Althoff, R. R., & Cohen, N. J. (1999). Eye-movement-based memory effect: A reprocessing effect in face perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25(4), 997–1010. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.25.4.997
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D., & Dale, H. C. A. (1966). The effect of semantic similarity on retroactive interference in long- and short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 5(5), 417–420. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(66)80054-3
Article
Google Scholar
Bainbridge, W. A. (2019). Memorability: How what we see influences what we remember. Psychology of Learning and Motivation—Advances in Research and Theory, 70, 1–27. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.plm.2019.02.001
Bainbridge, W. A. (2020). The resiliency of image memorability: A predictor of memory separate from attention and priming. Neuropsychologia, 141(June 2019), 107408. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2020.107408
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B. M., & Walker, S. C. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1–48. 10.18637/jss.v067.i01
Bayley, P. J., Wixted, J. T., Hopkins, R. O., & Squire, L. R. (2008). Yes/no recognition, forced-choice recognition, and the human hippocampus. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(3), 505–512. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2008.20038
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Benjamin, A. S., & Bawa, S. (2004). Distractor plausibility and criterion placement in recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 51(2), 159–172. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2004.04.001
Article
Google Scholar
Bylinskii, Z., Isola, P., Bainbridge, C., Torralba, A., & Oliva, A. (2015). Intrinsic and extrinsic effects on image memorability. Vision Research, 116, 165–178. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2015.03.005
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Bylinskii, Z., Judd, T., Oliva, A., Torralba, A., & Durand, F. (2019). What Do Different Evaluation Metrics Tell Us About Saliency Models? IEEE Transactions on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence, 41(3), 740–757.
Article
Google Scholar
Castelhano, M. S., Mack, M. L., & Henderson, J. M. (2009). Viewing task influences eye movement control during active scene perception. Journal of Vision, 9(6), 1–15. https://doi.org/10.1167/9.3.6.Introduction
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Chun, M. M. and Turk-Browne, N. B. (2007) ‘Interactions between attention and memory’, Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 17(2), pp. 177–184. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2007.03.005.
Coco, M. I., & Keller, F. (2014). Classification of visual and linguistic tasks using eye-movement features. Journal of Vision, 14(3), 11. https://doi.org/10.1167/14.3.11
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Coco, M. I., Merendino, G., Zappalà, G., & Della Sala, S. (in press). Semantic interference mechanisms on long-term visual memory and their eye-movement signatures in mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychology. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000734
Coco, M. I., Nuthmann, A., & Dimigen, O. (2020). Fixation-related brain potentials during semantic integration of object–scene information. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 32(4), 571–589. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01504
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Cowan, N., Beschin, N., & Della Sala, S. (2004). Verbal recall in amnesiacs under conditions of diminished retroactive interference. Brain, 127(4), 825–834. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awh107
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Craig, K. S., Berman, M. G., Jonides, J., & Lustig, C. (2013). Escaping the recent past: Which stimulus dimensions influence proactive interference? Memory & Cognition, 41(5), 650–670. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-012-0287-0
Article
Google Scholar
Cunningham, C. A., Yassa, M. A., & Egeth, H. E. (2015). Massive memory revisited: Limitations on storage capacity for object details in visual long-term memory. Learning & Memory, 22(11), 563–566. https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.039404.115
Article
Google Scholar
Dale, H. C. A. (1964). Retroactive interference in short-term memory. Nature, 203(1063), 1964. https://doi.org/10.1038/2031408a0
Article
Google Scholar
Dalmaijer, E. S., Mathôt, S., & Van der Stigchel, S. (2014). PyGaze: An open-source, cross-platform toolbox for minimal-effort programming of eyetracking experiments. Behavior Research Methods, 46(4), 913–921. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-013-0422-2
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Damiano, C., & Walther, D. B. (2019). Distinct roles of eye movements during memory encoding and retrieval. Cognition, 184(December 2018), 119–129. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.12.014
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Dewar, M., Alber, J., Butler, C., Cowan, N., & Della Sala, S. (2012). Brief wakeful resting boosts new memories over the long term. Psychological Science, 23(9), 955–960. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612441220
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Dewar, M. T., Cowan, N., & Sala, S. Della. (2007). Forgetting due to retroactive interference: A fusion of Müller and Pilzecker’s (1990) early insights into everyday forgetting and recent research on anterograde amnesia. Cortex, 43(5), 616–634. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70492-1
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Dobbins, I. G., & Kroll, N. E. A. (2005). Distinctiveness and the recognition mirror effect: Evidence for an item-based criterion placement heuristic. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 31(6), 1186–1198. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.31.6.1186
Article
Google Scholar
Dubey, R., Peterson, J., Khosla, A., Yang, M. H., & Ghanem, B. (2015). What makes an object memorable? Proceedings of the IEEE International Conference on Computer Vision, 2015 Inter, 1089–1097. https://doi.org/10.1109/ICCV.2015.130
Article
Google Scholar
Hannula, D. E. (2018). Attention and long-term memory: Bidirectional interactions and their effects on behavior. In K. Federmeier (Ed.0, Psychology of learning and motivation—Advances in research and theory (Vol. 69). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.plm.2018.09.004
Harel, J., Koch, C., & Perona, P. (2006). Graph-based visual saliency. Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems, 19, 545–552.
Google Scholar
Hayes, T. R., & Henderson, J. M. (2020). Center bias outperforms image salience but not semantics in accounting for attention during scene viewing. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 82(3), 985–994. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-019-01849-7
Article
Google Scholar
Henderson, J. M. (2003). Human gaze control during real-world scene perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(11), 498–504. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2003.09.006
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Hessels, R. S., Niehorster, D. C., Kemner, C., & Hooge, I. T. C. (2017). Noise-robust fixation detection in eye movement data: Identification by two-means clustering (I2MC). Behavior Research Methods, 49(5), 1802–1823. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-016-0822-1
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Ishiguro, S., & Saito, S. (2020). The detrimental effect of semantic similarity in short-term memory tasks: A meta-regression approach. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1–25. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-020-01815-7
Isola, P., Xiao, J., Parikh, D., Torralba, A., & Oliva, A. (2014). What makes a photograph memorable? IEEE Transactions on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence, 36(7), 1469–1482. https://doi.org/10.1109/TPAMI.2013.200
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Itti, L., Koch, C., & Niebur, E. (1998). A model of saliency-based visual attention for rapid scene analysis. IEEE Transaction on Pattern Analysis and Machine Learning, 20(11), 1254–1259. https://doi.org/10.1109/TPAMI.2012.125
Article
Google Scholar
Kafkas, A., & Montaldi, D. (2011). Recognition memory strength is predicted by pupillary responses at encoding while fixation patterns distinguish recollection from familiarity. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(10), 1971–1989. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2011.588335
Article
Google Scholar
Konkle, T., Brady, T. F., Alvarez, G. A., & Oliva, A. (2010a). Conceptual distinctiveness supports detailed visual long-term memory for real-world objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 139(3), 558–578. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019165
Article
Google Scholar
Konkle, T., Brady, T. F., Alvarez, G. A., & Oliva, A. (2010b). Scene memory is more detailed than you think: The role of categories in visual long-term memory. Psychological Science, 21(11), 1551–1556. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610385359
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Loftus, G. R., Kaufman, L., Nishimoto, T., & Ruthruff, E. (1992). Effects of visual degradation on eye-fixation duration, perceptual processing, and longterm visual memory. In Eye movements and visual cognition (pp. 203–226). Springer, New York, NY.
Lyu, M., Choe, K. W., Kardan, O., Kotabe, H., Henderson, J., & Berman, M. (2020). Overt attentional correlates of scene memorability and their relationships to scene semantics. Journal of Vision, 20(9), 1–17. 10.31234/osf.io/3e8qm
Macrae, C. N., & Lewis, H. L. (2002). Do I know you? Processing orientation and face recognition. Psychological Science, 13(2), 194–196.
Mathôt, S., Schreij, D., & Theeuwes, J. (2012). OpenSesame: An open-source, graphical experiment builder for the social sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 44(2), 314–324. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-011-0168-7
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
McGeoch, J. A., & McDonald, W. T. (1931). Meaningful relation and retroactive inhibition. The American Journal of Psychology, 43(4), 579–588.
Article
Google Scholar
Meghanathan, R. N., van Leeuwen, C., & Nikolaev, A. R. (2015). Fixation duration surpasses pupil size as a measure of memory load in free viewing. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8(JAN), 1–9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.01063
Müller, G. E., & Pilzecker, A. (1900). Experimentelle beiträge zur lehre vom gedächtniss (J. Barth (ed.)).
Parkhurst, D., Law, K., & Niebur, E. (2002). Modeling the role of salience in the allocation of overt visual attention. Vision Research, 42(1), 107–123. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0042-6989(01)00250-4
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Peters, R. J., Iyer, A., Itti, L., & Koch, C. (2005). Components of bottom-up gaze allocation in natural images. Vision Research, 45(18), 2397–2416. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2005.03.019
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Pomplun, M., Ritter, H., & Velichkovsky, B. (1996). Disambiguating complex visual information: Towards communication of personal views of a scene. Perception, 25(8), 931–948. https://doi.org/10.1068/p250931
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Rosinski, R. R., Golinkoff, R. M., & Kukish, K. S. (1975). Automatic semantic processing in a picture word interference task. Child Develop., 46(1), 247–253. https://doi.org/10.2307/1128859
Article
Google Scholar
Ryan, J. D., Althoff, R. R., Whitlow, S., & Cohen, N. J. (2000). Amnesia is a deficit in relational memory. Psychological Science, 11(6), 454–461. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00288
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Ryan, J. D., Hannula, D. E., & Cohen, N. J. (2007). The obligatory effects of memory on eye movements. Memory, 15(5), 508–525. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210701391022
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Ryan, J. D., & Shen, K. (2020). The eyes are a window into memory. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 32, 1–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2019.12.014
Article
Google Scholar
Ryan, J. D., Shen, K., & Liu, Z. (2020). The intersection between the oculomotor and hippocampal memory systems : empirical developments and clinical implications. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1464(1), 115–141. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.14256
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Satterthwaite, F. E. (1946). An approximate distribution of estimates of variance components. Biometrics Bulletin, 2(6), 110–114. https://doi.org/10.2307/3002019
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Shulman, H. G. (1971). Similarity effects in short-term memory. Psychological Bulletin, 75(6), 399–415. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0031257
Article
Google Scholar
Skaggs, E. B. (1933). A discussion on the temporal point of interpolation and degree of retroactive inhibition. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 16, 411–414. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0074460
Article
Google Scholar
Tatler, B. W. (2007). The central fixation bias in scene viewing: Selecting an optimal viewing position independently of motor biases and image feature distributions. Journal of Vision, 7(14), 1–17. https://doi.org/10.1167/7.14.4
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Tavakoli, H. R., Rahtu, E., & Heikkil, J. (2011). Fast and efficient saliency detection using sparse sampling and kernel density estimation. In A. Heyden & F. Kahl (Eds.), Scandinavian conference on image analysis (pp. 666–675). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-21227-7_62
Underwood, B. J. (1945). The effect of successive interpolations on retroactive and proactive inhibition. Psychological Monographs, 59(273), 1–33. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0093547
Article
Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1999). Manual for the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence. Psychological Corporation.
Xiao, J., Hays, J., Ehinger, K. A., Oliva, A., & Torralba, A. (2010). SUN database: Large-scale scene recognition from abbey to zoo. Proceedings of the IEEE Computer Society Conference on Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition, 3485–3492. https://doi.org/10.1109/CVPR.2010.5539970