Abstract
The study of iconicity, or the resemblance between word forms and their meanings, has been the focus of increasing attention in recent years. Nevertheless, there is a lack of large-scale normative studies on the iconic properties of words, which could prove crucial to expanding our understanding of form–meaning associations. In this work, we report subjective iconicity ratings for 10,995 visually presented Spanish words from 1350 participants who were asked to repeat each of the words aloud before rating them. The response reliability and the consistency between the present and previous ratings were good. The relationships between iconicity and several psycholinguistic variables were examined through multiple regression analyses. We found that sensory experience ratings were the main predictor of iconicity, and that early-acquired and more abstract words received higher iconicity scores. We also found that onomatopoeias and interjections were the most iconic words, followed by adjectives. Finally, a follow-up study was conducted in which a subsample of 360 words with different levels of iconicity from the visual presentation study was auditorily presented to the participants. A high correlation was observed between the iconicity scores in the visual and auditory presentations. The normative data provided in this database might prove useful in expanding the body of knowledge on issues such as the processing of the iconic properties of words and the role of word-form associations in the acquisition of vocabularies. The database can be downloaded from https://osf.io/v5er3/.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.



References
Adelman, J. S., Estes, Z., & Cossu, M. (2018). Emotional sound symbolism: Languages rapidly signal valence via phonemes. Cognition, 175, 122–130. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.02.007
Aguasvivas, J. A., Carreiras, M., Brysbaert, M., Mandera, P., Keuleers, E., & Duñabeitia, J. A. (2018). SPALEX: A Spanish lexical decision database from a massive online data collection. Frontiers in psychology, 9, 2156. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02156
Ahlner, F., & Zlatev, J. (2010). Cross-modal iconicity: A cognitive semiotic approach to sound symbolism. Sign Systems Studies, 38(1/4), 298-348. https://doi.org/10.12697/SSS.2010.38.1-4.11
Alonso, M. A., Fernandez, A., & Díez, E. (2015). Subjective age-of-acquisition norms for 7,039 Spanish words. Behavior Research Methods, 47, 268–274. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-014-0454-2
Aryani, A., Hsu, C. T., & Jacobs, A. M. (2019). Affective iconic words benefit from additional sound–meaning integration in the left amygdala. Human Brain Mapping, 40(18), 5289-5300. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24772
Blasi, D. E., Wichmann, S., Hammarström, H., Stadler, P. F. & Christiansen, M. H. (2016). Sound–meaning association biases evidenced across thousands of languages. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 113(39), 10818–10823. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1605782113
Brackbill, Y., & Little, K. B. (1957). Factors determining the guessing of meanings of foreign words. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 54(3), 312-318. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0042411
Brown, R. W., Black, A. H., & Horowitz, A. E. (1955). Phonetic symbolism in natural languages. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 50(3), 388-393. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0046820
Brysbaert, M., & New, B. (2009). Moving beyond Kučera and Francis: A critical evaluation of current word frequency norms and the introduction of a new and improved word frequency measure for American English. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 977-990. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.4.977
Brysbaert, M., Stevens, M., De Deyne, S., Voorspoels, W., & Storms, G. (2014). Norms of age of acquisition and concreteness for 30,000 Dutch words. Acta Psychologica, 150, 80-84. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.04.010
Carroll, J. B., & White, M. N. (1973). Word frequency and age of acquisition as determiners of picture naming latency. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 12(1), 85-95. https://doi.org/10.1080/14640747308400325
Cimpian, A., & Salomon, E. (2014). The inherence heuristic: An intuitive means of making sense of the world, and a potential precursor to psychological essentialism. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 37(5), 461-527. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X13002197
Ćoso, B., Guasch, M., Ferré, P., & Hinojosa, J. A. (2019). Affective and concreteness norms for 3,022 Croatian words. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72(9), 2302-2312. https://doi.org/10.1177/1747021819834226
Cuskley, C. (2013). Mappings between linguistic sound and motion. Public Journal of Semiotics, 5(1), 39-62. https://doi.org/10.37693/pjos.2013.5.9651
De Saussure, F. (2011). Course in general linguistics. Columbia University Press.
Davis, R. (1961). The fitness of names to drawings. A cross-cultural study in Tanganyika. British Journal of Psychology, 52(3), 259-268. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1961.tb00788.x
Della Rosa, P. A., Catricalà, E., Vigliocco, G., & Cappa, S. F. (2010). Beyond the abstract—concrete dichotomy: mode of acquisition, concreteness, imageability, familiarity, age of acquisition, context availability, and abstractness norms for a set of 417 Italian words. Behavior Research Methods, 42(4), 1042-1048. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.42.4.1042
Díez-Álamo, A. M., Díez, E., Wojcik, D. Z., Alonso, M. A., & Fernandez, A. (2019). Sensory experience ratings for 5,500 Spanish words. Behavior research methods, 51(3), 1205-1215. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-018-1057-0
Dingemanse, M. (2012). Advances in the cross-linguistic study of ideophones. Language and Linguistics compass, 6(10), 654-672. https://doi.org/10.1002/lnc3.361
Dingemanse, M. (2018). Redrawing the margins of language: Lessons from research on ideophones. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics, 3(1), 4. https://doi.org/10.5334/gjgl.444
Dingemanse, M., Blasi, D. E., Lupyan, G., Christiansen, M. H., & Monaghan, P. (2015). Arbitrariness, iconicity, and systematicity in language. Trends in cognitive sciences, 19(10), 603-615. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.07.013
Dingemanse, M., Perlman, M., & Perniss, P. (2020). Construals of iconicity: experimental approaches to form–meaning resemblances in language. Language and Cognition, 12(1), 1-14. https://doi.org/10.1017/langcog.2019.48
Dingemanse, M., Schuerman, W., Reinisch, E., Tufvesson, S., & Mitterer, H. (2016). What sound symbolism can and cannot do: Testing the iconicity of ideophones from five languages. Language, 92(2), e117-e133. https://doi.org/10.1353/lan
Duchon, A., Perea, M., Sebastián-Gallés, N., Martí, A., & Carreiras, M. (2013). EsPal: One-stop shopping for Spanish word properties. Behavior research methods, 45(4), 1246-1258. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-013-0326-1
Elsen, H. (2017). The two meanings of sound symbolism. Open Linguistics, 3(1), 491-499. https://doi.org/10.1515/opli-2017-0024
Ertel, S., & Dorst, R. (1965). Expressive Lautsymbolik. Zeitschrift für experimentelle und angewandte Psychologie, 12(4), 557-569.
Ferré, P., Guasch, M., Martínez-García, N., Fraga, I., & Hinojosa, J. A. (2017). Moved by words: Affective ratings for a set of 2,266 Spanish words in five discrete emotion categories. Behavior Research Methods, 49, 1082-1094. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-016-0768-3
Ferré, P., Guasch, M., Moldovan, C., & Sánchez-Casas, R. (2012). Affective norms for 380 Spanish words belonging to three different semantic categories. Behavior Research Methods, 44(2), 395-403. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-011-0165-x
Guasch, M., Ferré, P., & Fraga, I. (2016). Spanish norms for affective and lexico-semantic variables for 1,400 words. Behavior Research Methods, 48, 1358–1369. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-015-0684-y
Haro, J. (2012). Testmaker: Aplicación para crear cuestionarios online [Computer software and manual]. Retrieved from http://jharo.net/dokuwiki/testmaker
Haro, J., Ferré, P., Boada, R., & Demestre, J. (2017). Semantic ambiguity norms for 530 Spanish words. Applied Psycholinguistics, 38(2), 457-475. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716416000266
Hashimoto, T., Usui, N., Taira, M., Nose, I., Haji, T., & Kojima, S. (2006). The neural mechanism associated with the processing of onomatopoeic sounds. Neuroimage, 31(4), 1762-1770. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.02.019
Hinojosa, J. A., Martínez-García, N., Villalba-García, C., Fernández-Folgueiras, U., Sánchez-Carmona, A., Pozo, M. A., & Montoro, P. R. (2016a). Affective norms of 875 Spanish words for five discrete emotional categories and two emotional dimensions. Behavior Research Methods, 48, 272–284. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-015-0572-5
Hinojosa, J. A., Rincón-Pérez, I., Romero-Ferreiro, M. V., Martínez-García, N., Villalba-García, C., Montoro, P. R., & Pozo, M. A. (2016b). The Madrid Affective Database for Spanish (MADS): Ratings of Dominance, Familiarity, Subjective Age of Acquisition and Sensory Experience. PLoS ONE, 11(5): e0155866. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0155866
Imai M, Kita S. 2014 The sound symbolism bootstrapping hypothesis for language acquisition and language evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 369, 20130298. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0298
Juhasz, B. J., Yap, M. J., Dicke, J., Taylor, S. C., & Gullick, M. M. (2011). Tangible words are recognized faster: The grounding of meaning in sensory and perceptual systems. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(9), 1683–1691. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2011.605150
Juhasz, B. J., & Yap, M. J. (2013). Sensory experience ratings for over 5,000 mono-and disyllabic words. Behavior Research Methods, 45(1), 160-168. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-012-0242-9
Kanero, J., Imai, M., Okuda, J., Okada, H., & Matsuda, T. (2014). How sound symbolism is processed in the brain: a study on Japanese mimetic words. PLoS one, 9(5), e97905. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0097905
Köhler, W. (1929). Gestalt psychology. New York, NY: Liveright.
Köhler, W. (1947). Gestalt psychology: An introduction to new concepts in modern psychology. New York, NY: Liveright.
Koo, T. K., & Li, M. Y. (2016). A guideline of selecting and reporting intraclass correlation coefficients for reliability research. Journal of Chiropractic Medicine, 15(2), 155-163. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcm.2016.02.012
Kuperman, V., Stadthagen-Gonzalez, H., & Brysbaert, M. (2012). Age-of-acquisition ratings for 30,000 English words. Behavior Research Methods, 44(4), 978-990.
Laing, C. (2019). A role for onomatopoeia in early language: evidence from phonological development. Language and Cognition, 11(2), 173-187. https://doi.org/10.1017/langcog.2018.23
Lockwood, G. & Dingemanse, M. (2015) Iconicity in the lab: a review of behavioral, developmental, and neuroimaging research into sound-symbolism. Frontiers in Psychology, 6:1246. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01246
Lockwood, G., & Tuomainen, J. (2015). Ideophones in Japanese modulate the P2 and late positive complex responses. Frontiers in Psychology, 6:933. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.0093
Lupyan, G., & Winter, B. (2018). Language is more abstract than you think, or, why aren't languages more iconic?. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 373(1752), 20170137. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2017.0137
Meteyard, L., Stoppard, E., Snudden, D., Cappa, S. F., & Vigliocco, G. (2015). When semantics aids phonology: A processing advantage for iconic word forms in aphasia. Neuropsychologia, 76, 264-275. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.01.042
Miron, M. S. (1961). A crosslinguistic investigation of phonetic symbolism. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 62(3), 623-630. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0045212
Monnier, C., & Syssau, A. (2014). Affective norms for French words (FAN). Behavior Research Methods, 46(4), 1128-1137. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-013-0431-1
Motamedi, Y., Little, H., Nielsen, A., & Sulik, J. (2019). The iconicity toolbox: empirical approaches to measuring iconicity. Language and Cognition, 11(2), 188-207. https://doi.org/10.1017/langcog.2019.14
Nielsen, A. K., & Dingemanse, M. (2020). Iconicity in word learning and beyond: A critical review. Language and Speech, https://doi.org/10.1177/0023830920914339
Occhino, C., Anible, B., Wilkinson, E., & Morford, J. P. (2017). Iconicity is in the eye of the beholder: How language experience affects perceived iconicity. Gesture, 16(1), 100-126. https://doi.org/10.1075/gest.16.1.04occ
Oda, Hiromi. 2000. An Embodied Semantic Mechanism for Mimetic Words in Japanese. Ph.D dissertation. Bloomington: Indiana University.
Ozturk, O., Krehm, M., & Vouloumanos, A. (2013). Sound symbolism in infancy: Evidence for sound–shape cross-modal correspondences in 4-month-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114(2), 173-186. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.05.004
Paivio, A., Yuille, J. C., & Madigan, S. A. (1968). Concreteness, imagery, and meaningfulness values for 925 nouns. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 76(1, Pt.2), 1–25. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0025327
Peeters, D. (2016). Processing consequences of onomatopoeic iconicity in spoken language comprehension. In 38th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (CogSci 2016) (pp. 1632-1647). Cognitive Science Society
Perlman, M., Little, H., Thompson, B., & Thompson, R. L. (2018). Iconicity in Signed and Spoken Vocabulary: A Comparison Between American Sign Language, British Sign Language, English, and Spanish. Frontiers in psychology, 9, 1433. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01433
Perniss, P., & Vigliocco, G. (2014). The bridge of iconicity: from a world of experience to the experience of language. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 369(1651), 20130300. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0300
Perry, L. K., Perlman, M., & Lupyan, G. (2015). Iconicity in English and Spanish and its relation to lexical category and age of acquisition. PloS one, 10(9); e0137147 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0137147
Perry, L. K., Perlman, M., Winter, B., Massaro, D. W., & Lupyan, G. (2018). Iconicity in the speech of children and adults. Developmental Science, 21(3), e12572. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12572
Ramachandran, V. S., & Hubbard, E. M. (2001). Synaesthesia – A window into perception, thought and language. Journal of Consciouness Studies, 8(12), 3–34.
San Miguel Abella, R.A., & González-Nosti, M. (2020). Motor content norms for 4,565 verbs in Spanish. Behavior Research Methods, 52, 447–454. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-019-01241-1
Sapir, E. (1929). A study in phonetic symbolism. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 12(3), 225–239. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0070931
Schmidtke, D., & Conrad, M. (2018). Effects of affective phonological iconicity in online language processing: Evidence from a letter search task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 147(10), 1544–1552. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000499
Sidhu, D. M., & Pexman, P. M. (2018). Lonely sensational icons: semantic neighbourhood density, sensory experience and iconicity. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 33(1), 25-31. https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2017.1358379
Sidhu, D. M., Vigliocco, G., & Pexman, P. M. (2020). Effects of iconicity in lexical decision. Language and Cognition, 12(1), 164-181. https://doi.org/10.1017/langcog.2019.36
Soares, A. P., Costa, A. S., Machado, J., Comesaña, M., & Oliveira, H. M. (2017). The Minho Word Pool: Norms for imageability, concreteness, and subjective frequency for 3,800 Portuguese words. Behavior Research Methods, 49(3), 1065-1081. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-016-0767-4
Stadthagen-González, H., & Davis, C. J. (2006). The Bristol norms for age of acquisition, imageability, and familiarity. Behavior Research Methods, 38(4), 598-605. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193891
Stadthagen-González, H., Ferré, P., Pérez-Sánchez, M. A., Imbault, C., & Hinojosa, J. A. (2018). Norms for 10,491 Spanish words for five discrete emotions: Happiness, disgust, anger, fear, and sadness. Behavior research methods, 50(5), 1943–1952. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-017-0962-ydoi.org/10.3758/s13428-017-0962-y
Sutherland, S. L., & Cimpian, A. (2015). An explanatory heuristic gives rise to the belief that words are well suited for their referents. Cognition, 143, 228-240. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.07.002
Tanz, C. (1971). Sound symbolism in words relating to proximity and distance. Language and Speech, 14(3), 266-276. https://doi.org/10.1177/002383097101400307
Thompson, A. L., & Do, Y. (2019). Defining iconicity: An articulation-based methodology for explaining the phonological structure of ideophones. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics, 4(1), 72. https://doi.org/10.5334/gjgl.872
Taylor, I. K., & Taylor, M. M. (1962). Phonetic symbolism in four unrelated languages. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 16(4), 344-356. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0083261
Taylor, I. K., & Taylor, M. M. (1965). Another look at phonetic symbolism. Psychological bulletin, 64(6), 413-427. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0022737
Thompson, P. D., & Estes, Z. (2011). Sound symbolic naming of novel objects is a graded function. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(12), 2392-2404. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2011.605898
Thompson, R. L., Vinson, D. P., Woll, B., & Vigliocco, G. (2012). The road to language learning is iconic: Evidence from British Sign Language. Psychological science, 23(12), 1443-1448. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612459763
Vigliocco, G., & Kita, S. (2006). Language-specific properties of the lexicon: Implications for learning and processing. Language and Cognitive Processes, 21(7-8), 790–816. https://doi.org/10.1080/016909600824070
Vigliocco, G., Zhang, Y., Del Maschio, N., Todd, R., & Tuomainen, J. (2020). Electrophysiological signatures of English onomatopoeia. Language and Cognition, 12(1), 15-35. https://doi.org/10.1017/langcog.2019.38
Warriner, A. B., Kuperman, V., & Brysbaert, M. (2013). Norms of valence, arousal, and dominance for 13,915 English lemmas. Behavior research methods, 45(4), 1191-1207. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-012-0314-x
Winter, B., Perlman, M., Perry, L. K., & Lupyan, G. (2017). Which words are most iconic? Iconicity in English sensory words. Interaction Studies: Social Behaviour and Communication in Biological and Artificial Systems, 18(3), 443–464. https://doi.org/10.1075/is.18.3.07win
Xiao, W., & Treiman, R. (2012). Iconicity of simple Chinese characters. Behavior Research Methods, 44(4), 954-960. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-012-0191-3
Acknowledgements
This study was supported by the Spanish Ministerio de Ciencia, Innovación y Universidades (PGC2018-098558-B-I00, PGC2018-097145-B-I00, PID2019-107206GB-I00, and RED2018-102615-T), by the Comunidad de Madrid (H2019/HUM-5705) and by the Universitat Rovira i Virgili (2018PFR-URV-B2-32). We thank Bodo Winter and an anonymous reviewer for their highly valuable comments on this manuscript.
Author information
Affiliations
Corresponding author
Additional information
Open practice statement
The data and materials for all experiments are available in the following link: https://osf.io/v5er3/
Publisher’s note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Appendix
Appendix
Instructions for iconicity ratings (Spanish)
En este cuestionario usted deberá calificar distintas palabras en castellano en función de hasta qué punto el sonido de las palabras se parece a su significado.
Déjenos explicarle algo más sobre esto. Algunas palabras en castellano tienen un sonido que se parece a su significado. Por ejemplo, la palabra RONCAR suena como el ruido que alguien hace al respirar mientras duerme. La onomatopeya ¡PLAS! suena como el ruido que un objeto hace al caer al suelo. Otro ejemplo es la palabra CHICO, que suena como algo pequeño (en comparación con GIGANTE, que suena grande). Estas palabras son icónicas. Podría adivinar qué significan aunque no supiera hablar castellano. Otras palabras no son icónicas, es decir, son arbitrarias. Por ejemplo, no hay nada que suene canino o felino en las palabras PERRO y GATO. Si usted no supiera castellano, no podría adivinar su significado.
Para cada palabra que vea, califique en una escala del 1 al 7 cuán icónica cree que es, tomando 1 como muy arbitraria (es decir, el sonido de la palabra no tiene nada que ver con su significado) y 7 como muy icónica (es decir, el sonido de la palabra está muy relacionado con su significado). Antes de otorgarle una puntuación, repítase la palabra en voz alta, con el objetivo de prestar verdadera atención a cómo suena y compararlo con su significado. Siéntase libre de usar todo el rango de números, del 1 al 7; al mismo tiempo, no se preocupe por la frecuencia con la que usa un número en particular, siempre y cuando sea honesto en sus calificaciones. Si no conoce el significado de una palabra, elija la opción “No sé el significado de esta palabra”. Trabaje con bastante rapidez, pero no sea descuidado en sus calificaciones, lo importante es que sea lo más preciso posible. Recuerde que no hay respuestas buenas o malas, ya que le estamos preguntando por su impresión acerca de las palabras.
Instructions for the iconicity questionnaires (English translation)
In this questionnaire you will be asked to rate the extent to which the sound of different Spanish words resembles their meaning.
Let us explain this a little further. Some Spanish words have a sound that resembles their meaning. For example, the word RONCAR (“SNORE”) sounds like the noise someone makes when they breathe in their sleep. The onomatopoeia ¡PLAS! sounds like the noise an object makes when it falls to the ground. Another example is the word CHICO (“SMALL”), which sounds like something small (compared to GIGANTE (“GIANT”), which sounds big). These words are iconic. You could guess what they mean even if you didn’t speak Spanish. Other words are not iconic, that is, they are arbitrary. For example, there is nothing that sounds canine or feline in the words PERRO (“DOG”) or GATO (“CAT”). If you didn’t speak Spanish, you wouldn’t be able to guess their meaning.
Rate on a scale of 1 to 7 how iconic you think each word is, taking 1 as very arbitrary (i.e., the sound of the word is not related at all to its meaning) and 7 as very iconic (i.e., the sound of the word is very related to its meaning). Before giving a rating, please repeat the word aloud to pay close attention to how it sounds and to compare its sound to its meaning. Feel free to use the entire range of values, from 1 to 7. Don’t worry about how often you use a particular value, as long as you are accurate in your ratings. If you don’t know the meaning of a word, choose the “I don’t know the meaning of this word” option. Try to answer quickly, but don’t be careless with your ratings—it is important to be as accurate as possible. Remember that there are no right or wrong answers, because we are asking you about your impression of the words.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Hinojosa, J.A., Haro, J., Magallares, S. et al. Iconicity ratings for 10,995 Spanish words and their relationship with psycholinguistic variables. Behav Res (2020). https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-020-01496-z
Accepted:
Published:
Keywords
- Iconicity
- Sound-symbolism
- Concreteness
- SERs
- Subjective AoA