Advertisement

A Formal Model of the Process of Wayfinding in Built Environments

  • Martin Raubal
  • Michael Worboys
Conference paper
Part of the Lecture Notes in Computer Science book series (LNCS, volume 1661)

Abstract

Previous recent research on human wayfinding has focused primarily on mental representations rather than processes of wayfinding. This paper presents a formal model of some aspects of the process of wayfinding, where appropriate elements of human perception and cognition are formally realized using image schemata and affordances. The goal-driven reasoning chain that leads to action begins with incomplete and imprecise knowledge derived from imperfect observations of space. Actions result in further observations, derived knowledge and, recursively, further actions, until the goal is achieved or the wayfinder gives up. This paper gives a formalization of this process, using a modal extension to classical propositional logic to represent incomplete knowledge. Both knowledge and action are represented through a wayfinding graph. A special case of wayfinding in a building, that is finding one’s way through an airport, is used to demonstrate the formal model.

Keywords

Wayfinding Image Schemata Affordances Spatial Reasoning Knowledge Frames Logic Graphs 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. [1]
    Chellas, B., Modal Logic. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press, 1980.zbMATHGoogle Scholar
  2. [2]
    Cohn, A., “The Challenge of Qualitative Spatial Reasoning”. ACM Computing Surveys, vol. 27, pp. 323–325, 1995.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. [3]
    Epstein, S., “Spatial Representation for Pragmatic Navigation”. In Spatial Information Theory-A Theoretical Basis for GIS, International Conference COSIT’’97, Laurel Highlands, PA, vol. 1329, Lecture Notes in Computer Science, Hirtle, S. and Frank, A., Eds. Berlin: Springer, 1997, pp. 373–388.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. [4]
    Fagin, R., Halpern, J., Moses, Y., and Vardi, M., Reasoning about Knowledge. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1996.Google Scholar
  5. [5]
    Frank, A., “Spatial Reasoning-Theoretical Considerations and Practical Applications”. EGIS’92, vol. 1. München, Deutschland, 1992.Google Scholar
  6. [6]
    Frank, A., “Qualitative spatial reasoning: cardinal directions as an example”. International Journal of Geographical Information Systems, vol. 10, pp. 269–290, 1996.Google Scholar
  7. [7]
    Freksa, C., “Using Orientation Information for Qualitative Spatial Reasoning”. In Theories and Methods of Spatio-Temporal Reasoning in Geographic Space, Lecture Notes in Computer Science 639, Frank, A., Campari, I., and Formentini, U., Eds.: Springer-Verlag, 1992, pp. 162–178.Google Scholar
  8. [8]
    Gärling, T., Bö öök, A., and Lindberg, E., “Spatial orientation and wayfinding in the designed environment: A conceptual analysis and some suggestions for postoccupancy evaluation.”. Journal of Architectural Planning Resources, vol. 3, pp. 55–64, 1986.Google Scholar
  9. [9]
    Gärling, T., Lindberg, E., and Möntylä, T., “Orientation in buildings: Effects of familiarity, visual access, and orientation aids.”. Journal of Applied Psychology, vol.68, pp. 177–186, 1983.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. [10]
    Gibson, J., The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Company, 1979.Google Scholar
  11. [11]
    Gluck, M., “Making Sense of Human Wayfinding: Review of Cognitive and Linguistic Knowledge for Personal Navigation with a New Research Direction”. In Cognitive and Linguistic Aspects of Geographic Space, vol. 63, Series D: Behavioural and Social Sciences, Mark, D. and Frank, A., Eds. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers, 1991, pp. 117–135.Google Scholar
  12. [12]
    Golledge, R., “Place Recognition and Wayfinding: Making Sense of Space”. Geoforum, vol. 23, pp. 199–214, 1992.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. [13]
    Gopal, S., Klatzky, R., and Smith, T., “NAVIGATOR: A Psychologically Based Model of Environmental Learning Through Navigation.”. Journal of Environmental Psychology, pp. 309–331, 1989.Google Scholar
  14. [14]
    Hintikka, J., J., Knowledge and Belief. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press, 1962.Google Scholar
  15. [15]
    Johnson, M., The Body in the Mind: The Bodily Basis of Meaning, Imagination, and Reason. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press, 1987.Google Scholar
  16. [16]
    Kripke, S., “A completeness theorem in modal logic”. Journal of Symbolic Logic, vol. 24, pp. 1–14, 1959.zbMATHCrossRefMathSciNetGoogle Scholar
  17. [17]
    Kuhn, W., “Handling Data Spatially: Spatializing User Interfaces”. In SDH’96, Advances in GIS Research II, Proceedings, vol. 2, Kraak, M. and Molenaar, M., Eds. Delft: International Geographical Union, 1996, pp. 13B.1–13B.23.Google Scholar
  18. [18]
    Kuipers, B., “Modeling Spatial Knowledge”. Cognitive Science, vol. 2, pp. 129–154, 1978.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. [19]
    Kuipers, B., “The’ Map in the Head’ Metaphor”. Environment and Behaviour, vol. 14, pp. 202–220, 1982.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. [20]
    Lakoff, G., “Cognitive Semantics”. In Meaning and Mental Representations, Eco, U., Santambrogio, M., and Violi, P., Eds. Bloomington: Indiana University Press, 1988, pp. 119–154.Google Scholar
  21. [21]
    Leiser, D. and Zilbershatz, A., “The Traveller-A Computational Model of Spatial Network Learning”. Environment and Behavior, vol. 21, pp. 435–463, 1989.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. [22]
    Lynch, K., The Image of the City. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press, 1960.Google Scholar
  23. [23]
    Mark, D. and Frank, A., “Experiential and Formal Models of Geographic Space”. Environment and Planning B, vol. 23, pp. 3–24, 1996.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. [24]
    McCalla, G., Reid, L., and Schneider, P., “Plan Creation, Plan Execution, and Knowledge Acquisition in a Dynamic Microworld”. International Journal of Man-Machine Studies, vol. 16, pp. 89–112, 1982.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. [25]
    McDermott, D. and Davis, E., “Planning Routes through Uncertain Territory”.Artificial Intelligence, vol. 22, pp. 107–156, 1984.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. [26]
    Norman, D., The Design of Everyday Things. New York: Doubleday, 1988.Google Scholar
  27. [27]
    O’Neill, M., “Effects of signage and floor plan configuration on wayfinding accuracy”. Environment and Behavior, vol. 23, pp. 553–574, 1991a.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. [28]
    O’Neill, M., “Evaluation of a conceptual model of architectural legibility”. Environment and Behavior, vol. 23, pp. 259–284, 1991b.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. [29]
    Pawlak, Z., “Rough Sets”. International Journal of Computer and Information Sciences, vol. 11, pp. 341–356, 1982.CrossRefzbMATHMathSciNetGoogle Scholar
  30. [30]
    Piaget, J. and Inhelder, B., The Child’s Conception of Space. New York: Norton, 1967.Google Scholar
  31. [31]
    Raubal, M., “Structuring Wayfinding Tasks with Image Schemata”. Department of Spatial Information Science and Engineering, University of Maine, U.S.A., 1997.Google Scholar
  32. [32]
    Raubal, M. and Egenhofer, M., “Comparing the complexity of wayfinding tasks in built environments”. Environment & Planning B, vol. 25, pp. 895–913, 1998.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. [33]
    Raubal, M., Egenhofer, M., Pfoser, D., and Tryfona, N., “Structuring Space with Image Schemata: Wayfinding in Airports as a Case Study”. In Spatial Information Theory-A Theoretical Basis for GIS, International Conference COSIT’97, Laurel Highlands, PA, vol. 1329, Lecture Notes in Computer Science, Hirtle, S. and Frank, A., Eds. Berlin: Springer, 1997, pp. 85–102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. [34]
    Seidel, A., “Way-Finding in Public Spaces: The Dallas/Fort Worth, USA Airport.”. 20th International Congress of Applied Psychology. Edinburgh, Scotland, 1982.Google Scholar
  35. [35]
    Siegel, A. and White, S., “The development of spatial representations of large-scale environments”. In Advances in child development and behavior, vol. 10, Reese, H., Ed. New York: Academic Press, 1975, pp. 9–55.Google Scholar
  36. [36]
    Weisman, J., “Evaluating architectural legibility: Way-finding in the built environment”. Environment and Behavior, vol. 13, pp. 189–204, 1981.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. [37]
    Worboys, M., “The Role of Modal Logics in the Description of Knowledge in a Geographic Information System”. In Cognitive and Linguistic Aspects of Geographic Space, Mark, D. and Frank, A., Eds. Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer, 1991, pp. 403–414.Google Scholar
  38. [38]
    Worboys, M., “Computation with imprecise geospatial data”. Computers, Environment and Urban Systems, vol. 22, pp. 85–106, 1998.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. [39]
    Worboys, M., “Imprecision in finite resolution spatial data”. GeoInformatica, vol. 2, pp. 257–279, 1998.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. [40]
    Worboys, M., “Observations and Knowledge”. Department of Computer Science, Keele University, Staffs., England, Science Technical Report TR99.01, 1999.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 1999

Authors and Affiliations

  • Martin Raubal
    • 1
  • Michael Worboys
    • 2
  1. 1.Department of GeoinformationTechnical University ViennaVienna
  2. 2.Department of Computer ScienceKeele UniversityKeeleUK

Personalised recommendations