Skip to main content

The Self and Its Good Vary Cross-Culturally: A Dozen Self-variations and Chinese Familial Selves

  • Chapter
  • First Online:

Abstract

In the first part, we present a taxonomy of a dozen self-variations that appear in the contemporary studies of cross-cultural anthropology, psychology and philosophy. The taxonomy is designed to serve as a general template to analyse cultural variation in views of self, which ranges from the metaphysical self (or no-self) to ideal emotional self. In the second part, we apply three aspects of the taxonomy to analyse a distinctive way that the self is conceived in the Chinese tradition, and its relation to the family and non-family members. In particular, we look at the Chinese notion of the ‘soul’ (hun-po), which informs a unique, family-oriented notion of self. This understanding helps explain the distinctive texture of the Chinese virtue of filial piety that emphasises not only respect and support of the elderly members of the family, but also reproducing children who can carry the family lineage. It also helps us understand certain family-oriented ways of decision-making in the Chinese society.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Notes

  1. 1.

    Of course, in Zhu Xi’s understanding, it has to be the direct male descendants, because the family name and the bloodline are eventually carried by the men.

  2. 2.

    An interesting question to ask is how the husband and wife are related to each other. Chinese marriage customs suggest that when the wife is married into the husband’s family, she becomes a member of her husband’s clan. She adopts the family name of her husband, and addresses the husband’s parents as ‘mother’ and ‘father’. When she dies, her husband’s clan, and in particular, her sons and unmarried daughters, are responsible for preparing ritual sacrifices and offerings for her. In this sense, the wife is ‘adopted’ by her husband’s family through the special channel of marriage.

References

  • Ames, R. T., & Rosemont, H., Jr. (2011). Were the early confucians virtuous? In C. Fraser, D. Robins, & T. O’Leary (Eds.), Ethics in early China: An anthology (pp. 17–39). Hong Kong: Hong Kong University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S. (2002). Making stories: Law, literature, life. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chan, H.-M. (2004). Informed consent Hong Kong style: An instance of moderate familism. Journal of Medicine and Philosophy, 29(2), 195–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, C. (1997). Misery and company: Sympathy in everyday life. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Creel, H. G. (1937). The birth of China. New York: Reynal and Hitchcock.

    Google Scholar 

  • Descola, P. (1996). The spears of twilight: Life and death in the Amazon Jungle. New York: New Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Everett, D. L. (2009). Don’t sleep, there are snakes: Life and language in the Amazonian Jungle. London: Profile Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fan, R., & Tao, J. (2004). Consent to medical treatment: The complex interplay of patients, families, and physicians. Journal of Medicine and Philosophy, 29(2), 139–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fan, R. (2016). DNA, brain, mind, and soul: A confucian perspective. In: Manuscript Presented at the 5th International Workshop on Bioethics, Multiculturalism and Religion, Houston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flanagan, O. (2017). Geographies of morals: Varieties of moral possibility. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flanagan, O. (1991). Varieties of moral personality: Ethics and psychological realism. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geertz, C. (1973). The interpretation of cultures: Selected essays. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hu, S. (1937). The indianization of China: A case study in cultural borrowing. In Independence, Convergence and Borrowing in Institutions, Thought, and Art (pp. 224–225). Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hampden-Turner, C., & Trompenaars, A. (1993). The seven cultures of capitalism: Value systems for creating wealth in the United States, Japan, Germany, France, Britain, Sweden, and the Netherlands. New York: Currency/Doubleday.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 61–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keillor, Garrison. (1985) Lake Wobegon Days. Viking.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacIntyre, A. C. (1981). After virtue: A study in moral theory. Notre Dame, IN: University of Notre Dame Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacIntyre, A. C. (1984). After virtue: A study in moral theory (2nd ed.). Notre Dame, IN: University of Notre Dame Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98(2), 224–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (2010). Cultures and selves: A cycle of mutual constitution. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5(4), 420–430.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, J. G. (1984). Culture and the development of everyday social explanation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46(5), 961–978.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Needdam, J. (1974). Science and civilization in China. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R. (2004). The geography of thought: How asians and westerners think differently … and why. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ricoeur, P. (1984). Time and narrative. 3 vol. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shweder, R. A., & Bourne, E. J. (1982). Does the concept of the person vary cross-culturally? In R. A. Shweder, & R. Alan LeVine. Culture theory: Essays on mind, self, and emotion (pp. 158–99). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strawson, G. (2004). Against narrativity. Ratio, n.s., 17(4), 428–452.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, S. E. (1989). Positive illusions: Creative self-deception and the healthy mind. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. (1994). Positive illusions and well-being revisited: Separating fact from fiction. Psychology Bulletin, 116(1), 21–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Triandis, H. C., Bontempo, R., Villareal, M. J., Asai, M., & Lucca, N. (1988). Individualism and collectivism: Cross-cultural perspectives on self-ingroup relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(2), 323–338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tsai, J. L. (2007). Ideal affect cultural causes and behavioral consequences. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2(3), 242–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tsai, J. L., Louie, J. Y., Chen, E. E., & Uchida, Y. (2007a). Learning what feelings to desire: Socialization of ideal affect through children’s storybooks. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33(1), 17–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tsai, J. L., Miao, F. F., & Seppala, E. (2007b). Good feelings in christianity and buddhism: Religious differences in ideal affect. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33(3), 409–421.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tsai, J. L., Miao, F. F., Seppala, E., Fung, H. H., & Yeung, D. Y. (2007c). Influence and adjustment goals: Sources of cultural differences in ideal affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92(6), 1102–1117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitehead, A. N. (1979). Process and reality. Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yü, Y.-S. (1987). O Soul, Come Back! A study in the changing conceptions of the soul and afterlife in pre-buddhist China. Harvard Journal of Asiatic Studies, 47(2), 363–395.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yü, Y.-S. (2005). Views of life and death in later Han China (东汉生死观) Shanghai Guji Chubanshe 上海古籍出版社.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhao, W. (2015). A confucian worldview and family-based informed consent: A case of concealing illness from the patient in China. In R. Fan (ed.), Family-oriented informed consent (pp. 231–43). Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Owen Flanagan thanks the Center for Advanced Study in the Behavioral Sciences, where he was a Berggruen Fellow in 2016–17, for its support. Both authors thank the John Templeton Foundation. The opinions expressed in this publication are those of the authors and do not necessarily express the views of the John Templeton Foundation.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Wenqing Zhao .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2017 Springer Nature Singapore Pte Ltd.

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Flanagan, O., Zhao, W. (2017). The Self and Its Good Vary Cross-Culturally: A Dozen Self-variations and Chinese Familial Selves. In: Menon, S., Nagaraj, N., Binoy, V. (eds) Self, Culture and Consciousness. Springer, Singapore. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-10-5777-9_17

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics