaning of alarm calls: Functional reference in an avian vocal system, Anim. Behav. 46: 23–38Google Scholar
  48. Fabre, J. H., 1916, The Hunting Wasps. Hodder and Stoughton.Google Scholar
  49. Flaherty, C. F., and Checke, S., 1982, Anticipation of incentive gain, Anim. Learn. Behav. 10: 177–182Google Scholar
  50. Fritz, J., and Kotrschal, K., 1999, Social learning in common ravens, Corvus corax, Anim. Behav. 57: 785–793Google Scholar
  51. Gallup, G. G., Jr., 1970, Chimpanzees: Self-recognition, Science 167: 341–343Google Scholar
  52. Gallup, G. G., Jr., 1982, Self-awareness and the emergence of mind in primates, Am. J. Primatol. 2:237–248.Google Scholar
  53. Gallup, G. G., Jr., 1994, Self-recognition: Research strategies and experimental design, in: Self-Awareness in Animals and Humans: Developmental Perspectives, S. T. Parker, R. W. Mitchell, and M. L. Boccia, eds., Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 35–50.Google Scholar
  54. Gallup, G. G., Jr., and Capper, S. A., 1970, Preference for mirror-image stimulation in finches (Passer domesticus domesticus) and parakeets (Melopsittacus undulates), Anim. Behav. 18: 621–624Google Scholar
  55. Gallup, G. G., Jr., Povinelli, D. J., Suarez, S. D., Anderson, J. R, Lethmate, J., and Menzel, E. W., Jr., 1995, Further reflections on self-recognition in primates, Anim. Behav. 50: 1525–1532Google Scholar
  56. Gardner, R A., and Gardner, B. T., 1969, Teaching sign language to a chimpanzee, Science 165: 664–672Google Scholar
  57. Gerlai, R, and Clayton, N. S., 1999, Analyzing hippocampal function in transgenic mice: An ethological perspective, TINS 22: 47–51Google Scholar
  58. Gibson, K. R, 1986, Cognition, brain size, and the extraction of embedded food resources, in: Primate Ontogeny, Cognition and Social Behavior, J. G. Else and P. C. Lee, eds., Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 93–105.Google Scholar
  59. Goldman-Rakic, P. S., Lidow, M. S., Smiley, J. R, and Williams, M. S., 1992, The anatomy of dopamine in monkey and human prefrontal cortex, J. Neural Trans. Suppl. 36: 163–177Google Scholar
  60. Goodall, J., 1986, The Chimpanzees of Gombe. Patterns of Behavior. Harvard University Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar
  61. Gould-Beierle, K., 2000, A comparison of four corvid species in a working and reference memory task using a radial arm maze,. Comp. Psychol. 114: 347–356Google Scholar
  62. Granon, S., and Poucet, B., 1995, Medial prefrontal lesions in the rat and spatial navigation: Evidence for impaired planning, Behav. Neurosci. 109: 474–484Google Scholar
  63. Griffiths, D., Dickinson, A., and Clayton, N. S., 1999, Episodic and declarative memory: What can animals remember about their past? Trends Cog. Sci. 3: 74–80Google Scholar
  64. Gunturkiin, O., 1997, Cognitive impairments after lesions of the neostriatum caudolaterale and its thalamic afferent in pigeons: Functional similarities to the mammalian prefrontal system? J. Hirnforsch 38: 133–143Google Scholar
  65. Gyger, M., Karakashian, S., and Marler, P., 1986, Avian alarm calling: Is there an audience effect? Anim. Behav. 34: 1570–1572Google Scholar
  66. Hampton, R R, 1994, Sensitivity to information specifying the line of gaze of humans in sparrows (Passer domesticus).Behaviour 130: 41–51Google Scholar
  67. Hare, B., 2001, Can competitive paradigms increase the validity of experiments on primate social cognition? Anim. Cog. 4: 269–280Google Scholar
  68. Hare, B., and Tomasello, M., 1999, Domestic dogs (Canisfamiliaris) use human and conspecific social cues to locate hidden food,. Comp. Psychol. 113: 173–177Google Scholar
  69. Hare, B., Call, J., and Tomasello, M., 2001, Do chimpanzees know what conspecifics know? Anim. Behav. 61: 139–151Google Scholar
  70. Hare, B., Call, J., Agnetta, B., and Tomasello, M., 2000, Chimpanzees know what conspecifics do and do not see, Anim. Behav. 59: 771–785Google Scholar
  71. Hartmann, B., and Gunturkun, O., 1998, Selective deficits in reversal learning after neostriatum caudolaterale lesions in pigeons: Possible behavioral equivalencies to the mammalian prefrontal system, Behav. Brain Res. 96: 125–133Google Scholar
  72. Hauser, M. D., 2000, Wild Minds: What Animals Really Think. Henry Holt and Company, New York.Google Scholar
  73. Heinrich, B., 1996, An experimental investigation of insight in common ravens, Corvus corax, Auk 112: 994–1003Google Scholar
  74. Heinrich, B., 1999, The Mind of the Raven: Investigations and Adventures with Wolf-Birds. Google Scholar
  75. Heinrich, B., 2000, Testing insight in ravens, in: The Evolution of Cognition, C. M. Heyes and L. Huber, eds., MIT Press, Cambridge, pp. 289–309.Google Scholar
  76. Heinrich, B., and Pepper, J. W., 1998, Influence of competitors on caching behavior in the common raven, Anim. Behav. 56: 1083–1090Google Scholar
  77. Heinrich, B., and Smolker, R., 1998, Play in common ravens (Corvus cor ax), in: Animal Play: Evolutionary, Comparative and Ecological Perspectives, M. Bekoff and J. A. Byers, eds., Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 27–44.Google Scholar
  78. Held, S., Mendl, M., Devereux, C, and Byrne, R. W., 2001, Behaviour of domestic pigs in a visual perspective taking task, Behaviour 138: 1337–1354Google Scholar
  79. Heyes, CM., 1996, Genuine imitation? in: Social Learning in Animals: The Roots of Culture, C. M. Heyes, and B. G. Jr. Galef, (eds.) Academic Press, San Diego, pp. 371–404.Google Scholar
  80. Heyes, C. M., 1998, Theory of mind in nonhuman primates, Behav. Brain Sci. 21: 101–148Google Scholar
  81. Heyes, C. M., and Dawson, G. R., 1990, A demonstration of observational learning using a bi-directional control, Q.J. Exp. Psychol. 42B: 59–71Google Scholar
  82. Heyes, C. M., and Galef, B. G. Jr., eds., 1996, Social Learning in Animals: The Roots of Culture, Academic Press, Inc., San Diego.Google Scholar
  83. Hodos, W., and Campbell, C. B. G., 1969, Scala naturae: Why there is no theory in comparative psychology, Psych. Rev. 76: 337–350Google Scholar
  84. Horn, G., 1985, Memory, Imprinting and the Brain: An Inquiry into Mechanisms. Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
  85. Huber, L., Rechberger, S., and Taborsky, M., 2001, Social learning affects object exploration and manipulation in keas, Nestor notabilis, Anim. Behav. 62: 945–954Google Scholar
  86. Humphrey, N. K., 1976, The social function of intellect, in: Growing Points in Ethology, P. P. G. Bateson and R. A. Hinde, eds., Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 303–317.Google Scholar
  87. Hunt, G. R., 1996, Manufacture and use of hook-tools by New Caledonian crows, Nature 379: 249–251Google Scholar
  88. Hunt, G. R, 2000, Human-like, population-level specialization in the manufacture of pandanus tools by the New Caledonian crows (Corvus moneduloides), Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond:B. 267: 403–413Google Scholar
  89. Jerison, H. J., 1971, Evolution of Brain and Intelligence, Academic Press, Inc., New York.Google Scholar
  90. Jolly, A., 1966, Lemur social behavior and primate intelligence, Science 153: 501–507Google Scholar
  91. Jones, T. B., and Kamil, A. C, 1973, Tool-making and tool-using in the northern blue jay, Science 180: 1076–1078Google Scholar
  92. Kamil, A. C, 1988, A synthetic approach to the study of animal intelligence, in: Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, Volume 35, D. W. Leger, ed., University of Nebraska Press, Lincoln, pp. 257–308.Google Scholar
  93. Kamil, A. C, Balda, R P., and Olson, D. J., 1994, Performance of four seed-caching corvid species in the radial-arm maze analog,. Comp. Psychol. 108: 385–393Google Scholar
  94. Kaminski, J., 2002, Do animals know what others can and cannot see. Paper presented at the 2nd International Symposium on Comparative Cognitive Science, Inuyama, Japan.Google Scholar
  95. Karakashian, S. J., Gyger, M., and Marler, P., 1988, Audience effects on alarm calling in chickens (Gallusgalius), J Comp. Psychol. 102: 129–135Google Scholar
  96. Karten, H. J., 1969, The organization of the avian telencephalon and some speculations on the phylogeny of the amniote telencephalon, in: Comparative and Evolutionary Aspects of the Vertebrate Central Nervous System, J. N. Petras and C. Noback, eds., New York Academy of Sciences, New York, pp. 164–179.Google Scholar
  97. Karten, H. J., 1991, Homology and the evolutionary origins of the “neocortex”. Brain, Behav. Evol. 38: 264–272Google Scholar
  98. Kohler, W., 1927, The Mentality of Apes. Routledge & Kegan Paul (original work published in 1917, English translation by E. Winter, 1927).Google Scholar
  99. Kusayama, T., Bischoff, H.-J., and Watanabe, S., 2000, Responses to mirrorimage stimulation in jungle crows (Corvus macrorhynchos), Anim. Cog. 3: 61–64Google Scholar
  100. Lefebvre, L., Nicolakakis, N., and Boire, D., 2002, Tools and brains in birds. Behaviour 139: 939–973Google Scholar
  101. Lefebvre, L., Whittle, P., Lascaris, E., and Finkelstein, A., 1997b, Feeding innovations and forebrain size in birds, Anim. Behav. 53: 549–560Google Scholar
  102. Macintosh, N. J., 1988, Approaches to the study of animal intelligence, Br. J. Psychol. 79: 509–525Google Scholar
  103. Madge, S., and Burn, H., 1999, Crows and Jays: A Guide to the Crows, Jays and Magpies of the World, Houghton Mifflin Co., Boston.Google Scholar
  104. Marler, P., 1997, Social cognition: Are primates smarter than birds? in: Current Ornithology, Volume 13, V. Nolan and E. D. Ketterson, eds., Plenum Press, New York, pp. 1–32.Google Scholar
  105. McKinley, J., and Sambrook, T. D., 2000, Use of human-given cues by domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) and horses (Equus cab alius), Anim. Cog. 3: 13–22Google Scholar
  106. Menzel, C. R., 1999, Unprompted recall and reporting of hidden objects by a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) after extended delays, J. Comp. Psychol. 113: 1–9Google Scholar
  107. Millikan, G. C, and Bowman, R I., 1967, Observations on Galapogos tool-using finches in captivity, Living Bird 6: 23–41Google Scholar
  108. Moore, B. R., 1992, Avian movement imitation and a new form of mimicry: Tracing the evolution of a complex form of learning, Behaviour 122: 231–263Google Scholar
  109. Nicolakakis, N., and Lefebvre, L., 2000, Forebrain size and innovation rate in European birds: Feeding, nesting and confounding variables, Behaviour 137: 1415–1429Google Scholar
  110. Nikei, Y., 1995, Variations of behavior of carrion crows (Corvus cor one) using automobiles as nutcrackers, Jap. J. Ornithol. 44: 21–35Google Scholar
  111. Panksepp, J., 1998, Affective Neuroscience, Oxford University Press, New York.Google Scholar
  112. Parker, S. T., Mitchell, R W., and Boccia, M. L., eds., 1994, Self-Awareness in Animals and Humans, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar
  113. Passingham, R E., 1982, The Human Primate, WH Freeman, Oxford.Google Scholar
  114. Pepperberg, I. M., 1990, Some cognitive capacities of an African Grey Parrot (Psittacus erithacus), Adv. Study Behav., 19: 357–409Google Scholar
  115. Pepperberg, I. M., 1999, The Alex Studies: Communication and Cognitive Capacities of an African Grey Parrot. Harvard University Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar
  116. Pepperberg, I. M., and McLaughlin, M. A., 1996, Effects of avian-human joint attention in allospecific vocal learning by grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus), J. Comp. Psych. 110: 286–297Google Scholar
  117. Pepperberg, I. M., Garcia, S. E., Jackson, E. C, and Marconi, S., 1995, Mirror use by African grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus), J. Comp. Psychol. 109: 182–195Google Scholar
  118. Portmann, A., 1946, Etude sur la cerebralisation chez les oiseaux I, Alauda 14:2–20. Portmann, A., 1947, Etude sur la cerebralisation chez les oiseaux II. Les indices intra-cerebraux, Alauda 15: 1–15Google Scholar
  119. Povinelli, D. J., 2000, Folk Physics for Apes, Oxford University Press, New York.Google Scholar
  120. Povinelli, D. J., and Eddy, T. J., 1996, What young chimpanzees know about seeing, Mono. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 61(3) (Serial No. 247).Google Scholar
  121. Povinelli, D. J., Nelson, K. E., and Boysen, S. T., 1990, Inferences about guessing and knowing by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), J. Comp. Psychol. 104: 203–210Google Scholar
  122. Premack, D., 1971, Language in chimpanzee? Science 172: 808–822Google Scholar
  123. Premack, D., and Premack, A. J., 1983, The Mind of an Ape, W. W. Norton & Co., New York.Google Scholar
  124. Premack, D., and Woodruff, G., 1978, Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behav. Brain Sci. 1: 515–526Google Scholar
  125. Raleigh, M. J., and Steklis, H. D., 1981, Effects of orbitofrontal and temporal neocortical lesions on the affiliative behavior of vervet monkeys (Cercopthecus aethiops sabaeus), Exp. Neurol. 73: 378–389Google Scholar
  126. Rehkamper, G., Frahm, H. D., and Zilles, K., 1991, Quantitative development of brain and brain structures in birds (Galliformes and Passeriformes) compared to that in mammals (Insectivores and Primates), Brain, Behav. Evol. 37: 125–143Google Scholar
  127. Reid, J. B., 1982, Tool-use by a rook (Corvus frugilegus) and its causation, Anim. Behav. 30: 1212–1216Google Scholar
  128. Reiss, D., and Marino, L., 2001, Mirror self-recognition in the bottlenose dolphin: A case of cognitive convergence, Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. 98: 5937–5942Google Scholar
  129. Ristau, C. A., 1991, Aspects of the cognitive ethology of an injury feigning plover, in: Cognitive Ethology: The Min As of Other Animals, C. A. Ristau, ed., Erlbaum, Hillsdale, pp. 91–126.Google Scholar
  130. Rumbaugh, D. M., ed., 1977, Language Learning by a Chimpanzee, Academic Press, New York.Google Scholar
  131. Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., and Lewin, R., 1994, Kanzi, the Ape at the Brink of the Human Mind. John Wiley & Sons, New York.Google Scholar
  132. Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., Rumbaugh, D. M., and Boysen, S. T., 1978, Sarah’s problems of comprehension. Commentary to Premack and Woodruff (1978), Behav. Brain Sci. 4: 555–557Google Scholar
  133. Schwartz, B. L., and Evans, S., 2001, Episodic memory in primates, Am. J. Primatol. 55: 71–85Google Scholar
  134. Semendeferi, K., Lu, A., Schenker, N., and Damasio, H., 2002, Humans and great apes share a large frontal cortex, Nature Neurosci. 5: 272–276Google Scholar
  135. Seyfarth, R. M., Cheney, D. L., and Marler, P., 1980, Vervet monkey alarm calls: Semantic communication in a free-ranging primate, Science 210: 801–803Google Scholar
  136. Shettleworth, S. J., 1998, Cognition, Evolution, and Behavior. Oxford University Press, New York.Google Scholar
  137. Stephan, H. D., Frahm, H., and Baron, G., 1981, New and revised data on volumes of brain structures in insectivores and primates, Folia primatol. 35: 1–29Google Scholar
  138. Stoinski, T. S., Wrate, J. L., Ure, N., and Whiten, A., 2001, Imitative learning by captive Western lowland gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) in a simulated food- processing task, J. Comp. Psychol. 115: 272–281Google Scholar
  139. Struhsaker, T. T., 1967, Auditory communication among ververt monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops), in: Social Communication among Primates, S. A. Altmann, ed., University of Chicago Press, Chicago.Google Scholar
  140. Stuss, D. T., and Alexander, M. P., 2000, Executive functions and the frontal lobes: A conceptual view, Psychol. Res. 63: 289–298Google Scholar
  141. Suarez, S. D., and Gallup, G. G., Jr., 1981, Self-recognition in chimpanzees and orangutans but not gorillas,. Comp. Psychol. 343: 35–56Google Scholar
  142. Suddendorf, T., and Corballis, M. C, 1997, Mental time travel and the evolution of the human mind, Genetic Soc. Gen. Psychol. Monographs 123: 133–167Google Scholar
  143. Tebbich, S., 2000, Tool Use in the Woodpecker Pinch Cactospiza Pallida: Ontogeny and Ecological Relevance. Unpublished Doctoral thesis, University of Vienna, Austria.Google Scholar
  144. Tebbich, S., Taborsky, M., Fessl, B., and Blomqvist, D., 2001, Do woodpecker finches acquire tool-use by social learning? Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond.: B. 268: 2189–2193Google Scholar
  145. Templeton, J. J., Kamil, A. C, and Balda, R P., 1999, Sociality and social learning in two species of corvids: the Pinyon jay (Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus) and the Clark’s nutcracker (Nucifraga columbiana), J. Comp. Psych. 113: 450–455Google Scholar
  146. Terrace, H. S., Pettito, L. A., Sanders, R J., and Bever, T. G., 1979, Can an ape create a sentence? Science 206: 891–902Google Scholar
  147. Thompson, & K. R, and Contie, C. L., 1994, Further reflections on mirror usage by pigeons: Lessons from Winnie-the-Pooh and Pinocchio too, in: Self-Awareness in Animals and Humans: Developmental Perspectives, S. T. Parker, & W. Mitchell, and M. L. Boccia, eds., Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 392–409.Google Scholar
  148. Thorpe, W. H., 1963, Learning and Instinct in Animals, Second edition, Methuen, London.Google Scholar
  149. Thouless, C. R., Fanshawe, J. H., and Bertram, C. R, 1987, Egyptian vultures Neophron percnopterus and Ostrich Struthio camelus eggs: The origins of stonethrowing behaviour, Ibis 131: 9–15Google Scholar
  150. Timmermans, S., Lefebvre, L., Boire, D., and Basu, P., 2000, Relative size of the hyperstriatum ventrale is the best predictor of feeding innovation rate in birds, Brain, Behav Evol. 56: 196–203Google Scholar
  151. Todt, D., 1975, Social learning of vocal patterns and modes of their application in grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus), Z. Tierpsychol. 39: 178–188Google Scholar
  152. Tomasello, M., and Call, J., 1997, Primate Cognition, Oxford University Press, New York.Google Scholar
  153. Tomasello, M., Call, J., and Hare, B., 1998, Five primate species follow the visual gaze of conspecifics, Anim. Behav. 55: 1063–1069Google Scholar
  154. Tschudin, A., 1999, Relative neocortex size and its correlates in dolphins: comparisons with humans and implications for mental evolution. Unpublished Doctoral thesis, University of Natal, Pietermaritzburg, South Africa.Google Scholar
  155. Tschudin, A., Call, J., Dunbar, R. I. M., Harris, G., and van der Elst, C, 2001, Comprehension of signs by dolphins (Tursiops truncates), J. Comp. Psych. 115: 100–115Google Scholar
  156. Tulving, E., 1972, Episodic and semantic memory, in: Organisation of Memory, E. Tulving and W. Donaldson, eds., New York, Academic Press, pp. 381–403.Google Scholar
  157. Tulving, E., 1983, Elements of Episodic Memory. Clarendon Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
  158. Tulving, E., 2002, Chronesthesia: Conscious awareness of subjective time, in: The Age of the Frontal Lobes, D. T. Stuss and R C. Knight, eds.Google Scholar
  159. van Lawick-Goodall, J., 1968, The behaviour of free-living chimpanzees in the Gombe Stream reserve, Anim. Behav. Mono. 1: 161–311Google Scholar
  160. van Lawick-Goodall, J., and van Lawick, H., 1966, Use of tools by the Egyptian vulture, Neophron percnopterus, Nature 212: 1468–1469Google Scholar
  161. Voronov, L. N., Bogoslovskaya, L. G., and Markova, E. G., 1994, A comparative study of the morphology of forebrain in corvidae in view of their trophic specialization, Zoo. Z. 73: 82–96Google Scholar
  162. Weir, A. A. S., Chappell, J., and Kacelnik, A., 2002, Shaping of hooks in New Caledonian crows, Science 297: 981Google Scholar
  163. Wheeler, M. A., 2000, Episodic memory and autonoetic awareness, in: The Oxford Handbook of Memory, E. Tulving and F. I. M. Craik, eds., Oxford, Oxford University Press, pp.597–625.Google Scholar
  164. Whiten, A., 1998, Imitation of the sequential structure of actions by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), J. Comp. Psychol. 112: 270–281Google Scholar
  165. Whiten, A., and Byrne, R W., 1988, Tactical deception in primates, Behav. Brain Sci. 11: 233–244Google Scholar
  166. Whiten, A., Custance, D. Al, Gomez, J. C, Texidor, P., and Bard, K. A., 1996, Imitative learning of artificial fruit processing in children (Homo sapiens) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), J. Comp. Psychol. 110: 3–14Google Scholar
  167. Whiten, A., Goodall, J., McGrew, W. C, Nishida, T., Reynolds, V., Sugiyama, Y., Tutin, C. E. G., Wrangham, R. W., and Boesch, C, 1999, Cultures in chimpanzees, Nature 399: 682–685Google Scholar
  168. Whiten, A., and Ham, R, 1992, On the nature and evolution of imitation in the animal kingdom: Reappraisal of a century of research, in: Advances in the Study of Behavior, Volume 21, P. J. B. Slater, J. S. Rosenblatt, C. Beer, and M. Milinski, eds., Academic Press, Inc., New York, pp. 239–283.Google Scholar
  169. Woolfenden, G. E., and Fitzpatrick, J. W., 1984, The Florida Scrub Jay: Demography of a Cooperative-breeding Bird, Princeton University Press, Princeton.Google Scholar
  170. Yoerg, S. I., 1991, Ecological frames of mind: The role of cognition in behavioral ecology, Q. Rev. Biol. 66: 287–301Google Scholar
  171. Zilles, K., and Rehkamper, G., 1988, The brain, with special reference to the telen- cephalon, in: Orang-utan Biology, J. H. Schwartz, ed., Oxford University Press, New York, pp. 157–176Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2004

Authors and Affiliations

  • Nathan J. Emery
    • 1
  • Nicola S. Clayton
    • 2
  1. 1.Sub-Department of Animal BehaviourUniversity of CambridgeCambridgeUK
  2. 2.Department of Experimental PsychologyUniversity of CambridgeCambridgeUK

Personalised recommendations