Skip to main content

On the Functions of Sadness and Grief

  • Chapter
  • First Online:

Abstract

An account of sadness and grief is offered that focuses on their evolutionary function. Sadness and grief are distinct yet complementary adaptive responses to stress. Sadness is characterized by low physiological arousal, whereas grief is characterized by higher physiological arousal and a propensity to weep. Three general responses to stress are proposed: (1) an immune response, principally an energy-conserving state that is coordinated with enhanced immune activity; (2) a cognitive response, principally a reflective disposition characterized by more realistic situational appraisals, ultimately encouraging adaptive actions; and (3) a social response, minimally an appeal to halt aggression and, more broadly, an appeal for altruistic assistance.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   69.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   89.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD   119.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

References

  • Ahn, T. K., Janssen, M. A., & Ostrom, E. (2004). Signals, symbols, and human cooperation. In R. W. Sussman & A. R. Chapman (Eds.), The origins and nature of sociality (pp. 122–140). Hawthorne, NY: Aldine de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alicke, M. D., & Govorun, O. (2005). The better-than-average effect. In M. D. Alicke & J. I. Krueger (Eds.), Studies in self and identity. The self in social judgment (pp. 85–108). New York, NY: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allen, N. B., & Badcock, P. B. T. (2003). The social risk hypothesis of depressed mood: Evolutionary, psychosocial, and neurobiological perspectives. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 887–913.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Alloy, L. B., & Abramson, L. Y. (1979). Judgment of contingency in depressed and nondepressed students: Sadder but wiser? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 108, 441–485.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Andreoni, J., & Miller, J. H. (1993). Rational cooperation in the finitely repeated prisoner’s dilemma: Experimental evidence. Economic Journal, 103, 57–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Andrews, P. W., & Thomson, J. A., Jr. (2009). The bright side of being blue: Depression as an adaption for analyzing complex problems. Psychological Review, 116, 620–654.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Archer, J. (1999). The nature of grief: The evolution and psychology of reaction to loss. London, UK: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Axelrod, R., & Hamilton, W. D. (1981). The evolution of cooperation. Science, 211, 1390–1396.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Badcock, P. B. T., & Allen, N. B. (2003). Adaptive social reasoning in depressed mood and depressive vulnerability. Cognition and Emotion, 17, 647–670.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Banse, R., & Scherer, K. R. (1996). Acoustic profiles in vocal emotion expression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 614–636.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bard, K. A. (2000). Crying in infant primates: Insights into the development of crying in chimpanzees. In R. G. Barr, B. Hopkins, & J. A. Green (Eds.), Crying as a sign, a symptom, and a signal: Clinical, emotional and developmental aspects of infant and toddler crying, Clinics in Developmental Medicine No. 152 (pp. 157–175). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Becker, H. (1933). The sorrow of bereavement. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 27, 391–410.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bless, H., & Fiedler, K. (2006). Mood and the regulation of information processing and behavior. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), Affect in social thinking and behavior (pp. 65–84). New York, NY: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowlby, J. (1961). Process of mourning. International Journal of Psycho-Analysis, 42, 317–340.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss; Vol. 2: Separation. London, UK: Hogarth.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradbury, J. W., & Vehrenkamp, S. L. (1998). Principles of animal communication. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, J. D., & Marshall, M. A. (2000). Great expectations: Optimism and pessimism in achievement settings. In D. C. Change (Ed.), Optimism and pessimism: Implications for theory, research, and practice (pp. 239–256). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Capuron, L., Gumnick, J. F., Musselman, D. L., Lawson, D. H., Reemsnyder, A., Nemeroff, C. B., & Miller, A. H. (2002). Neurobehavioral effects of interferon-alpha in cancer patients: Phenomenology and paroxetine responsiveness of symptom dimensions. Neuropsychopharmacology, 26, 643–652.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cará, A. M., Lopes-Martins, R. A., Antunes, E., Nahoum, C. R., & De Nucci, G. (1995). The role of histamine in human penile erection. British Journal of Urology, 75, 220–224.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clore, G. L., & Huntsinger, J. R. (2007). How emotions inform judgment and regulate thought. Trends in Cognitive Science, 11, 393–399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cornelius, R. R. (1997). Toward a new understanding of weeping and catharsis? In A. J. J. M. Vingerhoets, F. J. Van Bussel, & A. J. W. Boelhouwer (Eds.), The (non)expression of emotions in health and disease (pp. 303–321). Tilburg, Netherlands: Tilburg University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dallaspezia, S., & Benedetti, F. (2011). Chronobiological therapy for mood disorders. Expert Review of Neurotherapeutics, 11, 961–970.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C. R. (1872). The expression of emotions in man and animals. London, UK: John Murray.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Delp, M. J., & Sackeim, H. A. (1987). Effects of mood on lacrimal flow: Sex differences and asymmetry. Psychophysiology, 24, 550–556.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ebersole, G. L. (2000). The function of ritual weeping revisited: Affective expression and moral discourse. History of Religions, 39, 211–246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eisenberger, N. I., & Lieberman, M. D. (2004). Why rejection hurts: A common neural alarm system for physical and social pain. Trends in Cognitive Science, 8, 294–300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P. (1982). Emotion in the human face. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P. (2003). Emotions revealed: Understanding faces and feelings. New York, NY: Times Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, L. (1995). Dominance and reproductive success among nonhuman animals: A cross-species comparison. Ethology and Sociobiology, 16, 257–333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Engel, G. L. (1962). Anxiety and depression-withdrawal. The primary affects of unpleasure. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 43, 89–97.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Erickson, D., Yoshida, K., Menezes, C., Fujino, A., Mochida, T., & Shibuya, Y. (2006). Exploratory study of some acoustic and articulatory characteristics of sad speech. Phonetica, 61, 1–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Everson, C. A. (1993). Sustained sleep deprivation impairs host defense. American Journal of Physiology, 265, R1148–R1154.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Falus, A., Grosman, N., & Darvas, Z. (2004). Histamine: Biology and medical aspects. Budapest, Hungary: SpringMed.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farberow, N. L., & Shneidman, E. S. (1961). The cry for help. New York, NY: Blakiston Division, McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Felger, J. C., & Lotrich, F. E. (2013). Inflammatory cytokines in depression: Neurobiological mechanisms and therapeutic implications. Neuroscience, 246, 199–229.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fenigstein, A., Scheier, M. F., & Buss, A. H. (1975). Public and private self-consciousness: Assessment and theory. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 43, 522–527.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Forgas, J. P. (1995). Mood and judgment: The Affect Infusion Model (AIM). Psychological Bulletin, 116, 39–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Forgas, J. P. (2002). Feeling and doing: Affective influences on interpersonal behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 13, 1–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Forgas, J. P., & East, R. (2008). How real is that smile? Mood effects on accepting or rejecting the veracity of emotional facial expressions. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 32, 157–170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Forgas, J. P., Goldenberg, L., & Unkelbach, C. (2009). Can bad weather improve your memory? A field study of mood effects on memory in a real-life setting. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 54, 254–257.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freed, P. (2009). Is sadness an evolutionarily conserved brain mechanism to dampen reward seeking? Depression may be a “sadness disorder”. Neuropsychoanalysis, 11, 61–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freud, S. (1917). Trauer und Melancholie. Internationale Zeitschrift für Psychoanalyse, 4, 288–301.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frey, W. H. (1985). Crying: The mystery of tears. Minneapolis, MN: Winston Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frey, W. H., Ahern, C., Gunderson, B. D., & Tuason, V. B. (1986). Biochemical behavioral and genetic aspects of psychogenic lacrimation: The unknown function of emotional tears. In E. J. Holly (Ed.), The preocular tear film. Lubbock, TX: Dry Eye Institute.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frick, R. W. (1985). Communicating emotion: The role of prosodic features. Psychological Bulletin, 97, 412–429.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frodi, A. (1985). When empathy fails: Aversive infant crying and child abuse. In B. Lester & C. F. Boukydis (Eds.), Infant crying (pp. 263–277). New York, NY: Plenum Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Frodi, A., & Lamb, M. E. (1980). Child abusers’ responses to infant smiles and cries. Child Development, 51, 238–241.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Geangu, E., Benga, O., Stahl, D., & Striano, T. (2010). Contagious crying beyond the first days of life. Infant Behavior and Development, 33, 279–288.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gelstein, S., Yeshurun, Y., Rozenkrantz, L., Shushan, S., Frumin, I., Roth, Y., & Sobel, N. (2011). Human tears contain a chemosignal. Science, 331, 226–230.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gertsman, E. (2011). Crying in the Middle Ages: Tears of history. London, UK: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldenberg, L., & Forgas, J. P. (2012). Can happiness make us lazy? Hedonic discounting can make us reduce perseverance and the motivation to perform. Sydney, NSW: University of New South Wales.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gorer, G. (1965). Death, grief, and mourning. New York, NY: Doubleday.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gortner, E. M., Rude, S. S., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2006). Benefits of expressive writing in lowering rumination and depressive symptoms. Behavior Therapy, 37, 292–303.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gould, S. J., & Vrba, E. S. (1982). Exaptation – a missing term in the science of form. Paleobiology, 8, 4–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graf, M. C., Gaudiano, B. A., & Geller, P. A. (2008). Written emotional disclosure: A controlled study of the benefits of expressive writing homework in outpatient psychotherapy. Psychotherapy Research, 18, 389–399.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hagen, E. H. (2011). Evolutionary theories of depression: A critical review. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 56, 716–726.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hasson, O. (2009). Emotional tears as biological signals. Evolutionary Psychology, 7, 363–370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Rapson, R. L. (1994). Emotional contagion. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, S. (1974). Care-eliciting behavior in man. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 159, 172–181.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Henry, J. (1964). Jungle people: A Kaingáng tribe of the highlands of Brazil. New York, NY: Vintage Books. (Original work published 1941).

    Google Scholar 

  • Hinde, R. A., & Spencer-Booth, Y. (1971). Effects of brief separation from mother on rhesus monkeys. Science, 173, 111–118.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hockey, J. L., Katz, J., & Small, N. (2001). Grief, mourning, and death ritual. Buckingham, UK: Open University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hofer, M. A. (1984). Relationships as regulators: A psychobiologic perspective on bereavement. Psychosomatic Medicine, 46, 183–197.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hollien, H. (1960). Some laryngeal correlates of vocal pitch. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 3, 52–58.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hopcroft, R. L. (2006). Sex, status, and reproductive success in the contemporary United States. Evolution and Human Behavior, 27, 104–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Horwitz, A., & Wakefield, J. (2007). The loss of sadness: How psychiatry transformed normal sadness into depressive disorder. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huron, D., & Shanahan, D. (2013). Eyebrow movements and vocal pitch height: Evidence consistent with an ethological signal. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 133, 2947–2952.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Immelmann, K., & Beer, C. (1989). A dictionary of ethology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnstone, R. A. (1997). The evolution of animal signals. In J. R. Krebs & N. B. Davies (Eds.), Behavioural ecology (pp. 155–178). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Joireman, J., Parrott, L., & Hammersla, J. (2002). Empathy and the self-absorption paradox: Support for the distinction between self-rumination and self-reflection. Self and Identity, 1, 53–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaufmann, W. (1992). Tragedy and philosophy. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keedwell, P. (2008). How sadness survived: The evolutionary basis of depression. Oxford, UK: Radcliffe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kenis, G., & Maes, M. (2002). Effects of antidepressants on the production of cytokines. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 5, 401–412.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kottler, J. A. (1996). The language of tears. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kottler, J. A., & Montgomery, M. J. (2001). Theories of crying. In A. J. J. M. Vingerhoets & R. R. Cornelius (Eds.), Adult crying: A biopsychosocial approach (pp. 1–−7). East Sussex, UK: Brunner-Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kraemer, D. L., & Hastrup, J. L. (1988). Crying in adults: Self-control and autonomic correlates. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 6, 53–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kraepelin, E. (1921). Psychiatrie. Ein Lehrbuch für Studierende und Ärzte [Manic-depressive insanity and paranoia]. In G. M. Robertson (Ed.), R. M. Barclay (Trans.), Text-book of psychiatry vols. iii and iv (8th ed.). Edinburgh, Scotland: Livingstone. (Original work published 1899)

    Google Scholar 

  • Landreth, C. (1941). Factors associated with crying in young children in the nursery school and the home. Child Development, 12, 81–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lane, C. J. (2006). Evolution of gender differences in adult crying (Doctoral dissertation). University of Texas at Arlington, Arlington, TX.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larson, T. (2010). The saddest music ever written: The story of Samuel Barber’s Adagio for Strings. New York, NY: Pegasus Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, A. J. (1934). Melancholia: A clinical survey of depressive states. British Journal of Psychiatry, 80, 277–378.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, C. S. (1961). A grief observed. New York, NY: Seabury Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lorenz, K. (1937). Über die Bildung des Instinktbegriffes. Naturwissenschaften, 25, 289–300. 307–318, 324–331.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lutz, T. (2001). Crying: A natural and cultural history of tears. New York, NY: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maguire, H. (1977). The depiction of sorrow in Middle Byzantine art. Dumbarton Oaks Papers, 31, 123–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marsella, A. J., Sartorius, N., Jablensky, A., & Fenton, F. R. (1985). Cross-cultural studies of depressive disorders: An overview. In A. Kleinman & B. Good (Eds.), Culture and depression: Studies in the anthropology and cross-cultural psychiatry of affect and disorder (pp. 299–324). Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maynard Smith, J. M., & Harper, D. (2003). Animal signals. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, A. H., Haroon, E., Raison, C. L., & Felger, J. C. (2013). Cytokine targets in the brain: Impact on neurotransmitters and neurocircuits. Depression and Anxiety, 30, 297–306.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, B. L., & Cummings, J. L. (2007). The human frontal lobes: Functions and disorders. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Montagu, A. (1960). Natural selection and the origin and evolution of weeping in man. Journal of the American Medical Association, 174, 392–397.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, M. T., & Fresco, D. (2012). Depressive realism: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 32, 496–509.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, H. B. M., Wittkower, E., & Chance, N. (1964). Cross-cultural inquiry into the symptomatology of depression. Transcultural Psychiatric Research Review, 1, 5–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murray, I. R., & Arnott, J. L. (1993). Toward the simulation of emotion in synthetic speech: A review of the literature on human vocal emotion. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 93, 1097–1108.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Murube, J. (2009). Hypotheses on the development of psychoemotional tearing. The Ocular Surface, 7, 171–175.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Musselman, D. L., Lawson, D. H., Gumnick, J. F., Manatunga, A. K., Penna, S., Goodkin, R. S., … Miller, A. H. (2001). Paroxetine for the prevention of depression induced by high-dose interferon alfa. New England Journal of Medicine, 344, 961–966.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nesse, R. M. (1991). What good is feeling bad? The evolutionary benefits of psychic pain. The Sciences, 31, 30–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nesse, R. M. (2000). Is depression an adaptation? Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 14–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nesse, R. M., & Williams, G. C. (1994). Why we get sick: The new science of Darwinian medicine. New York, NY: Times Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Panksepp, J., & Bernatzky, G. (2002). Emotional sounds and the brain: The neuro-affective foundations of musical appreciation. Behavioural Processes, 60, 133–155.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Partan, S., & Marler, P. (1999). Communication goes multimodal. Science, 283, 1272–1273.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pflug, B., & Tölle, R. (1971). Disturbance of the 24-hour rhythm in endogenous depression and the treatment of endogenous depression by sleep deprivation. International Pharmacopsychiatry, 6, 187–196.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Provine, R. R., Cabrera, M. O., Brocato, N. W., & Krosnowski, K. A. (2011). When the whites of the eyes are red: A uniquely human cue. Ethology, 117, 1–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raison, C. L., Capuron, L., & Miller, A. (2006). Cytokines sing the blues: Inflammation and the pathogenesis of depression. Trends in Immunology, 27, 24–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raison, C. L., & Miller, A. H. (2013). The evolutionary significance of depression in Pathogen Host Defense (PATHOS-D). Molecular Psychiatry, 18, 15–37.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raleigh, M. J., McGuire, M. T., Brammer, G. L., Pollack, D. B., & Yuwiler, A. (1991). Serotonergic mechanisms promote dominance acquisition in adult male vervet monkeys. Brain Research, 559, 181–190.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roes, F. L. (1989). On the origin of crying and tears. Human Ethology Newsletter, 5, 5–6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, N. L., Szuba, M. P., Staab, J. P., Evans, D. L., & Dinges, D. F. (2001). Neuroimmunologic aspects of sleep and sleep loss. Seminars in Clinical Neuropsychiatry, 6, 295–307.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenblatt, P. C., Walsh, R. P., & Jackson, D. A. (1976). Grief and mourning in cross-cultural perspective. New Haven, CT: Human Relations Area Files.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ross, L., & Nisbett, R. E. (1991). The person and the situation: Perspectives of social psychology. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saul, L. J., & Bernstein, C. (1941). The emotional settings of some attacks of urticaria. Psychosomatic Medicine, 3, 349–369.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1985). The self-consciousness scale: A revised version for use with general populations. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 15, 687–699.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scherer, K. R., Johnstone, T., & Klasmeyer, G. (2003). Vocal expression of emotion. In R. J. Davidson, K. R. Scherer, & H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of the affective sciences (pp. 433–456). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sharot, T. (2011). The optimism bias: A tour of the irrationally positive brain. New York, NY: Pantheon Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, A., & Sapru, H. N. (2006). Essential neuroscience. London, UK: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slavich, G. M., Way, B. M., Eisenberger, N. I., & Taylor, S. E. (2010). Neural sensitivity to social rejection is associated with inflammatory responses to social stress. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107, 14817–14822.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spencer-Booth, Y., & Hinde, R. A. (1971). Effects of brief separations from mothers during infancy on behavior of rhesus monkeys 6-24 months later. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 12, 157–172.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Storbeck, J., & Clore, G. L. (2005). With sadness comes accuracy; with happiness, false memory: Mood and the false memory effect. Psychological Science, 16, 785–791.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sundberg, J. (1987). The science of the singing voice. DeKalb, IL: Northern Illinois University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Švec, J. G., & Pešák, J. (1994). Vocal breaks from the modal to the falsetto register. Folia Phoniatrica et Logopaedica, 46, 97–103.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tan, H. B., & Forgas, J. P. (2010). When happiness makes us selfish, but sadness makes us fair: Affective influences on interpersonal strategies in the dictator game. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 571–576.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tartter, V. C. (1980). Happy talk: Perceptual and acoustic effects of smiling on speech. Perception & Psychophysics, 27, 24–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tinbergen, N. (1952). “Derived” activities; their causation, biological significance, origin, and emancipation during evolution. Quarterly Review of Biology, 27, 1–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tinbergen, N. (1964). The evolution of signaling devices. In W. Etkin (Ed.), Social behavior and organization among vertebrates (pp. 206–230). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomkins, S. S. (1980). Affect as amplification: Some modifications in theory. In R. Plutchik & H. Kellerman (Eds.), Emotion: Theory, research and experience (pp. 141–164). New York, NY: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Trapnell, P. D., & Campbell, J. D. (1999). Private self-consciousness and the five-factor model of personality: Distinguishing rumination from reflection. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 284–304.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Trimble, M. (2012). Why humans like to cry: Tragedy, evolution, and the brain. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Quarterly Review of Biology, 46, 35–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trivers, R. L. (1974). Parent-offspring conflict. American Zoologist, 14, 249–264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Urban, G. (1991). A discourse-centered approach to culture: Native South American myths and rituals. Austin, TX: University of Texas Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Viggiano, D., Ruocco, L. A., Arcieri, S., & Sadile, A. G. (2004). Involvement of norepinephrine in the control of activity and attentive processes in animal models of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neural Plasticity, 11, 133–149.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Vingerhoets, A. J. J. M. (2013). Why only humans weep: Unravelling the mysteries of tears. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Vingerhoets, A. J. J. M., & Cornelius, R. R. (2001). Adult crying: A biopsychosocial approach. East Sussex, UK: Brunner-Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vingerhoets, A. J. J. M., Cornelius, R. R., Van Heck, G. L., & Becht, M. C. (2000). Adult crying: A model and review of the literature. Review of General Psychology, 4, 354–377.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weary, G., & Edwards, J. A. (1994). Social cognition and clinical psychology: Anxiety, depression, and the processing of social information. In R. S. Wyer Jr. & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (pp. 289–338). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weinstein, N. D. (1987). Unrealistic optimism about susceptibility to health problems: Conclusions from a community-wide sample. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 10, 481–500.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wiley, R. H. (1983). The evolution of communication: Information and manipulation. In T. R. Halliday & P. J. B. Slater (Eds.), Communication (pp. 82–113). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, E. (2008). Against happiness: In praise of melancholy. New York, NY: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yost, J. H., & Weary, G. (1996). Depression and the correspondent inference bias: Evidence for more effortful cognitive processing. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 192–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Young, K. (Ed.). (2010). The art of losing: Poems of grief and healing. New York, NY: Bloomsbury.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi, A., & Zahavi, A. (1997). The handicap principle: A missing piece of Darwin’s puzzle. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zeifman, D. M. (2001). An ethological analysis of human infant crying: Answering Tinbergen’s four questions. Developmental Psychobiology, 39, 265–285.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to David Huron .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2018 Springer International Publishing AG, part of Springer Nature

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Huron, D. (2018). On the Functions of Sadness and Grief. In: Lench, H. (eds) The Function of Emotions. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-77619-4_5

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics