The nature of culture

  • Juan D. Delius

Abstract

Niko Tinbergen laid great stress on the essential importance of cultural evolution for the understanding of human behaviour although he never, of course, made it a central subject of his research interests. In a short cautionary note about the future of humanity he wrote for example that ‘our unique position in the modern world is due to the consequences of our cultural evolution, which … has … progressively … superimposed (itself) on our still ongoing genetic evolution’ and that ‘we transfer …, from one generation to the next, not only our genetic heritage but also (our) accumulated non-genetically acquired … experience’ (Tinbergen, 1977, see also Tinbergen, 1976). Niko’s insights into the details of the processes of cultural evolution went much further than his writings reflect, however. A casual but memorable conversation between him and Konrad Lorenz in Stuttgart, Germany in 1959, at which I happened to be present, revealed that clearly. The role of song behaviour as a species-isolating mechanism in some sympatric birds had somehow cropped up. They were considering the selective forces that might have shaped the divergence of song patterns in such situations when Niko raised the important question: Selection of what? Surely not genes since the song of these birds was likely to be learned, not innate.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Alderks, C.E. (1986) Observational learning in the pigeon: effects of model’s rate of response and percentage of reinforcement. Animal LearningBehaviour, 14, 331–35.Google Scholar
  2. Anderson, R.M. and May, R.M. (eds) (1982) Population biology of infectious diseases, Springer, Berlin.Google Scholar
  3. Baker, A.J. and Jenkins, P.F. (1987) Founder effect and cultural evolution of songs in an isolated population of chaffinches, Fringilla coelebs, in the Chatham Islands. Animal Behaviour, 35, 1793–1803.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bajema, C.J. (1972) Transmission of information about the environment in the human species: a cybernetic view of genetic and cultural evolution. Social Biology, 19, 224–26.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Ball, I.A. (1984) Memes as replicators. Ethology and Sociobiology, 5, 145–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Barker, M.C. and Cunningham, M.A. (1985) The biology of bird song dialects. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 8, 85–133.Google Scholar
  7. Baudoin, M. (1975) Host castration as a parasitic strategy. Evolution, 29, 335–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Biederman, G.B., Robertson, H.A. and Vaughan, M. (1986) Observational learning of two visual discriminations by pigeons: a within-subject design. Journal for the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 46, 45–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Bonner, J.T. (1980) The evolution of culture in animals, Princeton University Press, Princeton.Google Scholar
  10. Borgia, G. (1986) Sexual selection in bowerbirds. Scientific American, 256, 70–79.Google Scholar
  11. Boyd, R. and Richerson, P.J.V. (1985) Culture and the evolutionary process, Chicago University Press, Chicago.Google Scholar
  12. Campbell, D.T. (1969) Variation and selective retention in sociocultural evolution. General Systematics Yearbook, 14, 69–85.Google Scholar
  13. Cann, R.L., Stoneking, M. and Wilson, A.C. (1987) Mitochondrial DNA and human evolution. Nature, 325, 31–36.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Catania, C. (1985) Rule governed behaviour and the origins of language, in Behaviour analysis and contemporary psychology, (eds C.F. Lowe, M. Richelle, D.E. Blackman and C.M. Bradshaw), Erlbaum, London, pp. 135–156.Google Scholar
  15. Catchpole, C.K. (1986). The biology and evolution of bird songs. Perspectives in Biology and Medicine, 30, 47–62.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Catchpole, C.K. (1987) Bird song, sexual selection and female choice. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 2, 94–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Cavalli-Sforza, L.L. and Feldman, M.W. (1981) Cultural transmission and evolution, a quantitative approach, Princeton University Press, Princeton.Google Scholar
  18. Cavalli-Sforza, L.L., Feldman, M.W., Chen, K.H. and Dornbusch, S.M. (1982) Theory and observation in cultural transmission. Science 218, 19–27.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Chesler, P. (1969) Maternal influence in learning by observation in kittens. Science 166, 901–903.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Changeaux, J.-P., Heideman, T. and Patte, P. 1984. Learning by selection, in The biology of learning (eds P. Marler and H.S. Terrace), Springer, Berlin, pp. 115–133.Google Scholar
  21. Christie, A.B. (1981) Rabies, Journal of Infection, 3, 201–218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Curio, E., Ernst, E. and Vieth, W. (1978) Cultural transmission of enemy recognition: one function of mobbing. Science, 202, 899–901.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Davies, B.D., Dulbecco, R., Eisen, H.N. and Ginsberg, H.S. (eds) (1980) Microbiology, Harper and Row, Philadelphia.Google Scholar
  24. Dawkins, R. (1976) The selfish gene. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  25. Delius, J.D. (1989) Of mind memes and brain bugs, a natural history of culture, in The nature of culture, (ed W.A. Koch), Brockmeyer, Bochum, pp. 26–79Google Scholar
  26. Delius, J.D. (1990) Sapient sauropsids and hollering hominids, in Geneses of language, (ed W. Koch). Brockmeyer, Bochum, pp. 1–29.Google Scholar
  27. Devoogd, T.J., Nixdorf, B. and Nottebohm, F. (1985) Synaptogenesis and changes in synaptic morphology related to acquisition of a new behaviour. Brain Research, 329, 304–308.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Dudai, Y. (1989) The neurobiology of memory: concepts, findings, trends. Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
  29. Falk, J.L. and Feingold, D.A. (1987) Environmental and cultural factors in the behavioral action of drugs, in Psychopharmacology: the third generation of progress, (ed H.Y. Meltzer), Raven Press, New York, pp. 1503–1509.Google Scholar
  30. Fenner, F. and Ratcliffe, F.N. (1965) Myxomatosis, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar
  31. Futuyma, D.J. (1986) Evolutionary biology, 2nd edn, Sinauer, Sunderland, Mass.Google Scholar
  32. Futuyma, D.J. and Slatkin, M. (1983) Coevolution, Sinauer, Sunderland, Mass.Google Scholar
  33. Ewer, R.F. (1969) The ‘instinct to teach’. Nature, 222, 698.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Gould, J.L. and Marler, P.(1987) Learning by instinct. Scientific American, 256, 62–73.Google Scholar
  35. Hamilton, W.D. (1964) The evolution of social behavior. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7, 1–52.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Harris, M. (1987) Cultural anthropology, Harper and Row, New York.Google Scholar
  37. Hewlett, B.S. and Cavalli-Sforza, L.L. (1986) Cultural transmission among Aka pygmies. American Anthropologist, 88, 922–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Hogan, D.E. (1986) Observational learning of a conditional hue discrimination in pigeons. Learning and Motivation, 17, 40–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Hohorst, W. (1981) Parasitologie, in Biologie, (eds D. Starck, K. Fiedler, P. Harth and J. Richter), Verlag Chemie, Weinheim, S. 765–808.Google Scholar
  40. Horn, G. (1986) Imprinting, learning, and memory. Behavioral neuroscience, 100, 825–32.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Jackson, J.B.C., Buss, L.W. and Cook, R.E. (eds) (1986) Population biology and evolution of clonal organisms, Yale University Press, New Haven.Google Scholar
  42. Klopfer, P.H. and Hailman, J.P. (1965) Habitat selection in birds. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 1, 279–303.Google Scholar
  43. Kohonen, T. (1984) Self organization and associative memory, Springer, Berlin.Google Scholar
  44. Kroodsma, D.E. and Miller, E.H. (eds) (1982) Acoustic communication in birds, Academic Press, New York.Google Scholar
  45. Lefebvre, L. (1986) Cultural diffusion of a novel food-finding behaviour in urban pigeons: an experimental field test. Ethology, 71, 295–304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Lumsden, C.J. and Wilson, E.O. (1981) Genes, mind and culture, the coevolutionary process, Havard University Press, Cambridge, Mass.Google Scholar
  47. Margulis, L. (1981) Symbiosis in cell evolution, Freeman, San Fransisco.Google Scholar
  48. Medawar, P.B. (1976) Does ethology throw any light on human behaviour? in Growing points in ethology, (eds P.P.G. Bateson and R.A. Hinde), Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 497–506.Google Scholar
  49. Meire, P.M. and Ervynick, A. (1986) Are oystercatchers (Haemotopus ostralegus) selecting the most profitable mussels (Mytilus edulis)? Animal Behavior, 34, 1427–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Minkhoff, E.C. (1983) Evolutionary biology, Addison Wesley, Reading, Mass.Google Scholar
  51. Moore, J. (1984) Parasites that change the behaviour of their host. Scientific American, 250, 82–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Morris, R.G.M., Kandel, E.R. and Squire, L.R. (1988) Learning and memory. Trends in Neuroscience, 11, 125–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Murton, R.K., Coombs, C.F.B. and Thearle, R.J.P. (1972) Ecological studies of the feral pigeon. Flock behaviour and social organization. Journal of Applied Ecology, 9, 875–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Nishida, T. (1987) Local traditions and cultural transmission, in Primate societies (eds B.B. Smuts, D.L. Cheney, R.M. Seyfarth, R.W. Wrangham, and T.T. Struhsaker), Chicago University Press, Chicago, pp. 462–474.Google Scholar
  55. Norton-Griffiths, M. (1967) Some ecological aspects of feeding behaviour of the oystercatcher Haematopus ostralegus on the edible mussel Mytilus edulis. D.L. Cheney, R.M. Seyfarth, R.W. Wrangham, and T.T. Struhsaker), Chicago University Press, Chicago, Ibis, 109, 412–24.Google Scholar
  56. Palm, G. (1982) Associative memory. Springer, Berlin.Google Scholar
  57. Pringle, J.W.S. (1951) On the parallel between learning and evolution. Behaviour, 3, 174–215.Google Scholar
  58. Rummelhardt D.E. and McClelland, J.L. (1986) Parallel distributed processing. Exploration in the microstructure of cognition. MIT Press, Cambridge, Mass.Google Scholar
  59. Schmidt-Koenig, K. (1965) Current problems in bird orientation. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 1, 217–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Smith, D.C. and Douglas, A.E. (1987) The biology of symbiosis, Arnold, London.Google Scholar
  61. Staddon, J.E.R. (1983) Adaptive behaviour and learning, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar
  62. Subowski, M.D. (1989) Recognition in ethology. Perspectives in Ethology, 8, 137–71.Google Scholar
  63. Thielke, G. (1973) Die Wirkung erlernter Signale auf die Artbildung, Universitätsverlag, Konstanz.Google Scholar
  64. Tinbergen, N. (1953) Social behaviour in animals, Methuen, London.Google Scholar
  65. Tinbergen, N. (1976) Ethology in a changing world, in Growing Points in Ethology, (eds P.P.G. Bateson and R.A. Hinde), Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 507–27.Google Scholar
  66. Tinbergen, N. (1977) Time is running out, unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
  67. Trivers, R. (1985) Social evolution, Cummings, Menlo Park, CA.Google Scholar
  68. Wallhäusser, E. and Scheich, H. (1987) Auditory imprinting leads to differential 2-deoxyglucose uptake and dendritic spine loss in the chick rostral forebrain. Developmental Brain Research, 31, 29–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Yamashita, M., Krystal, M., Fitch, W.M. and Palese P. (1988) Influenza B virus evolution: co-circulating lineages and comparison of evolutionary patter with those of influenza A and C viruses. Virology, 163, 112–22.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Zentall, T.R. and Galef, B.G. Jr (eds) (1988) Social learning: psychological and biological perspectives. Erlbaum, Hillsdale.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Chapman & Hall 1991

Authors and Affiliations

  • Juan D. Delius
    • 1
  1. 1.Universität KonstanzKonstanzGermany

Personalised recommendations