Using Evolved Analogies to Overcome Creative Design Fixation

  • Steven M. Smith
  • Julie S. Linsey
  • Andruid Kerne

Abstract

Human cognition is critically important in all creative conceptual design. People are susceptible to design fixation, blocks or impasses caused by a variety of unconscious cognitive processes. Insight that resolves fixation can be triggered by accidentally encountered cues, but designers cannot know in advance which environmental triggers are most appropriate. Two domains, patents and life forms, encompass countless well-tested mechanisms for solving environmental problems. A patent database and a compendium of life forms could provide rich sources of analogies that might trigger insight, thereby overcoming design fixation.

Keywords

creativity cognition fixation incubation insight analogy biological 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Choi H, Smith SM, (2005) Incubation and the resolution of Tip-of-the-tongue states. The Journal of General Psychology 132(4):365–376CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Chrysikou EG, Weisberg RW, (2005) Following the wrong footsteps: fixation effects of pictorial examples in a design problem-solving task. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Learning, Memory and Cognition 31(5):1134–1148CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Diehl M, Stroebe W, (1987) Productivity loss in brainstorming groups: Toward the solution of a riddle. Journal of Personality Social Psychology 53:497–509CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Diehl M, Stroebe W, (1991) Productivity loss in ideagenerating groups: Tracking down the blocking effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 61:392–403CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Finke RA, Ward TB, Smith SM, (1992) Creative Cognition: Theory, Research, and Applications. Cambridge, MA: MIT PressGoogle Scholar
  6. Jansson D, Smith S, (1991) Design fixation. Design Studies 12(1):3–11CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Kinoshita S, Towgood K, (2001) Effects of dividing attention on the memory-block effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 27:889-895CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Kohn NW, Smith SM, (2009) Partly versus completely out of your mind: effects of incubation and distraction on resolving fixation. Journal of Creative Behavior 43(2):102–118Google Scholar
  9. Kohn NW, Smith SM, (2010) Collaborative fixation: Effects of others’ ideas on brainstorming. Applied Cognitive PsychologyGoogle Scholar
  10. Landau JD, Lehr DP, (2004) Conformity to experimenterprovided examples: Will people use an unusual feature? Journal of Creative Behavior 38(3):180–191Google Scholar
  11. Landau JD, Leynes PA, (2006) Do explicit memory manipulations affect the memory blocking effect? American Journal of Psychology 119:463–479CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Leynes PA, Rass O, Landau JD, (2008) Eliminating the memory blocking effect. Memory 16(8):852–872CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Linsey J, Tseng I, Fu K, Cagan J, Wood K, (2009) Reducing and perceiving design fixation: Initial results from a NSF-sponsored workshop. International Conference on Engineering Design, Stanford, CaliforniaGoogle Scholar
  14. Linsey J, Wood K, Markman A, (2008) Increasing Innovation: Presentation and Evaluation of the WordTree Design-by-Analogy Method. ASME IDETC Design Theory Methodology Conference, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  15. Osborn A, (1957) Applied imagination. New York: ScribnerGoogle Scholar
  16. Parnes SJ, Meadow A, (1959) Effects of ‘‘brainstorming’’ instructions on creative problem solving by trained and untrained subjects. Journal of Educational Psychology 50:171–176CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Purcell AT, Gero JS, (1996) Design and other types of fixation. Design Studies 17(4):363–383CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Seifert CM, Meyer DE, Davidson N, Patalano AL, Yaniv I (2005) Demystification of cognitive insight: Opportunistic assimilation and the prepared-mind perspective. In Sternberg RJ, Davidson JE, (Eds.), The nature of insight, 65–124, Cambridge, MA: MIT PressGoogle Scholar
  19. Smith SM, (1994) Getting Into and Out of Mental Ruts: A theory of Fixation, Incubation, and Insight. In Sternberg RJ, Davidson JE, (Eds.), The Nature of Insight, Cambridge, MA: MIT PressGoogle Scholar
  20. Smith SM, (1995) Fixation, incubation, and insight in memory and creative thinking. In The Creative Cognitive Approach. Smith SM, Ward TB, Finke RA, (Eds.), 135–156, Cambridge, MA: The MIT PressGoogle Scholar
  21. Smith SM, Blankenship SE, (1989) Incubation effects. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 27(4):311–314Google Scholar
  22. Smith SM, Blankenship SE, (1991) Incubation and the persistence of fixation in problem solving. American Journal of Psychology 104:61–87CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Smith SM, Kohn NW, Shah JJ, (in press) What you see is what you get: Effects of provocative stimuli in creative invention. In Studying design creativity, Gero JS, (Ed.) Springer PressGoogle Scholar
  24. Smith SM, Tindell DR, (1997) Memory blocks in word fragment completion caused by involuntary retrieval of orthographically similar primes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition 23(2):355–370CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Smith SM, Vela E, (1991) Incubated reminiscence effects. Memory Cognition 19(2):168–176Google Scholar
  26. Smith SM, Ward TB, Finke RA, (1995) The Creative Cognition Approach. Cambridge, MA: MIT PressGoogle Scholar
  27. Smith SM, Ward TB, Schumacher JS, (1993) Constraining effects of examples in a creative generation task. Memory Cognition 21:837–845Google Scholar
  28. Vul E, Pashler H, (2007) Incubation benefits only after people have been misdirected. Memory Cognition 35(4):701–710Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag London Limited 2011

Authors and Affiliations

  • Steven M. Smith
    • 1
  • Julie S. Linsey
    • 1
  • Andruid Kerne
    • 1
  1. 1.Texas A&M UniversityCollege StationUSA

Personalised recommendations