Advertisement

The Relationship Between Spatial Attention and Eye Movements

  • Amelia R. HuntEmail author
  • Josephine Reuther
  • Matthew D. Hilchey
  • Raymond M. Klein
Chapter
Part of the Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences book series (CTBN, volume 41)

Abstract

The nature of the relationship between spatial attention and eye movements has been the subject of intense debate for more than 40 years. Two ideas have dominated this debate. First is the idea that spatial attention shares common neural mechanisms with eye movement programming, characterizing attention as an eye movement that has been prepared but not executed. Second, based on the observation that attention shifts to saccade targets, several theories have proposed that saccade programming necessarily recruits attentional resources. In this chapter, we review the evidence for each of these ideas and discuss some of the limitations and challenges in confirming their predictions. Although they are clearly dependent under some circumstances, dissociations between spatial attention and eye movements, and clear differences in their basic functions, point to the existence of two interconnected, but separate, systems.

Keywords

Attention Eye movements Premotor theory Spatial orienting 

References

  1. Becker W, Juergens R (1979) An analysis of the saccadic system by means of double step stimuli. Vis Res 19(9):967–983PubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Belopolsky AV, Theeuwes J (2009) When are attention and saccade preparation dissociated? Psychol Sci 20(11):1340–1347PubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Belopolsky AV, Theeuwes J (2012) Updating the premotor theory: the allocation of attention is not always accompanied by saccade preparation. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 38(4):902–914PubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Berger A, Henik A (2000) The endogenous modulation of IOR is nasal-temporal asymmetric. J Cogn Neurosci 12(3):421–428PubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Bisley JW, Goldberg ME (2010) Attention, intention, and priority in the parietal lobe. Annu Rev Neurosci 33:1–21PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  6. Boon PJ, Theeuwes J, Belopolsky AV (2017) Eye abduction reduces but does not eliminate competition in the oculomotor system. J Vis 17(5):1–10Google Scholar
  7. Born S, Mottet I, Kerzel D (2014) Presaccadic perceptual facilitation effects depend on saccade execution: evidence from the stop-signal paradigm. J Vis 14(3):1–10Google Scholar
  8. Briand KA, Klein RM (1987) Is Posner’s beam the same as Treisman’s glue?: on the relation between visual orienting and feature integration theory. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 13(2):228–241PubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Broadbent DE (1957) A mechanical model for human attention and immediate memory. Psychol Rev 64(3):205–215PubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Bundesen C (1990) A theory of visual attention. Psychol Rev 97(4):523–547PubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Cavanagh P, Hunt AR, Afraz A, Rolfs M (2010) Visual stability based on remapping of attention pointers. Trends Cogn Sci 14(4):147–153PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  12. Clarke AD, Hunt AR (2016) Failure of intuition when choosing whether to invest in a single goal or split resources between two goals. Psychol Sci 27(1):64–74PubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Clarke AD, Green P, Chantler MJ, Hunt AR (2016) Human search for a target on a textured background is consistent with a stochastic model. J Vis 16(7):1–16Google Scholar
  14. Clarke AD, Stainer MJ, Tatler BW, Hunt AR (2017a) The saccadic flow baseline: accounting for image-independent biases in fixation behavior. J Vis 17(11):1–19Google Scholar
  15. Clarke AD, Mahon A, Irvine A, Hunt AR (2017b) People are unable to recognize or report on their own eye movements. Q J Exp Psychol 70(11):2251–2270Google Scholar
  16. Corbetta M, Shulman GL (2002) Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 3:201–215Google Scholar
  17. Craighero L, Nascimben M, Fadiga L (2004) Eye position effects orienting of visuospatial attention. Curr Biol 14(4):331–333PubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Descartes, R (1633) Treatise on man (trans: Steele Hall T, 1972). Harvard University Press and Oxford University Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  19. Deubel H, Schneider WX (1996) Saccade target selection and object recognition: evidence for a common attentional mechanism. Vis Res 36(12):1827–1837Google Scholar
  20. Dorris MC, Par M, Munoz DP (1997) Neuronal activity in monkey superior colliculus related to the initiation of saccadic eye movements. J Neurosci 17(21):8566–8579PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  21. Duhamel JR, Colby CL, Goldberg ME (1992) The updating of the representation of visual space in parietal cortex by intended eye movements. Science 255(5040):90–92PubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Fehd HM, Seiffert AE (2008) Eye movements during multiple object tracking: where do participants look? Cognition 108(1):201–209PubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Findlay JM, Walker R (1999) A model of saccade generation based on parallel processing and competitive inhibition. Behav Brain Sci 22(4):661–621PubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Friesen CK, Kingstone A (1998) The eyes have it! Reflexive orienting is triggered by nonpredictive gaze. Psychon Bull Rev 5(3):490–495Google Scholar
  25. Gabay S, Henik A, Gradstein L (2010) Ocular motor ability and covert attention in patients with Duane Retraction Syndrome. Neuropsychologia 48(10):3102–3109PubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Garey LJ, Powell TPS (1968) The projection of the retina in the cat. J Anat 102(2):189–222PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  27. Godijn R, Theeuwes J (2002) Parallel programming of saccades: evidence for a competitive integration model. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 28(5):1039–1054PubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Godijn R, Theeuwes J (2004) The relationship between inhibition of return and saccade trajectory deviations. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 30(3):538–554PubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Goldberg ME, Wurtz RH (1972) Activity of superior colliculus in behaving monkeys. II. Effect of attention on neuronal responses. J Neurophysiol 35(4):560–574PubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Golomb JD, Kanwisher N (2011) Higher level visual cortex represents retinotopic, not spatiotopic, object location. Cereb Cortex 22(12):2794–2810PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  31. Golomb JD, L’Heureux ZE, Kanwisher N (2014) Feature-binding errors after eye movements and shifts of attention. Psychol Sci 25(5):1067–1078PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  32. Gregory NJ, Hermens F, Facey R, Hodgson TL (2016) The developmental trajectory of attentional orienting to socio-biological cues. Exp Brain Res 234(4):1351–1362PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  33. Hall N, Colby C (2014) S-cone visual stimuli activate superior colliculus neurons in old world monkeys: implications for understanding blindsight. J Cogn Neurosci 26(6):1234–1256PubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Hayhoe M, Ballard D (2005) Eye movements in natural behavior. Trends Cogn Sci 9(4):188–194PubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Hayhoe M, Ballard D (2014) Modeling task control of eye movements. Curr Biol 24(13):R622–R628PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  36. Higgins E, Rayner K (2015) Transaccadic processing: stability, integration, and the potential role of remapping. Atten Percept Psychophys 77(1):3–27PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  37. Hodgson TL, Müller HJ (1999) Attentional orienting in two-dimensional space. Q J Exp Psychol 52(3):615–623Google Scholar
  38. Hodgson TL, Müller HJ, O’Leary MJ (1999) Attentional localization prior to simple and directed manual responses. Percept Psychophys 61(2):308–312PubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Hoffman JE, Subramanian B (1995) The role of visual attention in saccadic eye movements. Percept Psychophys 57(6):787–795PubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Hood BM, Willen JD, Driver JD (1998) Adult’s eyes trigger shifts of visual attention in human infants. Psychol Sci 9(2):131–134Google Scholar
  41. Hunt AR, Cavanagh P (2011) Remapped visual masking. J Vis 11(1):1–8Google Scholar
  42. Hunt AR, Kingstone A (2003a) Covert and overt voluntary attention: linked or independent? Cogn Brain Res 18(1):102–105Google Scholar
  43. Hunt AR, Kingstone A (2003b) Inhibition of return: dissociating attentional and oculomotor components. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 29(5):1068–1074PubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Hunt AR, Olk B, von Mühlenen A, Kingstone A (2004) Integration of competing saccade programs. Cogn Brain Res 19(2):206–208Google Scholar
  45. Ignashchenkova A, Dicke PW, Haarmeier T, Thier P (2004) Neuron-specific contribution of the superior colliculus to overt and covert shifts of attention. Nat Neurosci 7(1):56–64PubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Jonides J (1981) Voluntary versus automatic control over the mind’s eye movements. In: Long AD (ed) Attention and performance IX. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsadale, pp 187–203Google Scholar
  47. Juan CH, Shorter-Jacobi SM, Schall JD (2004) Dissociation of spatial attention and saccade preparation. Proc Natl Acad Sci 101(43):15541–15544PubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Khan AZ, Blohm G, McPeek RM, Lefevre P (2009) Differential influence of attention on gaze and head movements. J Neurophysiol 101:198–206PubMedGoogle Scholar
  49. Kingstone A, Klein RM (1993) Visual offsets facilitate saccadic latency: does pre disengagement of attention mediate this gap effect? J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 19(6):1251–1265PubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Klein RM (1980) Does oculomotor readiness mediate cognitive control of visual attention? In: Nickerson R (ed) Attention and performance VIII. Academic Press, New York, pp 259–276Google Scholar
  51. Klein RM (2004) On the control of visual orienting. In: Posner MI (ed) Cognitive neuroscience of attention. Guilford Press, New York, pp 29–44Google Scholar
  52. Klein R (2009) On the control of attention. Can J Exp Psychol 63(3):240PubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Klein RM, Pontefract A (1994) Does oculomotor readiness mediate cognitive control of visual attention? Revisited! In: Nickerson R (ed) Attention and performance XV: conscious and nonconscious information processing. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, pp 333–350Google Scholar
  54. Kopesz K (1995) Saccadic reaction time in gap/overlap paradigm: a model based on integration of intentional and visual information on neural, dynamic fields. Vis Res 35:2911–2925Google Scholar
  55. Kowler E, Anderson E, Dosher B, Blaser E (1995) The role of attention in the programming of saccades. Vis Res 35(13):1897–1916PubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Lambert A, Norris A, Naiker N, Aitken V (2000) Effects of informative peripheral cues on eye movements: revisiting William James’ “derived attention”. Vis Cogn 7(5):545–569Google Scholar
  57. Land MF (2018) The evolution of gaze shifting eye movements. In: Hodgson T (ed) Processes of visuo-spatial attention and working memory. Current topics in behavioural neurosciences. Springer, ChamGoogle Scholar
  58. Land MF, McLeod P (2000) From eye movements to actions: how batsmen hit the ball. Nat Neurosci 3(12):1340–1345PubMedGoogle Scholar
  59. Land MF, Mennie N, Rusted J (1999) The roles of vision and eye movements in the control of activities of daily living. Perception 28(11):1311–1328PubMedGoogle Scholar
  60. Lu ZL, Dosher BA (2000) Spatial attention: different mechanisms for central and peripheral temporal precues? J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 26(5):1534–1548PubMedGoogle Scholar
  61. MacInnes WJ, Krüger HM, Hunt AR (2015) Just passing through: IOR in preplanned saccade sequences. Q J Exp Psychol 68:402–416Google Scholar
  62. MacLean GH, Klein RM, Hilchey MD (2015) Does oculomotor readiness mediate exogenous capture of visual attention? J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 41(5):1260–1270PubMedGoogle Scholar
  63. Marrocco RT, Li RH (1977) Monkey superior colliculus: properties of single cells and their afferent inputs. J Neurophysiol 40(4):844–860PubMedGoogle Scholar
  64. Matin L, Pearce DG (1965) Visual perception of direction for stimuli flashed during voluntary saccadic eye movements. Science 148(3676):1485–1488PubMedGoogle Scholar
  65. McCoy B, Theeuwes J (2018) Overt and covert attention to location-based reward. Vis Res 142:27–39PubMedGoogle Scholar
  66. Moore T, Fallah M (2001) Control of eye movements and spatial attention. Proc Natl Acad Sci 98(3):1273–1276Google Scholar
  67. Moore T, Fallah M (2004) Microstimulation of the frontal eye fields and its effect on covert spatial attention. J Neurophysiol 91(1):152–162Google Scholar
  68. Moore C, Dunham PJ, Dunham P (2014) Joint attention: its origins and role in development. Psychology Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  69. Morvan C, Maloney LT (2012) Human visual search does not maximize the post-saccadic probability of identifying targets. PLoS Comput Biol 8(2):e1002342PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  70. Müller JR, Philiastides MG, Newsome WT (2005) Microstimulation of the superior colliculus focuses attention without moving the eyes. Proc Natl Acad Sci 102(3):524–529PubMedGoogle Scholar
  71. Murthy A, Thompson KG, Schall JD (2001) Dynamic dissociation of visual selection from saccade programming in frontal eye field. J Neurophysiol 86(5):2634–2637PubMedGoogle Scholar
  72. Najemnik J, Geisler WS (2005) Optimal eye movement strategies in visual search. Nature 434(7031):387–390PubMedGoogle Scholar
  73. Nowakowska A, Clarke AD, Hunt AR (2017) Human visual search behaviour is far from ideal. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 284(1849):1–6Google Scholar
  74. Peterson MS, Kramer AF, Irwin DE (2004) Covert shifts of attention precede involuntary eye movements. Percept Psychophys 66(3):398–405Google Scholar
  75. Posner MI (1980) Orienting of attention. Q J Exp Psychol 32(1):3–25PubMedGoogle Scholar
  76. Posner MI, Cohen Y (1984) Component of visual orienting. In: Bouma H, Bonwhuis D (eds) Attention and performance X. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, pp 551–556Google Scholar
  77. Rafal RD, Calabresi PA, Brennan CW, Sciolto TK (1989) Saccade preparation inhibits reorienting to recently attended locations. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 15(4):673–685PubMedGoogle Scholar
  78. Rafal RD, Henik A, Smith J (1991) Extrageniculate contributions to reflex orienting in normal humans: a temporal hemifield advantage. J Cogn Neurosci 3(4):322–328PubMedGoogle Scholar
  79. Remington RW (1980) Attention and saccadic eye movements. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 6(4):726–744PubMedGoogle Scholar
  80. Reuter-Lorenz PA, Fendrich R (1992) Oculomotor readiness and covert orienting: differences between central and peripheral cues. Percept Psychophys 52(3):336–344PubMedGoogle Scholar
  81. Rizzolati G, Riggio L, Dascola I, Umilta C (1987) Reorienting attention across the horizontal and vertical meridians: evidence in favor of a premotor theory of attention. Neuropsychologia 25(1):31–40Google Scholar
  82. Robertson L (2003) Binding, perceptual attention and spatial awareness. Nat Rev Neurosci 4(2):93–103PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  83. Sapir A, Rafal RD, Henik A (2002) Attending to the thalamus: inhibition of return and nasal-temporal asymmetry in the pulvinar. Neuroreport 13(5):693–697PubMedGoogle Scholar
  84. Saslow MG (1967) Effects of components of displacement-step stimuli upon latency for saccadic eye movement. J Opt Soc Am 57(8):1024–1029PubMedGoogle Scholar
  85. Schiller PH (2013) Neural control of visually guided eye movements. In: Fafrowisc M, Marek T, Karwowski W, Schmorrow D (eds) Neuroadaptive systems: theory and applications. CRC Press, Boca RatonGoogle Scholar
  86. Schiller PH, True SD, Conway JL (1980) Deficits in eye movements following superior colliculus and frontal eye field ablations. J Neurophysiol 44(6):1175–1189PubMedGoogle Scholar
  87. Schiller PH, Sandell JH, Maunsell JHR (1987) The effect of frontal eye field and superior colliculus lesions on saccadic latencies in the rhesus monkey. J Neurophysiol 57(4):1033–1049PubMedGoogle Scholar
  88. Schlag J, Schlag-Rey M (1995) Illusory localization of stimuli flashed in the dark before saccades. Vis Res 35(16):2347–2357PubMedGoogle Scholar
  89. Schneider WX (1995) VAM: a neuro-cognitive model for visual attention control of segmentation, object recognition, and space-based motor action. Vis Cogn 2(2–3):331–376Google Scholar
  90. Sheliga BM, Riggio L, Rizzolatti G (1994) Orienting of attention and eye movements. Exp Brain Res 98(3):507–522PubMedGoogle Scholar
  91. Sheperd M, Findlay JM, Hockey RJ (1986) The relationship between eye movements and spatial attention. Q J Exp Psychol 38(3):475–491Google Scholar
  92. Shulman GL (1984) An asymmetry in the control of eye movements and shifts of attention. Acta Psychol 55(1):53–69Google Scholar
  93. Simion F, Valenza E, Umilta C, Barbra BD (1998) Inhibition of return in newborns is temporo-nasal asymmetrical. Infant Behav Dev 18(2):189–194Google Scholar
  94. Smith DT, Schenk T (2012) The premotor theory of attention: time to move on? Neuropsychologia 50(6):1104–1114PubMedGoogle Scholar
  95. Smith DT, Rorden C, Jackson SR (2004) Exogenous orienting of attention depends upon the ability to execute eye movements. Curr Biol 14(9):792–795PubMedGoogle Scholar
  96. Smith DT, Ball K, Ellison A, Schenk T (2010) Deficits of reflexive attention induced by abduction of the eye. Neuropsychologia 48(5):1269–1276PubMedGoogle Scholar
  97. Smith DT, Schenk T, Rorden C (2012) Saccade preparation is required for exogenous attention but not endogenous attention or IOR. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 38(6):1438–1447PubMedGoogle Scholar
  98. Smith DT, Ball K, Ellison A (2014) Covert visual search within and beyond the effective oculomotor range. Vis Res 95:11–17PubMedGoogle Scholar
  99. Sommer MA, Wurtz RH (2008) Brain circuits for the internal monitoring of movements. Annu Rev Neurosci 31:317–338PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  100. Stelmach LB, Campsall JM, Herdman CM (1997) Attentional and ocular movements. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 23(3):823–844Google Scholar
  101. Sterling D (1973) Quantitative mapping with electron microscope: retinal terminals in the superior colliculus. Brain Res 54:347–354PubMedGoogle Scholar
  102. Sumner P, Adamjee T, Mollon JD (2002) Signals invisible to the collicular and magnocellular pathways can capture visual attention. Curr Biol 12(15):1312–1316PubMedGoogle Scholar
  103. Tatler BW (2007) The central fixation bias in scene viewing: selecting an optimal viewing position independently of motor biases and image feature distributions. J Vis 7(14):1–17PubMedGoogle Scholar
  104. Tatler BW, Hayhoe MM, Land MF, Ballard DH (2011) Eye guidance in natural vision: reinterpreting salience. J Vis 11(5):1–23Google Scholar
  105. Taylor TL, Klein RM (1998) On the causes and effects of inhibition of return. Psychon Bull Rev 5(4):625–643Google Scholar
  106. Taylor T, Kingstone AF, Klein RM (1998) The disappearance of foveal and non-foveal stimuli: decomposing the gap effect. Can J Exp Psychol 52(4):192–200Google Scholar
  107. Theeuwes J, Kramer AF, Hahn S, Irwin DE (1998) Our eyes do not always go where we want them to go: capture of the eyes by new objects. Psychol Sci 9:379–385Google Scholar
  108. Thompson KG, Biscoe KL, Sato TR (2005) Neuronal basis of covert spatial attention in the frontal eye fields. J Neurosci 25(41):9479–9487PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  109. Trappenberg TP, Dorris MD, Munoz DP, Klein RM (2001) A model of saccade initiation based on competitive integration of endogenous and exogenous signals within the superior colliculus. J Cogn Neurosci 13(2):256–271PubMedGoogle Scholar
  110. Treisman A, Gelade G (1980) A feature-integration theory of attention. Cogn Psychol 12(1):97–136PubMedGoogle Scholar
  111. Umeno MM, Goldberg ME (1997) Spatial processing in the monkey frontal eye field. I. Predictive visual responses. J Neurophysiol 78(3):1373–1383PubMedGoogle Scholar
  112. Van der Stigchel S, De Vries JP (2015) There is no attentional global effect: attentional shifts are independent of the saccade endpoint. J Vis 15(15):1–12Google Scholar
  113. Võ MLH, Aizenman AM, Wolfe JM (2016) You think you know where you looked? You better look again. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 42(10):1477–1481PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  114. von Helmholtz H (1866/1962) Treatise on physiological optics, vol 2 (trans: Southall JPL). Dover, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  115. Walker MF, Fitzgibbon EJ, Goldberg ME (1995) Neurons in the monkey superior colliculus predict the visual result of impending saccadic eye movements. J Neurophysiol 73(5):1988–2003PubMedGoogle Scholar
  116. White BJ, Munoz DP (2017) Neural mechanisms of saliency, attention, and orienting. In: Computational and cognitive neuroscience of vision. Springer, Singapore, pp 1–23Google Scholar
  117. Williams C, Azzopardi P, Cowey A (1995) Nasal and temporal retinal ganglion cells projecting to the midbrain: implications for “blindsight”. Neuroscience 65(2):577–586PubMedGoogle Scholar
  118. Wischnewski M, Belardinelli A, Schneider WX, Steil JJ (2010) Where to look next? Combining static and dynamic proto-objects in a TVA-based model of visual attention. Cogn Comput 2(4):326–343Google Scholar
  119. Zackon DH, Casson EJ, Stelmach L, Faubert J, Racette L (1997) Distinguishing subcortical and cortical influences in visual attention: subcortical attentional processing. Invest Opthamol Vis Sci 38(2):364–371Google Scholar
  120. Zackon DH, Casson EJ, Zafar A, Stelmach L, Racette L (1999) The temporal order judgment paradigm: subcortical attentional contributions under exogenous and endogenous cueing conditions. Neuropsychologia 37(5):511–520PubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  • Amelia R. Hunt
    • 1
    Email author
  • Josephine Reuther
    • 1
  • Matthew D. Hilchey
    • 2
  • Raymond M. Klein
    • 3
  1. 1.University of AberdeenAberdeenUK
  2. 2.University of TorontoTorontoCanada
  3. 3.Dalhousie UniversityHalifaxCanada

Personalised recommendations