Skip to main content

A Neurophenomenological Study of Epileptic Seizure Anticipation

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Handbook of Phenomenology and Cognitive Science

Abstract

This article sets out to retrace the course of a neurophenomenological project initiated by Francisco Varela, the purpose of which is the anticipation of epileptic seizure, and to evaluate the relevance of the neurophenomenological approach from the methodological, therapeutic and epistemological viewpoints. New mathematical methods for analysing the neuro-electric activity of the brain have recently enabled researchers to detect subtle modifications of the cerebral activity a few minutes before the onset of an epileptic seizure. Do these neuro-electric changes correspond to modifications in the patients’ subjective experience, and if that is the case, what are they? In a first part, after having recalled the context of the project, I will describe the methods I used for trying to detect the dynamic micro-structure of preictal experience, the difficulties I met and the results I obtained. Then I will show how the “pheno-dynamic” and neuro-dynamic analyses have guided, determined and mutually enriched each other throughout this project. In a third part, I will show that this genetic approach to epileptic seizure opens a new line of research into a cognitive and non-pharmacological therapy for epilepsy. Finally, I will argue through this example that neurophenomenological co-determination could shed new light on the difficult problem of the “gap” which supposedly separates subjective experience from neurophysiological activity.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 259.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 329.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 329.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

     The structure and content of this article is partly inspired by Le Van Quyen and Petitmengin (2002), Petitmengin (2005), Petitmengin et al. (2006) and Petitmengin et al. (2007).

  2. 2.

    The term “pre-reflective” (in French “pré-réfléchi”, to use the vocabulary of Husserl (1913), later adopted by Sartre [1936, 1938] and Ricœur [1949]) qualifies the part of our lived experience which, although “lived through” subjectively, is not immediately accessible to consciousness, introspection or verbal report.

  3. 3.

     On the need to develop specific methods for studying lived experience: (Varela et al. 1993; Varela and Shear 1999a; Depraz et al. 2003; Petitmengin, 2009). For a synthesis of the difficulties of becoming aware of one’s pre-reflective experience: (Petitmengin 2006b; Petitmengin and Bitbol, 2009).

  4. 4.

    In this quotation the term “pre-reflexive” is equivalent to “pre-reflective”.

  5. 5.

    In Greek, epilambanein means “to fall suddenly”.

  6. 6.

    Inter-ictal (from Latin ictus: crisis) means “between two seizures”. Pre-ictal means “before a seizure”.

  7. 7.

    This figure was created by Michel Le Van Quyen.

  8. 8.

     Or LENA (CNRS UPR 640, Paris, France).

  9. 9.

    A partial seizure (versus a generalized seizure) is a seizure which starts locally in the brain at the level of the epileptogenic focus and progressively recruits other cerebral areas, but does not expand to the whole brain. It is not always accompanied with a loss of consciousness.

  10. 10.

    For a general description of the difficulties of description and the interview techniques used to overcome each of them, see Petitmengin (2006b). For a complete description of one of these methods, the “explicitation interview” see Vermersch (1994/2003).

  11. 11.

     For example the protocol that involves presenting the subjects with a 3D illusion (Lutz 2002).

  12. 12.

    These indicators are verbal, non-verbal and para-verbal. The verbal indicators are the use of “I”, the present tense, the specific context indicators (place and time), the concrete and detailed character (as opposed to conceptual and general) of the vocabulary used. An example of a non-verbal indicator is the direction of the eyes: when the subject is reliving the past experience, he takes his eyes off the interviewer to look “into space”, to the horizon. Concomitantly, the flow of speech slows, and the words are often cut with silences: these para-verbal clues are the sign that the subject is plunging into himself to make contact with the pre-reflective dimension of his experience. At the same time, metaphoric or deictic co-verbal gestures appear.

  13. 13.

    The focusing questioning mode is very well suited for helping a person direct his attention to his bodily feelings, intensify perception of the feeling and describe it (Gendlin 1996).

  14. 14.

     Various papers have shown that eye movements precisely indicate the sensorial register used (Kinsbourne 1972; Galin and Ornstein 1974; Grinder et al. 1977; Ellickson 1983; Buckner et al. 1987).

  15. 15.

    For more details on these operations of abstraction (classification/instantiation, aggregation/disaggregation and generalization/specialization) see (Petitmengin-Peugeot 1999).

  16. 16.

     These findings are confirmed by other studies, for example (Rajna et al. 1997), (Schulze-Bohage et al. 2006).

  17. 17.

     Fyodor Dostoyevsky, “The Idiot” (1868).

  18. 18.

    The epileptologist R. Efron (1956; 1957) explains that one of his patients managed to stop her incipient seizures by smelling a certain perfume. As her profession of singer did not allow her to have her bottle of perfume with her all the time, she learned to associate the smell of the perfume to a visual stimulus (a bracelet) first present and then only imagined, with the same result.

  19. 19.

    On this topic the reader may refer to the very interesting testimony of J. Benak (2001).

  20. 20.

    There are other contexts where a neurophenomenological correlation provides a neurological, “third person” confirmation of the existence of sensations or states that are not taken seriously and little studied. It is for example the case of some deep meditative states. Recent research carried out on very experienced Buddhist meditators shows that these states have quite characteristic neuronal signatures, which have never been observed in a non-pathological context (Lutz et al. 2004).

  21. 21.

    See Levine (1983). The “explanatory gap” is the core of the “hard problem” formulated by David Chalmers (1996): why does conscious experience emerge from some neurophysiological processes. The reader will find a presentation of this problem in (Roy et al. 2002). The recent article of M. Bitbol (2006) contains a summary of the theories - Behaviorism, Identity theory, Eliminativism, Functionalism, Idealism - that try to solve the problem, without providing any satisfactory solution.

  22. 22.

    “We need to turn to the only link between brain and consciousness that seems both obvious and natural: the structure of human experience itself” (Varela 1996, p. 330).

  23. 23.

    This figure was drawn by Vincent Navarro (Petitmengin et al. 2006).

  24. 24.

    Indeed, a homeomorphism between A and B brings elements of explanation, in the sense that “A is linked to B by a law”. This doesn’t mean at all that A is reducible to B, or B to A. And this link is symmetrical (if A is linked to B, B is linked to A).

  25. 25.

    On this topic we fully agree with Baynes (2004, p. 13): “Formal models can only capture the structure of a domain; they cannot capture its intrinsic nature. Those who think that the hard problem is hard do so because they think that the phenomenal character - the ‘what it is like’ of experience - cannot be fully captured by structural descriptions”.

  26. 26.

     Plato and Aristotle had already identified these characteristics, which they called “common sensibles”.

  27. 27.

    As the unceasing internal dialog, recognized as essential in the constitution of the “narrative self” (Gallagher 2000)

  28. 28.

    This neurophenomenological research of Lutz deals with 3D vision.

  29. 29.

     Is there even one experimentation that makes absolutely no reference to subjective experience? For example, a cerebral area is called “auditory” because a correlation is made between its activation and a first person report of an auditory experience. The first person point of view is always present, but in an implicit, naive manner.

References

  • Baynes T (2004) Closing the gap? Some questions to neurophenomenology. Phenomenol Cogn Sci 3(4):349-364

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benak J (2001) To quantify experience? Methodological issues in the behavioural/psychological treatment of complex partial seizures. Seizure 10:48-55

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bitbol M (2006) Une science de la conscience équitable. L’actualité de la neurophénoménologie de Francisco Varela. Intellectica 1(43):135-157

    Google Scholar 

  • Buckner W, Reese E, Reese R (1987) Eye movement as an indicator of sensory components in thought. Journal of Counseling Psychology 34(3):283-287

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chalmers D (1996) The conscious mind: in search for a fundamental theory. Oxford University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Dahl J (1992) Epilepsy. A behavior medicine approach to assessment and treatment in children. Hogrefe & Huber, Seattle/Toronto

    Google Scholar 

  • Depraz N, Varela F, Vermersch P (2003) On becoming aware. John Benjamin Press, Amsterdam

    Google Scholar 

  • Diebold G (1999) L’épilepsie, une maladie refuge. Calmann-Levy, Paris

    Google Scholar 

  • Efron R (1956) The effect of olfactory stimulus in arresting uncinate fits. Brain 79:267-281

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Efron R (1957) The conditioned inhibition of uncinate fits. Brain 80:251-262

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ellickson JL (1983) Representational systems and eye movements in an interview. Journal of Counseling Psychology 30(3):339-345

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freeman W (2001) Neurodynamics. Springer Verlag, Berlin

    Google Scholar 

  • Galin D, Orstein R (1974) Individual differences in cognitive style and reflective eye movements. Neuropsychologia 12:76-397

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher S (2000) Philosophical conceptions of the self: implications for cognitive science. Trends Cogn Sci 3(1):14-21

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gastaut H (1954) The epilepsies, electro-clinical correlations. Thomas, CC, Springfield, IL

    Google Scholar 

  • Gendlin E (1962/1997) Experiencing and the creation of meaning. Northwestern University Press, Evanston, IL

    Google Scholar 

  • Gendlin E (1996) Focusing-oriented psychotherapy. The Guilford Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Grinder J, Delozier J, Bandler R (1977) Patterns of the Hypnotic Techniques of Milton Erickson, vol. 2. Capitola, CA, Meta Publications

    Google Scholar 

  • Husserl E (1913/1977) Ideen zu einer reinen phaenomenologie und phaenomenologischen philosophie. Kluwer, The Hague, Netherlands

    Google Scholar 

  • Kinsbourne M (1972) Eye and head turning indicates cerebral lateralization. Science 179:539-541

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lachaux JP, Rodriguez E, Martinerie J, Varela FJ (1999) Measuring phase synchrony in brain signals. Hum Brain Mapp 8:194-208

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Le Van Quyen M (2003) Disentangling the dynamic core: a research program for neurodynamics at the large scale. Biol Res 36:67-88

    Google Scholar 

  • Le Van Quyen M, Petitmengin C (2002) Neuronal dynamics and conscious experience: an example of reciprocal causation before epileptic seizure. Phenomenol Cogn Sci 1:169-180

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Le Van Quyen M, Martinerie J, Navarro V, Baulac M, Varela F (2001) Characterizing the neuro-dynamical changes prior to seizures. J Clin Neurophysiol 18:191-208

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Le Van Quyen M, Navarro V, Martinerie J, Baulac M, Varela F (2003) Toward a neurodynamical understanding of ictogenesis. Epilepsia 44:30-43

    Google Scholar 

  • Le Van Quyen M, Soss J, Navarro V, Robertson R, Chavez M, Baulac M, Martinerie J (2005) Preictal state identification by synchronization changes in long-term intracranial EEG recordings. J Clin Neurophysiol 116:559-568

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levine J (1983) Materialim and qualia: the explanatory gap. Pac Philos Q 64:354-361

    Google Scholar 

  • Lutz A (2002) Toward a neurophenomenology of generative passages: a first empirical case study. Phenomenol Cogn Sci 1:133-167

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lutz A, Lachaux JP, Martinerie J, Varela J (2002) Guiding the study of brain dynamics using first person data: synchrony patterns correlate with on-going conscious states during a simple visual task. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 99:1586-1591

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lutz A, Greischar L, Rawlings N, Ricard M, Davidson R (2004) Long-term meditators self-induce high amplitude gamma synchrony during mental practice. PNAS 101(46):16369-16373

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lutz A, Thompson E (2003) Neurophenomenology. Integrating subjective experience and brain dynamics in the neuroscience of consciousness. J Conscious Stud 9-10:31-52

    Google Scholar 

  • Marchand L, de Ajuriaguerra J (1948) Les épilepsies. Desclée de Brouwer, Paris

    Google Scholar 

  • Merleau-Ponty M (1964) L’œil et l’esprit. Gallimard, Paris

    Google Scholar 

  • Mormann F, Lehnertz K, David P, Elger CE (2000) Mean phase coherence as a measure for phase synchronization and its application to the EEG of epileptic patients. Physica D 144:358-369

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mormann F, Andrzejak RG, Kreuz T, Rieke C, David P, Elger CE, Lehnertz K (2003a) Automated detection of a preseizure state based on a decrease in synchronization in intracranial electroencephalogram recordings from epilepsy patients. Phys Rev E 67. doi: 021912

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mormann F, Kreuz T, Andrzejak RG, David P, Elger CE, Lehnertz K (2003b) Epileptic seizures are preceded by a decrease in synchronization. Epilepsy Res 53:173-185

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nagel T (1970) What is it like to be a bat? Philos Rev 79:394-403

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Navarro V, Adam C, Petitmengin C, Baulac M (2006) Toothbrush-Thinking Seizures. Epilepsia 47:1971-1973

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petitmengin C (2001) L’expérience intuitive. L’Harmattan, Paris

    Google Scholar 

  • Petitmengin C (2005) Un exemple de recherche neuro-phénoménologique : l’anticipation des crises d’épilepsie. Intellectica 40:63-89

    Google Scholar 

  • Petitmengin C (2006a) L’énaction comme expérience vécue. Intellectica 43:85-92

    Google Scholar 

  • Petitmengin C (2006b) Describing one’s subjective experience in the second person. An interview method for the science of consciousness. Phenomenol Cogn Sci 5:229-269

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petitmengin C (2007) Towards the source of thoughts. The gestural and transmodal dimension of lived experience. J Conscious Stud 14(3):54-82

    Google Scholar 

  • Petitmengin C (2009) Ten years of viewing from within. Imprint Academic, London (in press)

    Google Scholar 

  • Petitmengin C, Navarro V, Baulac M (2006) Seizure anticipation: are neurophenomenological approaches able to detect preictal symptoms? Epilepsy Behav 9(2):298-306

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petitmengin C, Navarro V, Le Van Quyen M (2007) Anticipating seizure. Pre-reflective experience at the center of neurophenomenology. Conscious Cogn 16(3):746-764

    Google Scholar 

  • Petitmengin C, Bitbol M (2009) The reliability and validity of first person descriptions. In: Petitmengin (ed) Ten years of viewing from within. Imprint Academic, London (in press)

    Google Scholar 

  • Petitmengin-Peugeot C (1999) The intuitive experience. In: Varela FJ, Shear J (eds) The view from within. First-person approaches to the study of consciousness. Imprint Academic, London, pp 43-77, 1999a

    Google Scholar 

  • Rajna P, Clemens B, Csibri E, Geregely A, Gottschal M, György I, Horvath A, Horvath F, Mezöfi L, Velkey I, Veres J, Wagner E (1997) Hungarian multicentre epidemiologic study of the warning and initial symptoms (prodrome, aura) of epileptic seizures. Seizure 6:361-368

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reiter J, Andrews D (2000) A neurobehavioral approach for treatment of complex partial epilepsy efficacy. Seizure 9:198-203

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ricœur P (1949) Philosophie de la volonté. Le volontaire et l’involontaire. Aubier, Paris

    Google Scholar 

  • Roy JM, Petitot J, Pachoud B, Varela F (2002) Combler le déficit : introduction à la naturalisation de la phénoménologie. In: Petitot J, Varela F, Pachoud B, Roy JM (eds) Naturaliser la phénoménologie. CNRS Éditions, Paris, pp 1-100

    Google Scholar 

  • Sartre JP (1936) L’imagination. Presses Universitaires de France, Paris

    Google Scholar 

  • Sartre JP (1938) Esquisse d’une théorie des émotions. Hermann, Paris

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmid-Schönbein C (1998) Improvement of seizure control by psychological methods in patients with intractable epilepsies. Seizure 7:261-270

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schulze-Bohage A, Kurt C, Carius A, Steinhoff B, Mayer T (2006) Seizure anticipation by patients with focal and generalized epilepsy: a multicentre assessment of premonitory symptoms. Epilepsy Res 70:83-88

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Soulayrol R (1999) L’enfant foudroyé. Comprendre l’enfant épileptique. Odile Jacob, Paris

    Google Scholar 

  • Thompson E, Varela F (2001) Radical embodiment: neuronal dynamics and consciousness. Trends Cogn Sci 5:418-425

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stern D (1985) The interpersonal world of the infant. Basic Books, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Varela F (1995) Resonant cell assemblies: a new approach to cognitive functions and neural synchrony. Biol Res 28:81-95

    Google Scholar 

  • Varela F (1996) Neurophenomenology: a methodological remedy for the hard problem. J Conscious Stud 3:330-335

    Google Scholar 

  • Varela F (1997) The naturalization of phenomenology as the transcendance of nature: searching for generative mutual constraints. Alter: Rev Phénoménol 5:355-385

    Google Scholar 

  • Varela F (1999a) Steps to a science of interbeing: unfolding the Dharma implicit in modern cognitive science. In: Bachelor S, Claxton G, Watson G (eds) The psychology of awakening: Buddhism, science and our day to day lives. Rider/Random House, New York, pp 71-89

    Google Scholar 

  • Varela F (1999b) Quatre phares pour l’avenir des sciences cognitives. Théorie Littérature Enseignement 17: Dynamique et cognition: nouvelles approches

    Google Scholar 

  • Varela FJ, Shear J (eds) (1999a) The view from within. First-person approaches to the study of consciousness. Imprint Academic, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Varela FJ, Shear J (1999b) First-person methodologies: What, why, how? In: Varela FJ, Shear J (eds) The view from within. First-person approaches to the study of consciousness. Imprint Academic, London, pp 1-14, 1999a

    Google Scholar 

  • Varela FJ, Thompson E, Rosch E (1993) The embodied mind: cognitive science and human experience. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Varela F, Lachaux JP, Rodriguez E, Martinerie J (2001) The brain web: phase synchronisation and large scale integration. Nat Rev Neurosci 2:229-239

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vermersch P (1994/2003) L’entretien d’explicitation. ESF, Paris

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolf P (1997) Behavioral therapy. In: Engel J, Pedley TA (eds) Epilepsy: a comprehensive textbook. Lippincott-Raven, Philadelphia, PA, pp 1359-1364

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Ackowledgments

This work is dedicated to the memory of Francisco Varela who was the principle instigator of the project. I thank Vincent Navarro and Michel Le Van Quyen for our collaboration. I would also like to thank Jacques Martinerie from the Neurodynamics Group in the Cognitive Neurosciences and Brain Laboratory (CNRS UPR 640, France), and Michel Baulac and Claude Adam from the Epilepsy Unit of the Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, for their help throughout this project. Many thanks also to the patients who had the patience to answer my questions.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Claire Petitmengin .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2010 Springer Science+Business Media B.V.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Petitmengin, C. (2010). A Neurophenomenological Study of Epileptic Seizure Anticipation. In: Schmicking, D., Gallagher, S. (eds) Handbook of Phenomenology and Cognitive Science. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-90-481-2646-0_25

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics