Skip to main content

Predicting Cognitive Capacity from Natural History

Examples from Four Species of Corvids

  • Chapter
Current Ornithology

Part of the book series: Current Ornithology ((CUOR,volume 13))

Abstract

Birds have been studied for centuries because they are numerous, conspicuous, and aesthetically pleasing to humans Despite their overall regard for birds, historically, many ornithologists have considered birds as instinct-driven organisms of little intellectual capacity. For example, the ornithological textbook of choice from the 1960s states the following view of avian intelligence:

Flight has proven to be an enormously successful evolutionary venture, but one that has cost birds dearly in mental development. In effect, flight has become a substitute for cleverness; birds solve many potential problems merely by flying away from them…. As a consequence, much [avian] behavior is, by mam-malian standards, fragmentary, stereotyped, and at times amazingly stupid. (Welty, 1962, p.159).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Anderson, J. R., 1990, Cognitive Psychology and Its Implications, 3rd edition, Freeman and Co., New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Andersson, M., 1982, Female choice selects for extreme tail length in a widowbird, Nature 299: 818–820.

    Google Scholar 

  • Armstrong, D. P., Gass, C.L., and Sutherland, G. D., 1987, Should foragers remember where they’ve been? Explorations of a simulation model based on the behavior and energetics of a territorial hummingbird, in: Foraging Behavior ( A. C. Kamil, J R. Krebs, and H. R. Pulliam, eds.), Plenum Press, New York, pp. 563–586.

    Google Scholar 

  • Balda, R. P., 1980, Recovery of cached seeds by a captive Nucifraga caryocatactes Z. Tierpsychol, 52: 331–346.

    Google Scholar 

  • Balda, R. P:, 1987, Avian impact on pinyon-juniper woodland, in: Proceedings—Pinyonjuniper Conference (R. L. Everett, ed.), U.S.D.A. Gen. Tech. Report INT-215, pp. 526–533.

    Google Scholar 

  • Balda, R. P., and Bateman, G. C., 1971, Flocking and annual cycle of the Pinyon Jay (Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus), Condor 73: 287–302.

    Google Scholar 

  • Balda, R. P., and Bateman, G. C., 1972, The breeding biology of the Pinyon Jay, Living Bird 11: 5–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Balda, R. P., and Kamil, A. C., 1989, A comparative study of cache recovery by three corvid species, Anim. Behay. 38: 486–495.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barnea, A., and Nottebohm, F., 1994, Seasonal recruitment of hippocampal neurons in adult free-ranging Black-capped Chickadees, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 91: 11217–11221.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Basil, J. A., Kamil, A. C., Balda, R. P., and Fite, K. V., 1996, Differences in hippocampal volume among food-storing corvids, Brain Behay. Evol. 47: 156–164.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Basolo, A. L., 1990, Female preference predates the evolution of the sword in swordtail fish, Science 250: 808–810.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bateman, G. C., and Balda, R. P.,1973, Growth, development, and food habits of young Pinyon Jays, Auk 90:39–61.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beatty, W. W., and Shavalia, D. A., 1980, Spatial memory in rats: time course of working memory and effect of anesthetics, Behay. Neur. Biol. 28: 454–462.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bednekoff, P. A., and Balda, R. P., 1996a, Social caching and observational spatial memory in Pinyon Jays, Behaviour, 133: 807–826.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bednekoff, P. A., and Balda, R. P., 1996b, Observational spatial memory in Clark’s Nutcrackers and Mexican Jays, Anim. Behay. 52: 833–839.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bednekoff, P. A., and Balda, R. P., 1996b, Observational spatial memory in Clark’s Nutcrackers and Mexican Jays, Anim. Behay. 52: 833–839.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beecher, M. D., Campbell, S. E., and Burt, J. M., 1994, Song perception in the Song Sparrow: birds classify by song type but not by singer, Anim. Behay. 43: 283–287.

    Google Scholar 

  • Benkman, C. W., Balda, R. P., and Smith, C. C., 1984, Adaptations for seed dispersal and the compromises due to seed predation in limber pine, Ecology 65: 632–642.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berger, L. R., and Ligon, J. D., 1977, Vocal communication and individual recognition in the Pinon Jay Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus, Anim. Behay. 25: 567–584.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bitterman, M. E., 1960, Toward a comparative psychology of learning, Am. Psycho. 15: 704–712.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bock, W. J., Balda, R. P., and Vander Wall, S. B.,1973, Morphology of the sublingual pouch and tongue musculature in Clark’s Nutcracker, Auk 90: 491–519.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brodbeck, D. R., 1994, Memory for spatial and local cues: a comparison of a storing and a nonstoring species, Anim. Learn. Behay. 22: 119–133.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brooks, D. R., and McLennan, D. A., 1991, Phylogeny, Ecology, and Behavior, University of Chicago Press, Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, J. L., and Brown, E. R., 1981, Kin selection and individual selection in babblers, in: Natural Selection and Social Behavior: Recent Research and New Theory ( R. D. Alexander and D. W. Tinkle, eds.), Chiron Press, New York, pp. 244–256.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, J. L., and Brown, E. R., 1990, Mexican jays: uncooperative breeding, in: Cooperative Breeding in Birds( P. B. Stacy, and W. D. Koenig, eds.), Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, England, pp. 267–288.

    Google Scholar 

  • Catchpole, C. K., 1986, Song repertories and reproductive success in the Great Reed Warbler, Acrocephalus arundinaceus, Behay. Ecol. Sociobiol. 19: 439–445.

    Google Scholar 

  • Catchpole, C. K., Leisler, B., and Dittami, J., 1986, Sexual differences in the responses of captive Great Reed Warblers (Acrocephalus arundinaceus) to variation in song structure and size, Ethology 72: 69–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chance, M. R. A., and Mead, A. P., 1953, Social behavior and primate evolution, Evolution 7: 395–439.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheney, D. L., and Seyfarth, R. M., 1990, How Monkeys See the World, University of Chicago Press, Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clayton, N. S., 1992, The ontogeny of food-storing and retrieval in Marsh Tits, Behaviour 122: 11–25.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clayton, N. S., 1994, The role of age and experience in the behavioural development of food-storing and retrieval in Marsh Tits, Parus palustris, Anim. Behay. 47: 1435–1444.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clayton, N. S., 1995a, Development of memory and the hippocampus: Comparison of food-storing and nonstoring birds on a one-trial associative memory task, J. Neurosci. 15: 2796–2807.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Clayton, N. S., 1995b, The neuroethological development of food-storing memory: a case of use it or lose it! Behay. Brain Res. 70: 95–102.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Clayton, N. S., and Krebs, J. R., 1993, Lateralization in Paridae: Comparison of a storing and a non-storing species on a one-trial associative memory task, J. Comp. Physiol. A 171: 807–815.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clayton, N. S., and Krebs, J. R., 1994a, One-trial associative memory: Comparison of food-storing and nonstoring species of birds, Anim. Learn. Behay. 22: 366–372.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clayton, N. S., and Krebs, J. R., 1994b, Memory for spatial and object-specific cues in food-storing and non-storing birds, J. Comp. Physiol. A 174: 371–379.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole, S., Hainsworth, F. R., Kamil, A. C., Mercier, T., and Wolf, L. L., 1982, Spatial learning as an adaptation in hummingbirds, Science 217: 655–657.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Crook, J. H., 1964, The evolution of social organization and visual communication in the weaverbirds (Ploceidae), Behay. Suppl. 10: 1–178.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crook, J. H., 1965, The adaptive significance of avian social organization, Sympos. Zool. Soc. London 14: 181–218.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davies, N. B., 1992, Dunnock Behavior and Social Evolution, Oxford University Press, Oxford, England.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dimmick, C. R., 1993, Life History and the Development of Cache-recovery Behaviors in Clark’s Nutcrackers, Unpublished Ph. D. Dissertation, Northern Arizona University, Flagstaff.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dukas, R., and Real, L. A., 1991, Learning foraging tasks by bees: a comparison between social and solitary species, Anim. Behay. 44: 269–274.

    Google Scholar 

  • Emlen, S. T., and Wrege P. H., 1988, The role of kinship in helping decisions among White-fronted Bee-eaters, Behay. Ecol. Sociobiol. 23: 305–315.

    Google Scholar 

  • Endler, J. A., 1980, Natural selection on color patterns in Poecilia reticulata, Evolution 34: 76–91.

    Google Scholar 

  • Falls, J. B., 1992, Playback: a historical perspective, in: Playback and Studies in Animal Communication (P. K. McGregor, ed.), Plenum Press, New York, pp. 11–33. Felsenstein, J., 1985, Phylogenies and the comparative method, Am. Nat. 125: 1–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Felsenstein, J. 1988, Phylogenies and quantitative characters, Ann. Rev. Ecol. System. 19: 445–471.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, H., 1987, The Mind’s New Science: A History of the Cognitive Revolution, 2nd. edition, Basic Books, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gaulin, S. J. C., and FitzGerald, R. W., 1986, Sex differences in spatial ability: an evolutionary hypothesis and test, Amer. Natl. 127: 74–88.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gaulin, S. J. C., and FitzGerald, R. W., 1989, Sexual selection for spatial-learning ability, Anim. Behay. 37: 322–331.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gill, F. B., 1988, Trapline foraging by Hermit Hummingbirds: competition for an undefended, renewable resource, Ecology 69: 1933–1942.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gillan, J., 1981, Reasoning in the chimpanzee, II. Transitive inference, J. Exp. Psycho. Anim. Behay. Proc. 7: 150–164.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giuntoli, M., and Mewaldt, L. R., 1978, Stomach contents of Clark’s Nutcrackers collected in western Montana, Auk 95: 595–598.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, D., 1986, Crows of the World, 2nd edition, British Museum of Natural History, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goss-Custard, J. D., 1981, Feeding behavior of Red Shank, Tringa totanus, and optimal foraging theory, in: Foraging Behavior: Ecological, Ethological, and Psychological Approaches (A. C. Kamil and T. D. Sargent, eds.), New York, Garland Press, pp. 115134.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grant, B. R., and Grant, P. R., 1989, Natural selection in a population of Darwin’s Finches, Am. Nat. 133: 377–393.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guntert, M., Hay, D. B., and Balda, R. P., 1988, Communal roosting of the Pygmy Nuthatch: a winter survival strategy, in: Proceedings of the International Ornithological Congress ( H. Ouellet, ed.), University of Ottawa Press, Ottawa, pp. 1963–1972.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harvey, P. H., and Pagel, M. D., 1991, The Comparative Method in Evolutionary Biology, Oxford University Press, Oxford, England.

    Google Scholar 

  • Healy, S. D., and Krebs, J. R., 1992, Food storing and the hippocampus in corvids: amount and volume are correlated, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B248: 241–245.

    Google Scholar 

  • Healy, S. D., Clayton, N. S., and Krebs, J. R., 1994, Development of hippocampal spe-cialisation in two species of tits (Parus spp.), Behay. Brain Res. 61: 23–28

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Herrnstein, R. J., 1985, Riddles of natural categorization, in: Animal Intelligence ( L. Weiskrantz, ed.), Clarendon Press, Oxford, England, pp. 129–142.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hilton, S. C., and Krebs, J. R., 1990, Spatial memory of four species of Parus:performance in an open-field analogue of a radial maze,Quart. J Exper. Psychol.42B: 345–368.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hollis, K. L., 1984, The biological function of Pavlovian conditioning: the best defense is a good offense, J. Exper. Psychol.: Anim. Behay. Proc. 10: 413–425.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hope, S., 1989, Phylogeny of the Avian Family Corvidae, Unpublished Ph. D. Dissertation, City University of New York, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Humphrey, N. K., 1976, The social function of intellect, in: Growing Points in Ethology ( P. P. G. Bateson and R. A. Hinde, eds.), Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, England, pp. 303–317.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacobs, L. F., Gaulin, S. J. C., Sherry, D. F., and Hoffman, G. E., 1990, Evolution of spatial cognition: sex-specific patterns of spatial behavior predict hippocampal size,Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 87: 6349–6352.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Jolly, A., 1966, Lemur social behaviour and primate intelligence, Science 153: 501–506.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kamil, A. C., 1978, Systematic foraging by a nectar-feeding bird, the Amakihi (Loxops virens), J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 92: 388–396.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kamil, A. C., 1988a, A synthetic approach to the study of animal intelligence, in: Comparative Perspectives in Modern Psychology, Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, volume 35, ( D. W. Leger, ed.), University of Nebraska Press, Lincoln, pp. 230–257.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kamil, A. C., 1988b, Experimental design in ornithology, in: Current Ornithology, volume 5 ( D. M. Power, ed.), Plenum Press, New York, pp. 313–346.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kamil, A. C., and Yoerg, S. I., 1982, Learning and foraging behavior, in: Perspectives on Ethology, volume 5 ( P. P. G. Bateson and P. H. Klopfer, eds.), Plenum Press, New York, pp. 325–364.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kamil, A. C., Jones, T. B., Pietrewicz, A. T., and Maudlin, J., 1977, Positive transfer from successive reversal training to learning set in Blue Jays, J. Comp. Physiol. 91: 79–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kamil, A. C., Balda, R. P., and Olson, D. J., 1994, Performance of four seed-caching corvid species in the radial-arm maze analog, J. Comp. Psychol.:Anim. Behay. Proc. 108: 385–393.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ketterson, E. D., Nolan, V. Jr., Cawthorn, M. J., Parker, P. G., and Ziegenfus, C., 1996, Phenotypic engineering: using hormones to explore the mechanistic and functional bases of phenotypic variation in nature, Ibis 138: 70–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koenig, W. D., and Stacy, P. B., 1990, Acorn Woodpeckers: group living and food storage under contrasting ecological conditions, in: Cooperative Breeding in Birds( P. B. Stacy and W. D. Koenig, eds.), Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, England, pp. 415–453.

    Google Scholar 

  • Konishi, M., and Nottebohm, F., 1969, Experimental studies in the ontogeny of avian vocalization, in: Bird Vocalization: their Relation to Current Problems in Biology and Psychology ( R. A. Hinde, ed.), Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, England, pp. 29–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krebs, J. R., and Davies, N., eds., 1984, Behavioural Ecology, Sinauer Associates Inc., Sunderland, Massachusetts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krebs, J. R., Erichsen, T. J., Webber, M. I., and Charnov, E. L., 1977, Optimal prey selection in the Great Tit (Parus major), Anim. Behay. 25: 30–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krebs, J. R., Kacelnik, A., and Taylor, P., 1978, Test of optimal sampling by foraging Great Tits, Nature 275: 27–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krebs, J. R., Sherry, D. F., Healy, S. D., Perry, V. H., and Vaccarino, A. L., 1989, Hippocampal specialization of food-storing birds, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 86: 1388–1392.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Krebs, J. R., Healy, S. D., and Shettleworth, S., 1990, Spatial memory of Paridae: Comparison of a storing and non-storing species, P. ater and P. major, Anim. Behay. 39: 1127–1138.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krushinskya, N. L., 1966, Some complex forms of feeding behavior of nutcracker, Nucifraga caryocatactes, after removal of old cortex, Z. Evoluz. Biochem. Fisiolog. II, 563–568.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lack, D., 1947, Darwin’s Finches, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, England.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ligon, J. D., 1978, Reproductive interdependence of Pinyon Jays and pinyon pines, Ecol. Monog. 48: 111–126.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ligon, J. D., and Ligon, S. H., 1983, Reciprocity in the Green Woodhoopoe (Phoeniculus purpureus), Anim. Behay. 31: 480–489.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ligon, J. D., and Ligon, S. H., 1988, Territorial quality: key determinant of fitness in the group-living Green Woodhoopoe, in: The Ecology of Social Behavior ( C. N. Slobodchkoff, ed.), Academic Press, San Diego, pp. 229–253.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ligon, J. D., and Martin, D. J., 1974, Pinyon seed assessment by the Pinyon Jay, Anim. Behay. 22: 421–429.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lima, S. L., 1984, Downy Woodpecker foraging behavior: efficient sampling in simple stochastic environments, Ecology 65: 166–174.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lorenz, K., 1941, Vergleichende bewegungsstudien bei Anatiden, J. Ornithol. 89: 194–294.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacArthur, R. H., and Pianka, E. R., 1966, On optimal use of a patchy environment. Am. Nat. 100: 603–609.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marler, P., 1969, Tonal quality of bird sounds, in: Bird Vocalizations ( R. A. Hinde, ed.), Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, England, pp. 5–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marzluff, J. M., and Balda, R. P., 1992, The Pinyon Jay: Behavioral Ecology of a Colonial and Cooperative Corvid, Academic Press, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • McDonald, M. V., 1989, Function of song in Scott’s Seaside Sparrow, Ammodramus maritimus penensulae, Anim. Behay. 38: 468–485.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGowan, K. J., and Woolfenden, G. E., 1989, A sentinel system in the Florida Scrub Jay, Anim. Behay. 37: 1000–1006.

    Google Scholar 

  • McLennon, D. A., and McPhail, J. D., 1989, Experimental investigations of the evolutionary significance of sexually dimorphic nuptial colouration in Gasterosteus aculeatus (L.): Temporal changes in the structure of the male mosaic signal, Canad. J. Zool. 67: 1767–1777.

    Google Scholar 

  • McLennon, D. A., and McPhail, J. D., 1990, Experimental investigations on the evolutionary significance of sexually dimorphic nuptial colouration in Gasterosteus aculeatus (L.): The relationship between male colours and female behaviour, Canad. J. Zool. 68: 482–492.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mewaldt, L. R., 1956, Nesting behavior of the Clark’s Nutcracker, Condor 58: 3–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morton, E. S., 1977, On the occurrence and significance of motivation-structural rules in some bird and mammal sounds, Am. Nat. 111: 855–869.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morse, D. H., and Fritz, R. S., 1987, The consequences of foraging for reproductive success, in: Foraging Behavior ( A. C. Kamil, J. R. Krebs, and H. R. Pulliam, eds.), Plenum Press, New York, pp. 443–455.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moss, C. J., 1988, Elephant Memories, Houghton Mifflin, Boston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Myberg, A. A., Jr., and Riggio, R. J., 1985, Acoustically mediated individual recognition by a coral reef fish (Pomacentrus partitus), Anim. Behay. 33: 411–416.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olson, D. J., 1991, Species differences in spatial memory among Clark’s Nutcrackers, Scrub Jays, and Pigeons, J. Exper. Psycho.: Anim. Behav. Proc. 104: 326–334.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olson, D. J., Kamil, A. C., Balda, R. P., and Nims, P. J., 1995, Performance of four seed-caching corvid species in operant tests of nonspatial and spatial memory, J. Comp. Psycho.: Anim. Behay Proc. 109: 173–181.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Olton, D. S., and Collison, C., 1979, Intramaze cues and `odor trails“ fail to direct choice behavior on an elevated maze, Anim. Learn. Behay. 7: 221–223.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olton, D. S., and Samuelson, R. J., 1976, Remembrance of places passed: spatial memory in rats, J. Exper. Psycho. Anim. Behay. Proc. 2: 97–116.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pepperberg, I. M., 1990, Some cognitive capacities of an African Grey Parrot (Psittacus erithacus), in: Advances inthe Study of Behavior ( P. J. B. Slater, J. S. Rosenblatt, and C. Beer, eds.), Academic Press, New York, pp. 357–409.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, A. T., 1993, Adaptive geographical variation in bill shape of Scrub Jays, Am. Nat. 142: 508–527.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pettifor, R. A., Perrins, C. M., and McCleery, R. H., 1988, Individual optimization clutch size in great tits, Nature 336: 160–152.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pulliam, H. R., 1980, Do Chipping Sparrows forage optimally?, Ardea 68: 75–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pulliam, H. R., 1981, Learning to forage optimally, in: Foraging Behavior: Ecological, Ethological, and Psychological Approaches ( A. C. Kamil, and T. D. Sargent, eds.), Garland Press, New York, pp. 379–388.

    Google Scholar 

  • Real, L. A., 1991, Animal choice behavior and the evolution of cognitive architecture, Science 253: 980–986.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Roitblat, H. L., Beaver T., and Terrace, H., eds., 1984, AnimalCognition, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, New Jersey.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sherry, D. F., and Schacter, D. L., 1987, The evolution of multiple memory systems, Psycho. Rev. 94: 439–454.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sherry, D. F., and Vaccarino, A. L., 1989, Hippocampal aspiration disrupts cache recovery in Black-capped Chickadees, Behay. Neurosci. 103: 308–318.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sherry, D. F., Vaccarino, A. L., Buckenham, K., and Herz, R., 1989, The hippocampal complex of food-storing birds, Brain Behay. Evol. 34: 308–317.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sherry, D. F., Forbes, M. R. L., Khurgel, M., and Ivy, G. O., 1993, Females have a larger hippocampus than males in the brood-parasitic Brown-headed Cowbird, Proc. Natl. Mad. Sci. 90: 7839–7843.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Shettleworth, S. J., 1984, Learning and behavioral ecology, in: Behavioral Ecology, 2nd edition, ( J. R. Krebs, and N. B. Davies, eds.) Sinauer Associates, Sunderland, Massachusetts, pp. 170–194.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shettleworth, S. J., 1993, Where is the comparison in comparative cognition? Psychol. Sci. 4: 179–184.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spector, D., 1992, Wood-warbler song systems: a review of paruline singing behavior, in: CurrentOrnithology, Volume 9, (D. M. Powers, ed.), pp. 199–238.

    Google Scholar 

  • Staicer, C. A. 1991, Characteristics, use, and significance of two singing behaviors in Grace’s Warbler (Dendrocia graciae), Auk 106: 49–63.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stoddard, P. K., 1996, Vocal recognition of neighbors by territorial passerines, in: Ecology and Evolution of Acoustic Communication in Birds, ( D. E. Kroodsma, and E. H. Miller, eds.) Cornell University Press, Ithaca, New York, pp. 356–374.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sydeman, W. J., Guntert, M., and Balda, R. P., 1988, Annual reproductive yield in the cooperative Pygmy Nuthatch (Sitta pygmaea), Auk 105: 70–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tinbergen, N., 1960, The Herring Gull’s World, Doubleday, Garden City, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tinbergen, N., 1963, The shell menace. Nat. Hist. 72: 28–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tinbergen, N., Impekoven, M., and Frank, D., 1967, An experiment on spacing-out as a defense against predation, Behaviour 28: 307–321.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomback, D. F., 1980, How nutcrackers find their seed stores, Condor 82: 10–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vander Wall, S. B., and Balda, R. P., 1977, Coadaptations of the Clark’s Nutcracker and the pinyon pine for efficient seed harvest and dispersal, Ecol. Monog. 47: 89–111.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vander Wall, S. B., and Balda, R. P., 1981, Ecology and evolution of food-storage behavior in conifer-seed caching corvids, Z. Tierpsychol. 56: 217–242.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vander Wall, S. B., and Hutchins, H. E., 1983, Dependence of Clark’s Nutcracker (Nucifraga columbiana) on conifer seeds during the postfledgling period, Can. Field Natl. 97: 89–111.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vaughan, J. W., 1988, Formation of equivalence sets in pigeons, J. Exper. Psychol.: Anim. Behay.Proces. 14: 36–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weary, D. M., and Krebs, J. R., 1992, Great Tits classify songs by individual voice characteristics, Anim. Behay. 43: 283–287.

    Google Scholar 

  • Welty, J. C., 1962, The Life of Birds, W. B. Saunders Co., New York, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whiten, A., and Byrne, R. W., 1988, The Machiavellian intelligence hypotheses (edi-torial), in: Machiavellian Intelligence ( R. W. Byrne, and A. Whiten, eds.), Clarendon Press, Oxford, England, pp. 1–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, G. C., 1966, Adaptation and Natural Selection, Princeton University Press, Princeton, New Jersey.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woolfenden, G. E., and Fitzpatrick, J. W., 1984, The Florida Scrub Jay: Demography of a Cooperative-breeding Bird, Monogr. Pop. Biol. 20, Princeton University Press, Princeton, New Jersey.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wrege, P. H., and Emlen, S. T., 1994, Family structure influences mate choice in White-fronted Bee-eaters, Behay. Ecol. Evol. 35: 185–193.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wunderle, J. M., and Martinez, J. S., 1987, Spatial learning in the nectarivorous banana-quit: Juveniles versus adults, Anim. Behay. 35: 652–658.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wynne, C. D. L., 1995, Reinforcement accounts for transitive inference performance, Anim. Learn. Behay. 23: 207–217.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yoerg, S. I., and Kamil, A C, 1988, Diet choices of Blue Jays (Cyanocitta cristata) as a function of time spent foraging, J. Comp. Psychol. 102: 230–235.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1996 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Balda, R.P., Kamil, A.C., Bednekoff, P.A. (1996). Predicting Cognitive Capacity from Natural History. In: Nolan, V., Ketterson, E.D. (eds) Current Ornithology. Current Ornithology, vol 13. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-5881-1_2

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-5881-1_2

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4613-7697-2

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4615-5881-1

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics