Advertisement

Behavior Research Methods

, Volume 47, Issue 3, pp 720–735 | Cite as

Emotional connotations of words related to authority and community

  • Gesche SchauenburgEmail author
  • Jens Ambrasat
  • Tobias Schröder
  • Christian von Scheve
  • Markus Conrad
Article

Abstract

We present a database of 858 German words from the semantic fields of authority and community, which represent core dimensions of human sociality. The words were selected on the basis of co-occurrence profiles of representative keywords for these semantic fields. All words were rated along five dimensions, each measured by a bipolar semantic-differential scale: Besides the classic dimensions of affective meaning (valence, arousal, and potency), we collected ratings of authority and community with newly developed scales. The results from cluster, correlational, and multiple regression analyses on the rating data suggest a robust negativity bias for authority valuation among German raters recruited via university mailing lists, whereas community ratings appear to be rather unrelated to the well-established affective dimensions. Furthermore, our data involve a strong overall negative correlation—rather than the classical U-shaped distribution—between valence and arousal for socially relevant concepts. Our database provides a valuable resource for research questions at the intersection of cognitive neuroscience and social psychology. It can be downloaded as supplemental materials with this article.

Keywords

Affective word database Ratings Authority Community Valence Arousal Potency 

Notes

Author note

This research was funded by a grant from the Cluster of Excellence "Languages of Emotion" at Freie Universität Berlin to C.v.S., T.S., and M.C. (project #411) as part of the Excellence Initiative of the German Research Foundation (DFG).

Supplementary material

13428_2014_494_MOESM1_ESM.xlsx (367 kb)
ESM 1 (XLSX 367 kb)

References

  1. Altarriba, J., & Basnight-Brown, D. M. (2011). The representation of emotion vs. emotion-laden words in English and Spanish in the Affective Simon Task. International Journal of Bilingualism, 15, 310–328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Ambrasat, J., von Scheve, C., Conrad, M., Schauenburg, G., & Schröder, T. (2014). Consensus and stratification in the affective meaning of human sociality. PNAS. Advance online publication. doi: 10.107/pnas.131321111
  3. Baayen, R. H., Piepenbrock, R., & Gulikers, L. (1995). The CELEX lexical database, Release 2 (CD-ROM). Philadelphia, PA: Linguistic Data Consortium, University of Pennsylvania.Google Scholar
  4. Balota, D. A., Yap, M. J., Cortese, M. J., Hutchinson, K. A., Kessler, B., Loftis, B., & Treiman, R. (2007). The English Lexicon Project. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 445–459. doi: 10.3758/BF03193014 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Belica, C. (1995). Statistische Kollokationsanalyse und -clustering. Korpuslinguistische Analysemethode. Mannheim, Germany: Institut für Deutsche Sprache.Google Scholar
  6. Belica, C. (2001). Kookkurrenzdatenbank CCDB. Eine korpuslinguistische Denk- und Experimentierplattform für die Erforschung und theoretische Begründung von systemisch-strukturellen Eigenschaften von Kohäsionsrelationen zwischen den Konstituenten des Sprachgebrauchs. Mannheim, Germany: Institut für Deutsche Sprache.Google Scholar
  7. Belica, C. (2011). Semantische Nähe als Ähnlichkeit von Kookkurrenzprofilen. In A. Abel & R. Zanin (Eds.), Korpora in Lehre und Forschung (pp. 155–178). Bozen-Bolzano, Germany: University Press of the Freie Universität Bozen-Bolzano.Google Scholar
  8. Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1999). Affective norms for English words (ANEW): Stimuli, instruction manual and affective ratings (Technical Report No. C-1). Gainesville, FL: University of Florida, NIMH Center for Research in Psychophysiology.Google Scholar
  9. Brysbaert, M., Buchmeier, M., Conrad, M., Jacobs, A. M., Bölte, J., & Böhl, A. (2011). The word frequency effect: A review of recent developments and implications for the choice of frequency estimates in German. Experimental Psychology, 58, 412–424. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000123 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Brysbaert, M., & New, B. (2009). Moving beyond Kučera and Francis: A critical evaluation of current word frequency norms and the introduction of a new and improved word frequency measure for American English. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 977–990. doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.977 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Cai, Q., & Brysbaert, M. (2010). SUBTLEX-CH: Chinese word and character frequencies based on film subtitles. PLoS ONE, 5(e10729), 1–8. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0010729 Google Scholar
  12. Citron, F. M. (2012). Neural correlates of written emotion word processing: A review of recent electrophysiological and hemodynamic neuroimaging studies. Brain and Language, 122, 211–226.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Conrad, M., Recio, G., & Jacobs, A. M. (2011). The time course of emotion effects in first and second language processing: Across cultural ERP study with German–Spanish bilinguals. Frontiers in Psychology, 2(351), 1–16. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00351 Google Scholar
  14. Cuetos, F., Glez-Nosti, M., Barbón, A., & Brysbaert, M. (2011). SUBTLEX-ESP: Spanish word frequencies based on film subtitles. Psicológica, 32, 133–143.Google Scholar
  15. Dimitropoulou, M., Duñabeitia, J. A., Avilés, A., Corral, J., & Carreiras, M. (2010). Subtitle-based word frequencies as the best estimate of reading behavior: The case of Greek. Frontiers in Language Sciences, 1(218), 1–12. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00218 Google Scholar
  16. Doerksen, S., & Shimamura, A. P. (2001). Source memory enhancement for emotional words. Emotion, 1, 5–11.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Eilola, T. M., & Havelka, J. (2010). Affective norms for 210 British English and Finnish nouns. Behavior Research Methods, 42, 134–140. doi: 10.3758/BRM.42.1.134 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Ferré, P., Guasch, M., Moldovan, C., & Sánchez-Casas, R. (2012). Affective norms for 380 Spanish words belonging to three different semantic categories. Behavior Research Methods, 44, 395–403. doi: 10.3758/s13428-011-0165-x CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Fiske, A. P. (1992). The four elementary forms of sociality: Framework for a unified theory of social relations. Psychological Review, 99, 689–723. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.99.4.689 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Grimm, S., Weigand, A., Kazzer, P., Jacobs, A. M., & Bajbouj, M. (2012). Neural mechanisms underlying the integration of emotion and working memory. NeuroImage, 61, 1188–1194.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Hamann, S., & Mao, H. (2002). Positive and negative emotional verbal stimuli elicit activity in the left amygdala. NeuroReport, 13, 15–19.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Heise, D. R. (2007). Expressive order: Confirming sentiments in social actions. New York, NY: Springer.Google Scholar
  23. Heise, D. R. (2010). Surveying cultures: Discovering shared conceptions and sentiments. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
  24. Heister, J., Würzner, K.-M., Bubenzer, J., Pohl, E., Hanneforth, T., Geyken, A., & Kliegl, R. (2011). dlexDB—Eine lexikalische Datenbank für die psychologische und linguistische Forschung. Psychologische Rundschau, 62, 10–20. doi: 10.1026/0033-3042/a000029 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Herbert, C., Ethofer, T., Anders, S., Junghofer, M., Wildgruber, D., Grodd, W., & Kissler, J. (2009). Amygdala activation during reading of emotional adjectives—An advantage for pleasant content. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 4, 35–49.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Hofmann, M. J., Kuchinke, L., Tamm, S., Võ, M. L.-H., & Jacobs, A. M. (2009). Affective processing within 1/10th of a second: High arousal is necessary for early facilitative processing of negative but not positive words. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 9, 389–397. doi: 10.3758/9.4.389 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Huckauf, A., Heller, D., & Gouzouli-Mayfrank, E. (2003). Ein Instrument zur Messung der Wirkung emotionaler Valenzen auf die Wortverarbeitung. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 211, 129–137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Keibel, H. & Belica, C. (2007). CCDB: A corpus-linguistic research and development workbench. In M. Davies, P. Rayson, S. Hunston, P. Daniellson (Eds.), Proceedings of the 4th Corpus Linguistics Conference. Retrieved May 6, 2014, from http://ucrel.lancs.ac.uk/publications/CL2007/paper/134_Paper.pdf
  29. Kemper, T. D. (1978). A social interactional theory of emotions. New York, NY: Wiley.Google Scholar
  30. Kensinger, E. A., & Corkin, S. (2003). Memory enhancement for emotional words: Are emotional words more vividly remembered than neutral words? Memory & Cognition, 31, 1169–1180. doi: 10.3758/BF03195800 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Keuleers, E., Brysbaert, M., & New, B. (2010). SUBTLEX-NL: A new frequency measure for Dutch words based on film subtitles. Behavior Research Methods, 42, 643–650. doi: 10.3758/BRM.42.3.643 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Kissler, J., & Koessler, S. (2011). Emotionally positive stimuli facilitate lexical decisions—An ERP study. Biological Psychology, 86, 254–264.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Kousta, S.-T., Vinson, D. P., & Vigliocco, G. (2009). Emotion words, regardless of polarity, have a processing advantage over neutral words. Cognition, 112, 473–481. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.06.007 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Kuchinke, L., Jacobs, A. M., Grubich, C., Võ, M. L.-H., Conrad, M., & Herrmann, M. (2005). Incidental effects of emotional valence in single word processing: An fMRI study. NeuroImage, 28, 1022–1032.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Kuchinke, L., Võ, M. L.-H., Hofmann, M., & Jacobs, A. M. (2007). Pupillary responses during lexical decisions vary with word frequency but not emotional valence. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 65, 132–140. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2007.04.004 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Kuperman, V., Estes, Z., Brysbaert, M., & Warriner, A. B. (2014). Emotion and language: Valence and arousal affect word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. doi: 10.1037/a0035669. Advance online publication.Google Scholar
  37. Malhi, G., Lagopoulos, J., Sachdev, P., Ivanovski, B., & Shnier, R. (2005). An emotional Stroop functional MRI study of euthymic bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disorders, 7, 58–69.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Montefinese, M., Ambrosini, E., Fairfield, B., & Mammarella, N. (2013). The adaption of the Affective Norms for English Words (ANEW) for Italian. Behavior Research Methods. doi: 10.3758/s13428-013-0405-3. Advance online publication.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Nisbet, R. A. (1966). The sociological tradition. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction.Google Scholar
  40. Osgood, C. E. (1962). Studies on the generality of affective meaning systems. American Psychologist, 17, 10–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Osgood, C. E., Suci, G., & Tannenbaum, P. (1957). The measurement of meaning. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois.Google Scholar
  42. Recio, G., Conrad, M., Hansen, L. B., & Jacobs, A. M. (2014). On pleasure and thrill: The interplay between arousal and valence during visual word recognition. Brain and Language, 134, 34–43. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2014.03.009 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Redondo, J., Fraga, I., Comesaña, M., & Perea, M. (2005). Estudio normativo del valor afectivo de 478 palabras españolas. Psicológica, 26, 317–326.Google Scholar
  44. Redondo, J., Fraga, I., Padrón, I., & Comesaña, M. (2007). The Spanish adaptation of ANEW (Affective Norms for English Words). Behavior Research Methods, 39, 600–605. doi: 10.3758/BF03193031 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Russell, J. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 1161–1178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Russell, J. A., & Mehrabian, A. (1977). Evidence for a three-factor theory of emotions. Journal of Research in Personality, 11, 273–294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Sass, S. M., Heller, W., Stewart, J. L., Levin Silton, R., Egdar, J. C., Fisher, J. E., & Miller, G. A. (2010). Time course of attentional bias in anxiety: Emotion and gender specificity. Psychophysiology, 47, 247–259.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Schacht, A., & Sommer, W. (2009). Time course and task dependence of emotion effects in word processing. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 9, 28–43. doi: 10.3758/CABN.9.1.28 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Schmidtke, D. S., Schröder, T., Jacobs, A. M., & Conrad, M. (2014). ANGST: Affective norms for German sentiment terms, derived from the Affective Norms of English Words. Behavior Research Methods. Advance online publication. doi: 10.3758/s13428-013-0426-y
  50. Schneider, A. (2004). The ideal type of authority in the United States and Germany. Sociological Perspectives, 47, 313–327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Scholl, W. (2013). The socio-emotional basis of human interaction and communication: How we construct our social world. Social Science Information, 52, 3–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Schröder, T. (2011). A model of language-based impression formation and attribution among Germans. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 30, 82–102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Schröder, T., & Thagard, P. (2013). The affective meanings of automatic social behaviors: Three mechanisms that explain priming. Psychological Review, 120, 255–280. doi: 10.1037/a0030972 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. Sewell, A. A., & Heise, D. R. (2010). Racial differences in sentiments: Exploring variant cultures. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 34, 400–412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Silva, C., Montant, M., Ponz, A., & Ziegler, J. C. (2012). Emotions in reading: Disgust, empathy and the contextual learning hypothesis. Cognition, 125, 333–338.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Soares, A. P., Comesaña, M., Pinheiro, A. P., Simões, A., & Frade, C. S. (2012). The adaptation of the Affective Norms for English Words (ANEW) for European Portuguese. Behavior Research Methods, 44, 256–269. doi: 10.3758/s13428-011-0131-7 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  57. Tabert, M. H., Borod, J. C., Tang, C. Y., Lange, G., Wei, T. C., Johnson, R., & Buchsbaum, M. S. (2001). Differential amygdala activation during emotional decision and recognition memory tasks using unpleasant words: An fMRI study. Neuropsychologia, 39, 556–573.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  58. Thelwall, M., & Kappas, A. (2014). The role of sentiment in the social web. In C. von Scheve & M. Salmela (Eds.), Collective emotions (pp. 375–388). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Van Heuven, W. J. B., Mandera, P., Keuleers, E., & Brysbaert, M. (2014). SUBTLEX-UK: A new and improved word frequency database for British English. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67, 1176–1190. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2013.850521 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Võ, M. L.-H., Conrad, M., Kuchinke, L., Urton, K., Hofmann, M. J., & Jacobs, A. M. (2009). The Berlin Affective Word List Reloaded (BAWL-R). Behavior Research Methods, 41, 534–538. doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.2.534 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  61. Võ, M. L.-H., Jacobs, A. M., & Conrad, M. (2006). Cross-validating the Berlin Affective Word List. Behavior Research Methods, 38, 606–609. doi: 10.3758/BF03193892 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  62. Võ, M. L.-H., Jacobs, A. M., Kuchinke, L., Hofmann, M., Conrad, M., Schacht, A., & Hutzler, F. (2008). The coupling of emotion and cognition in the eye: Introducing the pupil old/new effect. Psychophysiology, 45, 130–140. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00606.x CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  63. Warriner, A. B., Kuperman, V., & Brysbaert, M. (2013). Norms of valence, arousal, and dominance for 13,915 English lemmas. Behavior Research Methods, 45, 1191–1207. doi: 10.3758/s13428-012-0314-x CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  64. Weigand, A., Grimm, S., Astalosch, A., Guo, J. S., Briesemeister, B. B., Lisanby, S. H., & Bajbouj, M. (2013). Lateralized effects of prefrontal repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation on emotional working memory. Experimental Brain Research, 227, 43–52.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  65. Wortschatz Universität Leipzig (2013). Retrieved May 7, 2013, from http://wortschatz.uni-leipzig.de
  66. Wundt, W. (1896). Gundriss der Psychologie. Leipzig, Germany: Entgelmann.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  • Gesche Schauenburg
    • 1
    Email author
  • Jens Ambrasat
    • 2
  • Tobias Schröder
    • 3
  • Christian von Scheve
    • 2
  • Markus Conrad
    • 4
  1. 1.Department of General and Neurocognitive Psychology and Cluster “Languages of Emotion”Freie Universität BerlinBerlinGermany
  2. 2.Department of Sociology and Cluster “Languages of Emotion”Freie Universität BerlinBerlinGermany
  3. 3.Institute for Urban FuturesPotsdam University of Applied SciencesPotsdamGermany
  4. 4.Department of Cognitive PsychologyUniversidad de La LagunaLa Laguna, Spain

Personalised recommendations