Advertisement

From Visual Information to Cognitive Maps

  • M. Jeanne Sholl
Part of the GeoJournal Library book series (GEJL, volume 32)

Abstract

It has been suggested that much of animal navigation takes place without reference to visual information in the environment (Gallistel 1990). A geocentric dead reckoning process, which tracks the travel trajectory of animals internally with only periodic reference to external visual cues, is thought to be a major component underlying animal navigation. Following Gibson (1979),and in contrast to the non-visual emphesis for animal navigation, two types of structure in visual flow are thought to be important for human navigation: perspective structure, which specifies self-motion and self-to-object relations, and invariant structure, which specifies object-to-object relations. A review of the cognitive mapping literature for sighted and blind human adults suggests that the invariant structure in visual flow is important to the formation of cognitive maps, or survey knowledge, of environments. In contrast, a geocentric dead reckoning process which uses the self-movement information provided by perspective structure may be importantly involved in the formation of route knowledge. Findings reviewed from studies of the development of navigational skills in children are consistent with the idea that cognitive mapping skills develop in concert with the ability to perceptually differentiate perspective and invariant structure in visual flow.

Keywords

Optical Flow Target Event Invariant Structure Dead Reckoning Instrumental Conditioning 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Acredolo L.P. (1987). Early development of spatial orientation in humans. In Cognitive processes and spatial orientation in animal and man (P. Ellen and C. Thinus-Blanc, eds.), pp. 185–201. Dordrecht: Marinus Nijhoff.Google Scholar
  2. Acredolo, L. P. (1978). Development of spatial orientation in infancy, Developmental Psychology, 14, 224–234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Acredolo, L. P. (1977). Developmental changes in the ability to coordinate perspectives of a large-scale space, Developmental Psychology, 13, 1–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Acredolo L.P. (1976). Frames of reference used by children for orientation in unfamiliar spaces. In Environmental knowing (G. Moore and R. Golledge, eds.), pp. 165–172. Stroudsburg, PA: Dowden, Hutchinson & Ross.Google Scholar
  5. Acredolo L.P. & Evans D. (1980). Developmental changes in the effects of landmarks on infant spatial behavior, ∖i Developmental Psychology, 16, 312–318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Acredolo L.P., Pick H. L. & Olsen M.G. (1975). Environmental differentiation and familiarity as determinants of children’s memory for spatial location. Developmental Psychology, 11, 495–501.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bai D.L. & Bertenthal B.I. (1992). Locomotor status and the development of spatial search skills. Child Development, 62, 215–226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bertenthal B.I. & Bai D.L. (1989). Infant’s sensitivity to optical flow for controlling posture. Developmental Psychology, 25, 936–945.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Byrne R.W. & Salter E. (1983). Distances and directions in the cognitive maps of the blind. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 37, 293–299.Google Scholar
  10. Dichgans J. & Brandt Th. (1974). The psychophysics of visually induced perception of self-motion and tilt, In J. Dichgans & Th. Brandt, The Neurosciences: Third Study Program, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  11. Easton R.D. & Sholl M.J. Frames of reference and accessibility of spatial knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, In press.Google Scholar
  12. Gallistel C.R. (1990). The organization of learning, Cambridge MA: MIT press.Google Scholar
  13. Gibson J.J. (1979). The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception, Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
  14. Gould J.L.& Able K.P. (1981). Human homing: An elusive phenomena. Science, 212, 1061–1063.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Hazen N.L., Lockman J.J. & Pick H.L. (1978). The development of children’s representations of large-scale environments. Child Development, 49, 623–636.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Herman J.F., Chatman, S.P. and Roth, S. F. (1983). Cognitive mapping in blind people: Acquisition of spatial relationships in a large-scale environment, Journal of Visual Impairment & Blindness, 77, 161–166.Google Scholar
  17. Juurmaa J. (1973). Transposition in mental spatial manipulation: A theoretical analysis, AFB Research Bulletin, 26, 87–134.Google Scholar
  18. Juurmaa J. (1965). An analysis of the components of orientation ability and mental manipulation of spatial relationships, Helinski: Reports from the Institute of Occupational Health (No. 28).Google Scholar
  19. Keating M.B., McKenzie, B.E. and Day, R.H. (1986). Spatial localization in infancy: Position constancy in a square and circular room with and without a landmark, Child Development, 57, 115–124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Klatzky R.L., Loomis J.M., Golledge R.G., Cicinelli J.G., Doherty S. & Pellegrino J.W. (1990). Acquisition of route and survey knowledge in the absence of vision, Journal of Motor Behavior, 22, 19–43.Google Scholar
  21. Landau B., Gleitman H. & Spelke E. (1981). Spatial knowledge and geometric representation in a child blind from birth, Science, 213, 1275–1278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Lee D.N. (1980). The optical flow field, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society (B), 290, 169–178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Lee D.N. (1974). Visual information during locomotion, In Perception: Essays in honor of James J. Gibson (R. B. MacLeod & H. L. Pick, eds.), pp. 250–269. Ithica: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
  24. Lepecq J.C. & Lafaite M. (1984). The early development of position constancy in a no-landmark environment, British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 7, 289–306.Google Scholar
  25. Levine M., Jankovic I.N. & Palij M. (1982). Principles of spatial problem solving, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 111, 157–175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Lindberg E. & Garling T. (1983). Acquisition of different types of locational information in cognitive maps: Automatic or effortful processing? Psychological Research, 45, 19–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Lishman J. R. & Lee D.N. (1973). The autonomy of visual kinesthesis, Perception, 2, 287–294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Loomis J.M., Klatzky R.L., Golledge R.G., Cicinelli J.G., Pellegrino J.W. & Pry P.A. (1993). Nonvisual navigation by blind and sighted: Assessment of path integration ability, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 73–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. McKenzie B.E., Day R.H., Colussa, S. & Connell S. (1988). Spatial localization by infants after rotational and translational shifts, Australian Journal of Psychology, 40, 165–178.Google Scholar
  30. McKenzie B.E., Day R.H. & Ihsen E. (1984). Localization of events in space: Young infants are not always egocentric, British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 2, 1–9.Google Scholar
  31. McNaughton B.L., Chen L.L. & Markus E.J. (1991). “Dead reckoning,” landmark learning, and sense of direction: A neurophysiological and computational hypothesis, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 3, 190–202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Millar W.S. & Shaffer H.R. (1972). The influence of spatially displaced feedback on infant operant conditioning, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 14, 442–452.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Mishkin M., Ungerleider L.G. & Macko K.A. (1983). Object vision and spatial vision: two cortical visual pathways, Trends in Neuroscience, 6, 414–417.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Miura T. (1990). Active function of eye movement and useful field of view in a realistic setting. In From eye to mind: Information acquisition in perception, search, and reading/ (R. Groner, G. d’Ydewaile, and R. Parham, eds.), pp., North-Holland: Elsevier Science.Google Scholar
  35. O’Keefe J. & Nadel L. (1978). The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map, Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
  36. Passini R. & Proulx G. (1988). Wayfinding without vision an experiment with congenitally totally blind people. Environment and Behavior 20, 227–252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Passini R., Proulx G. & Rainville C. (1990). The spatio-cognitive abilities of the visually impaired population, Environment and Behavior, 22, 91–118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Pick H.L. (1993). Organization of spatial knowledge in children. In Spatial Representation (N. Eilan, R. McCarthy, B. Brewer, eds.), pp. 31–42. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
  39. Poucet B. (1993). Spatial cognitive maps in animals: New hypotheses on their structure and neural mechanisms, Psychological Review, 100, 163–182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Rieser J.J. (1990). Development of perceptual-motor control while walking without vision: The calibration of perception and action. In Sensory-Motor Organizations and Development in Infancy and Early Childhood (H. Bloch & B. I. Bertenthal, eds.), pp. 379–408. Netherlands: Kluwer Academic.Google Scholar
  41. Rieser J.J. & Heiman M.L. (1982). Spatial self-reference systems and shortest route behavior in toddlers, Child Development, 53, 524–533.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Rieser J.J., Guth D.A. & Hill E. W. (1986). Sensitivity to perspective structure while walking without vision, Perception, 15, 173–188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Rieser J.J., Lockman J.J. & Pick H. L. (1980). The role of visual experience in knowledge of spatial layout, Perception & Psychophysics, 28, 185–190.Google Scholar
  44. Rieser J.J., Pick H.L., Ashmead D.H. & Garling A. E. (in press). The calibration of human locomotion and models of perceptual-motor organization, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance.Google Scholar
  45. Rieser J.J., Hill E.W., Taylor C.R., Bradfield A. & Rosen S. (1992). Visual experience, visual field size, and the development of nonvisual sensitivity to the spatial structure of outdoor neighborhoods explored by walking, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121, 210–221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Rolls E.T. (1991). Functions of primate hippocampus in spatial processing and memory, In Brain and Space (J. Paillard, ed.), pp. 334–352. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  47. Schmuckler M.A. & Gibson E.J. (1989). The effect of imposed optical flow on guided locomotion in young walkers, British Journal of Developmental Psychology 7, 193–206.Google Scholar
  48. Sholl M.J. (1987). Cognitive maps as orienting schemata, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, {∖i 13}, 615–628.Google Scholar
  49. Sholl M.J. (1993, November). The effect of restricting vision to the central field on spatial knowledge acquisition, Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Washington, DC.Google Scholar
  50. Smyth M.M. & Kennedy J.E. (1982). Orientation and spatial representation within multiple frames of reference, British Journal of Psychology, 73, 527–535.Google Scholar
  51. Stoffregen T.A. (1985). Flow structure versus retinal location in the optical control of stance, ∖i Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 11, 554–665.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Stoffregen T.A., Schmuckler M.A. & Gibson E.J. (1987). Use of central and peripheral optical flow in stance and locomotion in young walkers, Perception 16, 113–119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Telford L. & Frost B.J. (1993). Factors affecting the onset and magnitude of linear vection, Perception and Psychophysics, 53, 682–692.Google Scholar
  54. Thinus-Blanc C, Save E., Buhot M.C., Poucet B. (1991). The hippocampus, exploratory activity, and spatial memory, ∖i Brain and Space (J. Paillard, ed.), pp. 334–352. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  55. Thorndyke P.W. & Hayes-Roth B. (1982). Difference in spatial knowledge acquired from maps and navigation, Cognitive Psychology 14, 560–589.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Trevarthen C.B. (1968). Two mechanisms of vision in primates, Psychologische Forschung {∖i 31}, 299–337.Google Scholar
  57. Tyler D. & McKenzie B.E. (1990). Spatial updating and training effects in the first year of life, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 50, 445–461.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Ungerleider L.G. & Mishkin M. (1982). Two cortical visual systems. In D.J. Ingle, M.A. Goodale & R.J.W. Mansfield, Analysis of Visual Behavior, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  59. Veraart C. & Wanet-Defalque M.C. (1987). Representation of locomotor space by the blind, Perception & Psychophysics, 42, 132–139.Google Scholar
  60. Warren W.H., Morris M.W. & Kalish M. (1988). Perception of translational heading from optical flow, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14, 646–660.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Williams L.J. (1982). Cognitive load and the functional visual field, Human Factors, 24, 683–692.Google Scholar
  62. Worchel P. (1951). Space perception and orientation in the blind, Psychological Monographs, 65, 1–28.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Kluwer Academic Publishers 1996

Authors and Affiliations

  • M. Jeanne Sholl
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyBoston CollegeChestnut Hill

Personalised recommendations