Advertisement

How significant are cultural differences in spatial cognition?

  • Daniel R. Montello
Conference paper
Part of the Lecture Notes in Computer Science book series (LNCS, volume 988)

Abstract

In this essay, I critically discuss ideas about cultural differences in spatial cognition. A critique of the traditional empiricist framework for understanding the development of cognitive structures and processes is described. An evolutionary framework is provided as an alternative. The ambiguity between culture-related and culturally caused differences is also elaborated. Reasons for cultural universality in spatial cognition are then considered. With these ideas as background, some aspects of spatial cognition are enumerated that I believe are largely culturally universal. Following that, aspects that may show more significant cultural variability are discussed. In particular, the relationship of spatial language to spatial thought is addressed in some depth. I conclude that cultural differences in spatial cognition are not nearly as substantial as is often claimed. Finally, the extent to which cultural differences in spatial cognition are important for geographic information system (GIS) research and design is considered.

Keywords

Geographic Information System Cultural Difference Cultural Group Spatial Cognition Residential Environment 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Appelle, S. (1972). Perception and discrimination as a function of stimulus orientation: The “oblique effect” in man and animals. Psychological Bulletin, 78, 266–278.Google Scholar
  2. Blaut, J. M. (1991). Natural mapping. Transactions of the Institute of British Geographers, 16, 55–74.Google Scholar
  3. Brown, P., & Levinson, S. C. (1993). Linguistic and Nonlinguistic Coding of Spatial Arrays: Explorations in Mayan Cognition. Cognitive Anthropology Research Group Working Paper 24. Nijmegen, The Netherlands: Max Planck Institute.Google Scholar
  4. Buss, D. M. (1989). Sex differences in human mate preferences: Evolutionary hypotheses tested in 37 cultures. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 12, 1–49.Google Scholar
  5. Campari, I., & Frank, A. U. (1993). Cultural differences in GIS: A basic approach. In Proceedings of Fourth European Conference and Exhibition on Geographical Information Systems (pp. 10–16).Google Scholar
  6. Cosmides, L., Tooby, J., & Barkow, J. H. (1992). Introduction: Evolutionary psychology and conceptual integration. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 3–15). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  7. Couclelis, H. (1992). People manipulate objects (but cultivate fields): Beyond the raster-vector debate in GIS. In A. U. Frank, I. Campari, & U. Formentini (Eds.), Theories and methods of spatio-temporal reasoning in geographic space (pp. 65–77). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
  8. Deregowski, J. B. (1980). Illusions, patterns, and pictures. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  9. Egenhofer, M. J., & Golledge, R. G. (November, 1994). Time in geographic space: Report on the Specialist Meeting of Research Initiative 10 (Technical Report No. 94-9). National Center for Geographic Information and Analysis.Google Scholar
  10. Fodor, J. A. (1983). The modularity of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  11. Gentile, D. A. (1993). Just what are sex and gender, anyway? A call for a new terminological standard. Psychological Science, 4, 120–122.Google Scholar
  12. Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
  13. Gladwin, T. (1970). East is a big bird. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  14. Hagen, M. A. (Ed.). (1980). The perception of pictures (2 volumes). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  15. Hallowell, A. I. (1942). Some psychological aspects of measurement among the Saulteaux. American Anthropologist, 44, 62–77.Google Scholar
  16. Haviland, J. B., & Levinson, S. C. (1994). Special Issue: Spatial conceptualization in Mayan languages. Linguistics, 32.Google Scholar
  17. Hazen, N. L. (1983). Spatial orientation: A comparative approach. In H. L. Pick & L. P. Acredolo (Eds.), Spatial orientation: Theory, research, and application (pp. 3–37). New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  18. Heider, E. R. (1972). Universals in color naming and memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 93, 10–20.Google Scholar
  19. Kay, P., & Kempton, W. (1984). What is the Sapir-Whorf Hypothesis? American Anthropologist, 86, 65–79.Google Scholar
  20. Kenrick, D. T., Montello, D. R., & MacFarlane, S. (1985). Personality: Social learning, social cognition, or sociobiology? In R. Hogan & W. H. Jones (Eds.), Perspectives in personality (pp. 201–234). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.Google Scholar
  21. Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, fire, and dangerous things. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  22. Leibowitz, H. W., & Pick, H. A. (1972). Cross-cultural and educational aspects of the Ponzo perspective illusion. Perception & Psychophysics, 12, 430–432.Google Scholar
  23. Lewis, D. (1978). The voyaging stars: Secrets of the Pacific island navigators. Sydney: William Collins Publishers.Google Scholar
  24. Lewis-Williams, J. D., & Dowson, T. A. (1988). The signs of all times: Entoptic phenomena in Upper Paleolithic art. Current Anthropology, 29, 201–245.Google Scholar
  25. Liben, L. S., & Downs, R. M. (1989). Understanding maps as symbols: The development of map concepts in children. In H. W. Reese (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (pp. 145–201). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  26. Littlejohn, J. (1963). Temne space. Anthropological Quarterly, 36, 1–17.Google Scholar
  27. Lynch, K. (1960). The image of the city. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  28. Mark, D. M. (1993). Toward a theoretical framework for geographic entity types. In A. U. Frank & I. Campari (Eds.), Spatial information theory: A theoretical basis for GIS (pp. 270–283). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
  29. Montello, D. R. (1993). Scale and multiple psychologies of space. In A. U. Frank & I. Campari (Eds.), Spatial information theory: A theoretical basis for GIS (pp. 312–321). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
  30. Montello, D. R., & Frank, A. U. (in press). Modeling directional knowledge and reasoning in environmental space: Testing qualitative metrics. In J. Portugali (Ed.), The construction of cognitive maps. Kluwer Academic.Google Scholar
  31. Munroe, R. H., Munroe, R. L., & Brasher, A. (1985). Precursors of spatial ability: A longitudinal study among the Longoli of Kenya. Journal of Social Psychology, 125, 23–33.Google Scholar
  32. Neisser, U. (1987). A sense of where you are: Functions of the spatial module. In P. Ellen & C. Thinus-Blanc (Eds.), Cognitive processes and spatial orientation in animals and man (pp. 293–310). Boston: Martinus Nijhoff.Google Scholar
  33. Pederson, E. (1993). Geographic and manipulable space in two Tamil linguistic systems. In A. U. Frank & I. Campari (Eds.), Spatial information theory: A theoretical basis for GIS (pp. 294–311). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
  34. Pederson, E. (in press). Language as context, language as means: Spatial cognition and habitual language use. Cognitive Linguistics.Google Scholar
  35. Pick, A. D., & Pick, H. L. (1978). Culture and perception. In E. C. Carterette & M. P. Friedman (Eds.), Handbook of perception (pp. 19–39). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  36. Pinker, S. (1994). The language instinct. New York: William Morrow and Company.Google Scholar
  37. Rapoport, A. (1976). Environmental cognition in cross-cultural perspective. In G. T. Moore & R. G. Golledge (Eds.), Environmental knowing (pp. 220–234). Stroudsburg, PA: Dowden, Hutchinson & Ross.Google Scholar
  38. Segall, M. H., Campbell, D. T., & Herskovits, M. J. (1966). The influence of culture on visual perception. Indianapolis: The Bobbs-Merrill Company, Inc.Google Scholar
  39. Sheets-Johnstone, M. (1990). The roots of thinking. Philadelphia: Temple University Press.Google Scholar
  40. Shepard, R. N. (1984). Ecological constraints on internal representation: Resonant kinematics of perceiving, imagining, thinking, and dreaming. Psychological Review, 91, 417–447.Google Scholar
  41. Shepard, R. N., & Hurwitz, S. (1984). Upward direction, mental rotation, and discrimination of left and right turns in maps. Cognition, 18, 161–193.Google Scholar
  42. Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1992). The psychological foundations of culture. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 19–136). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  43. Turnbull, C. M. (1961). Some observations regarding the experiences and behavior of the BaMbuti Pygmies. American Journal of Psychology, 74, 304–308.Google Scholar
  44. Wallace, R. (1989). Cognitive mapping and the origin of language and mind. Current Anthropology, 30, 518–526.Google Scholar
  45. Weissenborn, J., & Klein, W. (Eds.) (1982). Here and there: Cross-linguistic studies on deixis and demonstration. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 1995

Authors and Affiliations

  • Daniel R. Montello
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of GeographyUniversity of CaliforniaSanta BarbaraUSA

Personalised recommendations