Abstract
Attentional lapses have been found to impair everything from basic perception to learning and memory. Yet, despite the well-documented costs of lapses on cognition, recent work suggests that lapses might unexpectedly confer some benefits. One potential benefit is that lapses broaden our learning to integrate seemingly irrelevant content that could later prove useful—a benefit that prior research focusing only on goal-relevant memory would miss. Here, we measure how fluctuations in sustained attention influence the learning of seemingly goal-irrelevant content that competes for attention with target content. Participants completed a correlated flanker task in which they categorized central targets (letters or numbers) while ignoring peripheral flanking symbols that shared hidden probabilistic relationships with the targets. We found that across participants, higher rates of attentional lapses correlated with greater learning of the target–flanker relationships. Moreover, within participants, learning was more evident during attentional lapses. These findings address long-standing theoretical debates and reveal a benefit of attentional lapses: they expand the scope of learning and decisions beyond the strictly relevant.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.



References
Amer, T., Campbell, K. L., & Hasher, L. (2016). Cognitive Control As a Double-Edged Sword. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(12), 905–915.
Amer, T., & Hasher, L. (2014). Conceptual Processing of Distractors by Older but Not Younger Adults. Psychological Science, 25(12), 2252–2258.
Ariga, A., & Lleras, A. (2011). Brief and rare mental “breaks” keep you focused: Deactivation and reactivation of task goals preempt vigilance decrements. Cognition, 118(3), 439–443.
Awh, E., Vogel, E. K., Hammer, R., Diesendruck, G., Weinshall, D., Hochstein, S., et al. (2008). The bouncer in the brain. Nature Neuroscience, 11(1), 749–767.
Biss, R. K., Rowe, G., Weeks, J. C., Hasher, L., & Murphy, K. J. (2018). Leveraging older adults’ susceptibility to distraction to improve memory for face-name associations. Psychology and Aging, 33(1), 158–164.
Campbell, K. L., Hasher, L., & Thomas, R. C. (2010). Hyper-Binding. Psychological Science, 21(3), 399–405.
Campbell, K. L., Healey, M. K., Lee, M. M. S., Zimerman, S., & Hasher, L. (2012). Brief report: Age differences in visual statistical learning. Psychology and Aging, 27(3), 650–656.
Carlson, K. A., & Flowers, J. H. (1996). Intentional versus unintentional use of contingencies between perceptual events. Percept Psychophys, 58(3), 460–470.
Champely, S. (2020). pwr: Basic Functions for Power Analysis. Retrieved from https://cran.r-project.org/package=pwr. Accessed 19 Nov 2020.
Cools, R., Sheridan, M., Jacobs, E., & D’Esposito, M. (2007). Impulsive personality predicts dopamine-dependent changes in frontostriatal activity during component processes of working memory. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(20), 5506–5514.
Curran-Everett, D. (2017). CORP: Minimizing the chances of false positives and false negatives. Journal of Applied Physiology, 122(1), 91–95.
deBettencourt, M. T., Keene, P. A., Awh, E., & Vogel, E. K. (2019). Real-time triggering reveals concurrent lapses of attention and working memory. Nature Human Behaviour. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-019-0606-6
DeBettencourt, M. T., Norman, K. A., & Turk-Browne, N. B. (2018). Forgetting from lapses of sustained attention. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 25(2), 605–611.
Decker, A., Finn, A., & Duncan, K. (2020). Errors lead to transient impairments in memory formation. Cognition, 204, 1–58.
Decker, A. L., & Duncan, K. (2020). Acetylcholine and the complex interdependence of memory and attention. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 32, 21–28.
Deng, W., & Sloutsky, V. M. (2016). Selective attention, diffused attention, and the development of categorization. Cognitive Psychology, 91, 24–62.
Duncan, K., Semmler, A., & Shohamy, D. (2019). Modulating the Use of Multiple Memory Systems in Value-based Decisions with Contextual Novelty. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 31(10), 1455–1467.
Esterman, M., Noonan, S. K., Rosenberg, M., & Degutis, J. (2012). In the zone or zoning out? Tracking behavioral and neural fluctuations during sustained attention. Cerebral Cortex, 23(11), 2712–2723.
Esterman, M., Rosenberg, M. D., & Noonan, S. K. (2014). Intrinsic fluctuations in sustained attention and distractor processing. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(5), 1724–1730.
Esterman, M., & Rothlein, D. (2019). Models of sustained attention. Current Opinion in Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.03.005
Foerde, K., Knowlton, B. J., & Poldrack, R. A. (2006). Modulation of competing memory systems by distraction. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103(31), 11778–11783.
Foerde, K., Poldrack, R. A., & Knowlton, B. J. (2007). Secondary-task effects on classification learning. Memory and Cognition, 35(5), 864–874.
Fortenbaugh, F. C., Degutis, J., & Esterman, M. (2017). Recent theoretical, neural, and clinical advances in sustained attention research. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1396, 70–91.
Gabrieli, J. D. E. (1998). Cognitive Neuroscience of Human Memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 49(1), 87–115.
Kalra, P. B., Gabrieli, J. D. E., & Finn, A. S. (2019). Evidence of stable individual differences in implicit learning. Cognition, 190(May), 199–211.
Killingsworth, M. A., & Gilbert, D. T. (2010). A wandering mind is an unhappy mind. Science, 330(6006), 932.
Kim, S., Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (2007). Aging and a benefit of distractibility. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 14(2), 301–305.
Klatt, L. I., Schneider, D., Schubert, A. L., Hanenberg, C., Lewald, J., Wascher, E., & Getzmann, S. (2019). Unraveling the relation between eeg correlates of attentional orienting and sound localization performance: A diffusion model approach. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 32(5), 945–962.
Landau, A. N., Elwan, D., Holtz, S., & Prinzmetal, W. (2012). Voluntary and involuntary attention vary as a function of impulsivity. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19(3), 405–411.
Lavie, N., Hirst, A., De Fockert, J. W., & Viding, E. (2004). Load theory of selective attention and cognitive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133(3), 339–354.
Logan, G. D., Schachar, R. J., & Tannock, R. (1997). Impulsivity and Inhibitory Control. Psychological Science, 8(1), 60–64.
Mackworth, N. H. (1948). The Breakdown of Vigilance during Prolonged Visual Search. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1(1), 6–21.
Madore, K. P., Khazenzon, A. M., Backes, C. W., Jiang, J., Uncapher, M. R., Norcia, A. M., & Wagner, A. D. (2020). Memory failure predicted by attention lapsing and media multitasking. Nature, 587(7832), 87–91.
Manly, T., Robertson, I. H., Galloway, M., & Hawkins, K. (1999). The absent mind: Further investigations of sustained attention to response. Neuropsychologia, 37(6), 661–670.
McVay, J. C., Kane, M. J., & Kwapil, T. R. (2009). Tracking the train of thought from the laboratory into everyday life: An experience-sampling study of mind wandering across controlled and ecological contexts. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 16(5), 857–863.
Miller, J. (1987). Priming is not necessary for selective-attention failures: Semantic effects of unattended, unprimed letters. Perception And Psychophysics, 41(5), 419–434.
Mittner, M., Hawkins, G. E., Boekel, W., & Forstmann, B. U. (2016). A Neural Model of Mind Wandering. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(8), 570–578.
Morey, R. D. (2008). Confidence Intervals from Normalized Data: A correction to Cousineau (2005). Tutorials in Quantitative Methods for. Psychology, 4(2), 61–64.
Myers, C. E., Shohamy, D., Gluck, M. A., Grossman, S., Onlaor, S., & Kapur, N. (2003). Dissociating medial temporal and basal ganglia memory systems with a latent learning task. Neuropsychologia, 41(14), 1919–1928.
Ngo, K. W. J., Biss, R. K., & Hasher, L. (2018). Time of day effects on the use of distraction to minimise forgetting. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71(11), 2334–2341.
Peirce, J. W. (2007). PsychoPy-Psychophysics software in Python. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 162(1–2), 8–13.
Per, B., Bojesen, R. H., & Version, D. (2017). lmerTest Package: Tests in Linear Mixed Effects. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v082.i13
Plebanek, D. J., & Sloutsky, V. M. (2017). Costs of Selective Attention: When Children Notice What Adults Miss. Psychological Science, 28(6), 723–732.
Poldrack, R. A., Clark, J., Pare-Blagoev, E., Shohamy, D., Moyano, J. C., Myers, C., et al. (2001). Interactive memory systems in the human brain. Nature, 414(6863), 546–550.
R Core Team. (2019). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Retrieved from https://www.r-project.org/. Accessed 19 Nov 2022.
Riley, E., Esterman, M., Fortenbaugh, F. C., & DeGutis, J. (2017). Time-of-day variation in sustained attentional control. Chronobiology International, 34(7), 993–1001.
Robertson, I. H., Manly, T., Andrade, J., Baddeley, B. T., & Yiend, J. (1997). `Oops!’: Performance correlates of everyday attentional failures in traumatic brain injured and normal subjects. Neuropsychologia, 35(6), 747–758.
Rosenberg, M., Noonan, S., DeGutis, J., & Esterman, M. (2013). Sustaining visual attention in the face of distraction: A novel gradual-onset continuous performance task. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 75(3), 426–439.
Rowe, G., Valderrama, S., Hasher, L., & Lenartowicz, A. (2006). Attentional disregulation: A benefit for implicit memory. Psychology and Aging, 21(4), 826–830.
Satterthwaite, T. D., Ruparel, K., Loughead, J., Elliott, M. A., Gerraty, R. T., Calkins, M. E., et al. (2012). Being right is its own reward: Load and performance related ventral striatum activation to correct responses during a working memory task in youth. NeuroImage, 61(3), 723–729.
Schacter, D. L., & Tulving, E. (Eds.). (1994). Memory systems 1994. Memory systems 1994. The MIT Press.
Schmitz, T. W., Cheng, F. H. T., & De Rosa, E. (2010). Failing to ignore: Paradoxical neural effects of perceptual load on early attentional selection in normal aging. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(44), 14750–14758.
Schooler, J. W., Smallwood, J., Christoff, K., Handy, T. C., Reichle, E. D., & Sayette, M. A. (2011). Meta-awareness, perceptual decoupling and the wandering mind. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(7), 319–326.
Seli, P., Beaty, R. E., Cheyne, J. A., Smilek, D., Oakman, J., & Schacter, D. L. (2018). How pervasive is mind wandering, really? Consciousness and Cognition, 66(May), 74–78.
Shohamy, D., Myers, C. E., Hopkins, R. O., Sage, J., & Gluck, M. A. (2009). Distinct hippocampal and basal ganglia contributions to probabilistic learning and reversal. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(9), 1821–1833.
Shomstein, S., Malcolm, G. L., & Nah, J. C. (2019). Intrusive effects of task-irrelevant information on visual selective attention: Semantics and size. Current Opinion in Psychology, 29, 153–159.
Smallwood, J. (2010). Why the Global Availability of Mind Wandering Necessitates Resource Competition: Reply to McVay and Kane (2010). Psychological Bulletin, 136(2), 202–207.
Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2006). The restless mind. Psychological Bulletin, 132(6), 946–958.
Stanley, M. L., Whitehead, P. S., Marsh, E. J., & Seli, P. (2022). Prior exposure increases judged truth even during periods of mind wandering. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, (0123456789). https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-022-02101-4
Sun, M., Wang, E., Huang, J., Zhao, C., Guo, J., Li, D., et al. (2018). Attentional selection and suppression in children and adults. Developmental Science, 21(6), 1–13.
Thompson-Schill, S. L., Ramscar, M., & Chrysikou, E. G. (2009). Cognition without control: When a little frontal lobe goes a long way. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(5), 259–263.
Thomson, D. R., Besner, D., & Smilek, D. (2015). A Resource-Control Account of Sustained Attention: Evidence from Mind-Wandering and Vigilance Paradigms. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(1), 82–96.
Unsworth, N., Fukuda, K., Awh, E., & Vogel, E. K. (2014). Working memory and fluid intelligence: Capacity, attention control, and secondary memory retrieval. Cognitive Psychology, 71, 1–26.
Unsworth, N., Redick, T. S., Heitz, R. P., Broadway, J. M., & Engle, R. W. (2009). Complex working memory span tasks and higher-order cognition: A latent-variable analysis of the relationship between processing and storage. Memory, 17(6), 635–654.
Unsworth, N., & Robison, M. K. (2016). The influence of lapses of attention on working memory capacity. Memory and Cognition, 44(2), 188–196.
Unsworth, N., & Robison, M. K. (2018). Tracking arousal state and mind wandering with pupillometry. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 18(4), 638–664.
Unsworth, N., & Robison, M. K. (2020). Working memory capacity and sustained attention: A cognitive-energetic perspective. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 46(1), 77–103.
Wagner, A. D., Maril, A., & Schacter, D. L. (2000). Interactions Between Forms of Memory: When Priming Hinders New Episodic Learning. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(supplement 2), 52–60.
Wamsley, E. J., & Summer, T. (2020). Spontaneous Entry into an “Offline” State during Wakefulness: A Mechanism of Memory Consolidation? Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 1–21. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01587
Weeks, J. C., Biss, R. K., Murphy, K. J., & Hasher, L. (2016). Face–name learning in older adults: A benefit of hyper-binding. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 23(5), 1559–1565.
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank Henrique Matulis for assistance programming the experiment, and Kiara Lu, Theresa Pham, Danielle Lim, Noel Yuen, Lavania Jeganathan, Christa Shi, and Luosha Ha for their help with data collection. This research was supported by an Ontario Graduate Scholarship and an Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC) graduate fellowship to M.D., an Ontario Graduate Scholarship and Brain Canada Kids Brain Network Scholarship to A.D., NSERC Discovery Grants (A.S.F.:RGPIN-2016-05; KD:RGPIN/05582-201) and Canada Foundation for Innovation and Ontario Research Foundation (A.S.F.: 34947; K.D.:34479).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
M.D., A.D., K.D., and A.S.F. designed research; M.D. performed the research. A.D. and M.D. analyzed data; M.D., A.D., K.D., and A.S.F. wrote the paper.
Corresponding authors
Ethics declarations
Competing Interest
The authors declare no competing interest.
Additional information
Open Practices Statements
The datasets and code generated during and/or analyzed during the current study are available at the following link:
https://github.com/alexandradecker/learning-more-when-attending-less.git. The stimuli and experiment code can be found at this link: https://github.com/FinnLandLab/learning-more-when-attending-less. The experiment was not pre-registered.
Publisher’s note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
ESM 1
(DOCX 518 kb)
Rights and permissions
Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.
About this article
Cite this article
Decker, A., Dubois, M., Duncan, K. et al. Pay attention and you might miss it: Greater learning during attentional lapses. Psychon Bull Rev 30, 1041–1052 (2023). https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-022-02226-6
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-022-02226-6