Introduction

Human behavior fluctuates over time. These fluctuations are usually regarded as measurement noise (Landau & Fries, 2012), yet recent evidence has demonstrated that they are informative of rhythmic characteristics that closely resemble the spectral features of neural oscillations (Benedetto & Morrone, 2017; Fiebelkorn & Kastner, 2019; Fiebelkorn, Saalmann, & Kastner, 2013; Ho, Burr, Alais, & Morrone, 2019; Ho, Leung, Burr, Alais, & Morrone, 2017; Huang, Chen, & Luo, 2015; Landau & Fries, 2012; Re, Inbar, Richter, & Landau, 2019; Song, Meng, Chen, Zhou, & Luo, 2014; Tomassini, Spinelli, Jacono, Sandini, & Morrone, 2015). However, as existing evidence is restricted within the study of perception, it is unclear whether complex cooperative behaviors also fluctuate rhythmically and, more importantly, whether these fluctuations are synchronized among co-actors in a neurophysiologically relevant manner (e.g., phase coherence).

Cooperation is of vital importance to human society, on which human beings rely to survive and thrive. One important form of cooperation is synchronized behaviors. For many cooperative activities, synchronized behavior is crucial for optimal performance. In constructing a pyramid in ancient Egypt, for example, hundreds of slaves needed to synchronize their pulling or lifting to move the massive stone blocks. The synchronization of co-actors’ behavior has been investigated in kinematic studies using body movement dynamics recorded via motion-tracking devices (Chauvigné, Walton, Richardson, & Brown, 2019; Cuijpers, Den Hartigh, Zaal, & de Poel, 2019; Richardson, Garcia, Frank, Gregor, & Marsh, 2012; Silva et al., 2016). However, these studies focused mainly on the synchronization of overt movements, without concerning the spectral feature or the underlying inner cognitive and neurophysiological processes. And the movement dynamics they studied are too slow (frequencies < 1 Hz, e.g., 0.6 Hz for chair rocking (Richardson et al., 2012), or ~30–50 strokes per minute for rowing (Cuijpers et al., 2019)) to inhabit timescales of change as fast as the timescales of cognitive and neurophysiological dynamics.

The present study aimed to probe the spectral synchrony of behavioral fluctuation during cooperation with a timescale as short as ~50 ms (frequencies as high as ~20 Hz) using a time-resolved approach. This approach has been successfully used to track spectral features (4–12 Hz) in the fluctuations of behavioral performance (accuracy, reaction times, sensitivity, or decision criteria) of visual (Landau & Fries, 2012; Tomassini et al., 2015; VanRullen, 2016) and auditory (Ho et al., 2019; Ho et al., 2017) perception. Using this approach in a well-established cooperation paradigm (Cheng, Li, & Hu, 2015; Cui, Bryant, & Reiss, 2012), we were able to access the fine-grained time course of behavioral fluctuation in cooperation, from which band-specific behavioral rhythms can be extracted and phase coherence between co-actors’ behavioral rhythms can be computed. Theta-band interbrain phase coherence has been reported to be enhanced during social cooperation (Hu et al., 2018; Kawasaki, Yamada, Ushiku, Miyauchi, & Yamaguchi, 2013; Liu et al., 2018; Sänger, Müller, & Lindenberger, 2012). Additionally, theta-band rhythm has been implicated in prediction (Huang et al., 2015), cognitive control (Cavanagh & Frank, 2014), and behavioral adaptation (Billeke, Zamorano, Cosmelli, & Aboitiz, 2012; Cavanagh, Frank, Klein, & Allen, 2010), all of which are essential for cooperation. Taken together, we expect the co-actors’ behavioral fluctuations to be synchronized within the theta band.

Methods

Participants

Twenty pairs of male college students (all right-handed, average age 20.72±1.45 years) with normal or corrected-to-normal vision participated in the experiment. Two extra pairs of participants were eliminated from the study due to an inadequate number of trials after data trimming (> 30 % of trials were discarded). This study recruited only males because a recent study reported that male-male dyads exhibited better cooperative performance than male-female and female-female dyads (Cheng et al., 2015). Participants were randomly paired, and members of pairs were not acquainted with each other before the experiment. The participants provided written informed consent before the experiment and received monetary compensation depending on their performance. This study conforms to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the research ethics committee of Zhejiang Normal University.

Stimuli and procedure

Using a time-resolved approach, we recruited 20 pairs of adults in a well-established cooperation paradigm (Cui et al., 2012). Each pair of participants sat side-by-side in front of a monitor (Fig. 1b), with the participant on the left side denoted as participant #1 and the one on the right as participant #2. They were asked to complete a cooperation task and a competition task (as a control condition) separately in 2 days. In both tasks, each pair of participants were informed that a green disk (“go” signal) would appear after a gray ring (“ready” signal) and they are required to press a button after seeing the green disk (Fig. 1a). Reaction times (RTs) were collected. In the cooperation task, participants were asked to minimize their RT difference. If the difference was smaller than a threshold (defined as one-eighth of the sum of their RTs in the current trial; Cui et al., 2012), both participants earned one point; otherwise, they both lost one point. In the competition task, participants were asked to press keys as fast as possible upon seeing the green signal; the participant with the faster response won a point and the other lost one point. Participants were asked to maximize their points, and their monetary compensates will depend on their winning scores in each task. After both participants responded, feedbacks were presented with information to reinforce cooperation or competition in respective tasks. It is worth noting that stimuli were exactly the same for cooperation and competition tasks before feedback screen, and participants needed to adjust their responses according to different task demands (see Supplementary Material for details)

Fig. 1
figure 1

Experimental procedure and design. (a) Schematic of a trial. Each trial started with the display of a gray ring (the “ready” signal), followed by a green disk (the “go” signal), which signaled both participants to press keys according to cooperation or competition demand. The stimulus onset asynchronies (SOAs) between the “ready” and “go” signals were varied from 0.2 s to 1 s in steps of 0.02 s. (b) Each pair of participants sat in front of a monitor side by side, with their responding hands covered with a black box (indicated by transparent black squares). (c) Schematic of phase coherence in the cooperation vs. competition conditions. The solid and dashed lines schematically represent the theta-band reaction time (RT) fluctuations of participant #1 and #2 in the cooperation (blue) vs. the competition (red) conditions. For cooperation, the phase differences between the two co-actors are consistent across time, as indicated by the relatively more clustered blue dots in the polar system, representing a high interpersonal phase coherence. For competition, the phase differences were more inconsistent across time, as indicated by the relatively more evenly distributed red dots in the polar space, representing a low interpersonal phase coherence. The length of the black lines in the polar space represents the values of interpersonal phase coherence

Critically, to achieve fine-grained temporal assessment of behavior performance, we used a time-resolved approach such that the stimulus onset asynchronies (SOAs) between the “ready” and “go” signals were arranged from 0.2 s to 1 s in steps of 20 ms, corresponding to a sampling frequency of 50 Hz. The assumption is that the “ready” signal serves as a time reference and a salient event to reset the phase of ongoing behavioral rhythms (VanRullen, 2016), leading to reproducible cooperative and competitive dynamics over multiple trials (Fig. 1c). An exhaustive set of SOAs allows densely sampling the rapid fluctuation of ongoing behavioral performance, such that a fine-grained time course for each condition can be generated for each participant (Figs. 2a and 2b). The resulting time courses were subsequently analyzed for oscillation and interpersonal synchronization.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Grand average reaction time (RT) time course. (a and b) Grand average RT time courses for the cooperation (a) and competition (b) conditions. Blue and red lines represent participant #1 and participant #2, respectively; shaded regions denote±SEM. Time courses were averaged separately for “participant #1” and “participant #2” for display purposes. Note that the grand averaged data only loosely represent the fluctuating characteristic because the main findings were generated from single-participant spectral or single-dyad coherence analysis. (c) Grand average time courses of absolute values of RT differences between participant #1 and participant #2. In the title, “RT#1” denotes RT for participant #1, and “RT#2” denotes RT for participant #2. Blue and red lines represent absolute RT differences in the cooperation and competition conditions, respectively; shaded regions denote±SEM. Black dots indicate SOAs in which the difference between the cooperation and competition conditions was statistically significant (ps < 0.05, FDR corrected)

Data analysis

The time-course data were subsequently submitted to Fourier transform and phase coherence analysis. Statistical analysis were performed using a non-parametric permutation test (see Supplemental Material for details).

Results

Figures 2a and 2b illustrate the raw RT time courses averaged over all dyads for participant #1 (blue) and participant #2 (red) as a function of SOA in the cooperation (A) and competition (B) conditions. Generally, the RT traces decreased over time courses, demonstrating a second-order polynomial trend. To better characterize the fluctuating pattern in the time courses, we removed the trend using robust detrending (de Cheveigné & Arzounian, 2018) (see Fig. S2 for fluctuations of the time courses of each dyad after normalizing and detrending). Moreover, Fig. 2c shows the time courses of absolute values of RT differences between participant #1 and participant #2 as a function of SOA. The absolute values of RT differences were significantly smaller in the cooperation than the competition conditions (permutation test, ps < 0.05, p-values were corrected by a false discovery rate (FDR) of 0.1 (Genovese, Lazar, & Nichols, 2002)). This finding was consistent with our experimental setup that participants were required to minimize their RT difference in the cooperation task but to respond as fast as possible in the competition task.

To measure the characteristics of oscillatory components in the ongoing behavioral performance, raw RT time courses of each participant in each condition were normalized, detrended, and then subjected to a fast Fourier transform. Figures 3a and 3b show the grand average amplitude spectrum averaged across all participants (both participants in all dyads) in the cooperation and competition conditions, respectively. To determine whether the spectrums simply reflected a random fluctuations in time, we performed permutation tests to examine statistical significance. The dashed lines in Figs. 3a and 3b represent the cutoff for statistical significance (ps < 0.05, FDR corrected). As shown in the figures, the amplitude spectrums in both conditions revealed no significant peak. Additionally, the spectral amplitude was generally higher in the cooperation than the competition conditions. However, the effect was significant only for several frequencies (as indicated by black dots in Fig. 3c; permutation test, ps < 0.05, FDR corrected) due to larger fluctuations in cooperation than competition (as shown in Figs. 2a and 2b).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Grand average amplitude and coherence spectrum. (a and b) Grand average amplitude spectrum for the cooperation (a) and competition (b) conditions. Dashed lines represent the cutoff of statistical significance (ps < 0.05, FDR corrected). (c) Comparison of the amplitude spectrum between the cooperation (blue) and competition (red) conditions. Shaded regions denote±SEM. Black dots indicate the frequencies for which the difference between cooperation and competition was statistically significant (permutation test, ps < 0.05, FDR corrected). (d and e) Grand average coherence spectrum for the cooperation (d) and competition (e) conditions. Dashed lines represent the cutoff of statistical significance (ps < 0.05, FDR corrected). (f) Comparison of the coherence spectrum between the cooperation (blue) and competition (red) conditions. Shaded blue and red regions denote±SEM. The gray-shaded rectangle indicates the frequencies for which the difference between cooperation and competition was statistically significant (permutation test, ps < 0.05, FDR corrected)

Next, to examine the synchronization between the two RT time courses of participant #1 and participant #2 in a dyad, we computed phase coherence between the two participants in each dyad. Figures 3d and 3e illustrate the coherence spectrum averaged over all dyads in the cooperation and competition conditions, respectively. To assess whether the coherence was due to a random relationship between the two time courses in a dyad, we performed permutation tests to examine statistical significance. The dashed lines in Figs. 3d and 3e represent the cutoff for statistical significance (ps< 0.05, FDR corrected). The results revealed significant coherences in the theta and alpha band (~4–10 Hz) in the cooperation condition but not in the competition condition. However, only theta-band coherence (~5–6 Hz) exhibited a significant difference between the cooperation and competition conditions, with enhanced coherence observed for the cooperation condition (permutation test, ps < 0.05, FDR corrected) (and see Fig. S3 for coherence difference in each dyad).

Recently, several studies have noted that cooperation is improved when leader-follower relationship is defined (Fairhurst, Janata, & Keller, 2014; Jiang et al., 2015; Konvalinka et al., 2014). In the present study, the leader was defined as the participant who had more trials with faster responses, whereas the follower was defined as his partner. Here, to quantify the leader-follower relationship within a dyad, we coined the term “balance index,” calculated as the ratio of the follower’s summed counts of faster-responding trials to the leader’s summed counts of faster-responding trials (see Fig. 4a for an exemplary dyad). This index ranges from 0 to 1, with zero indicating that the leader responded faster in all trials (thus forming a fully unbalanced relationship) and one indicating the absence of a leader/follower in a dyad (thus forming a fully balanced relationship). Indeed, in accordance with previous findings (Fairhurst et al., 2014; Konvalinka et al., 2014), we found a significant negative correlation (Spearman’s rho = -0.472, p = 0.037, Fig. 4b) between this index and cooperative performance, which was quantified by the winning score (points won minus points lost) in the cooperation task.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Correlations between phase coherence, balance index, and winning score. (a) Calculation of balance index in the cooperation task. The plot shows the histogram of trial counts as a function of reaction time (RT) difference (participant #1 − participant #2) in an exemplary dyad. First, we defined the participant in a dyad who had more trials with faster response as the leader (blue) and the other participant as the follower (red). Second, balance index was calculated as the ratio of the follower’s summed counts of faster-responding trials to the leader’s summed counts of faster-responding trials. A higher balance index indicates a less clear leader/follower roles in a dyad. (b) Balance index was significantly correlated with winning score in the cooperation condition (rho = -0.472, p = 0.037). The line represents the best linear fit. (c) Phase coherence was significantly correlated with balance index in the cooperation condition (rho = -0.537, p = 0.016). The line represents the best linear fit

Finally, to examine whether theta-band coherence in the cooperation task was of behavioral relevance, we calculated the correlation of theta-band coherence with balance index and winning score. Theta-band coherence was estimated by the average coherence across±3 frequency points around the peak of the single-dyad’s coherence spectrum in the range of 3–8 Hz in the cooperation task. We found that theta-band coherence was significantly negatively correlated with balance index (Spearman’s rho = -0.537, p = 0.016, Fig. 4c). Additionally, theta-band coherence was not significantly correlated winning score (Spearman’s rho = 0.385, p = 0.095). This might suggest winning score is not an appropriate indicator for cooperation compared to balance index, since the criterion we used to quantify winning or losing was, in some degree, arbitrary. By contrast, alpha-band (8–12 Hz) coherence in cooperation did not significantly correlate with winning score (Spearman’s rho = -0.226, p = 0.337) or balance index (Spearman’s rho = 0.015, p = 0.952).

Discussion

The present study reports, for the first time, that cooperative behaviors of co-actors fluctuated synchronously, in a manner that resembles the spectral features of interbrain synchrony found in hyperscanning studies (Hu et al., 2018; Kawasaki et al., 2013; Liu et al., 2018; Sänger et al., 2012). Using a time-resolved approach, we observed that theta-band interpersonal behavioral coherence was enhanced during cooperation relative to competition and significantly correlated with leader-follower relationships during cooperation, pinpointing a unique role of theta-band interpersonal behavioral coherence during cooperation.

Using a time-resolved approach, previous studies have repeatedly reported behavioral oscillation in visual (Landau & Fries, 2012; Tomassini et al., 2015) and auditory (Ho et al., 2019; Ho et al., 2017) detection task, implying that perception and cognition operate periodically, as a succession of cycles mirroring the underlying neural oscillations (Landau, 2018; VanRullen, 2013, 2016). Here, we used this approach in a complex social interaction context to probe behavioral oscillation in cooperation and competition. However, we did not find significant behavioral oscillation in individual’s amplitude spectrum. This might be due to task difference. The task used in the present study was not a pure visual perception task, because participants were asked to adjust the responses to visual stimuli according to cooperation or competition demands. And these adjustments would derail the individual’s behavioral responses from original behavioral rhythms, causing behavioral oscillation unable to survive statistical test.

More importantly, we further computed phase coherence between two co-actors’ ongoing behavioral fluctuations to probe interpersonal behavioral synchrony, and the results revealed significant theta and alpha band interpersonal phase coherence in cooperation, in contrast to non-significant coherence in competition. The findings of theta and alpha band behavioral synchrony are consistent with EEG-based hyperscanning studies reporting larger theta and alpha band interbrain synchrony in high cooperation tasks than in low cooperation tasks (Hu et al., 2018; Kawasaki et al., 2013; Liu et al., 2018). Why interpersonal phase coherence arose in cooperation? At the dyad level, co-actors’ ongoing behavioral performance might have become coordinated in cooperation through top-down modulation of mutual prediction and adaptation (Konvalinka, Vuust, Roepstorff, & Frith, 2010), leading to phase coherence in cooperation, whereas no such coordination occurred in competition. Taken together, our results, while preliminary, suggest that cooperation’s effect on rhythmic dynamics can be exhibited only at the dyad level, consistent with previous observations (Cui et al., 2012) that cooperation versus competition difference was only found in dyad-level interbrain synchrony but not in individual-level spectrotemporal dynamics. This finding demonstrates, for the first time, that cooperative behaviors of co-actors fluctuated synchronously, with a dynamic resembling the spectral features of neurophysiological dynamics, providing “mirrored” evidence of interbrain synchrony (Cheng et al., 2015; Cui et al., 2012; Hu et al., 2018; Kawasaki et al., 2013; Liu et al., 2018; Sänger et al., 2012) directly in behavior dynamics. Thus, interpersonal behavioral coherence may provide a sensitive and economic tool to probe cognitive neural mechanisms of cooperation compared to costly and inconvenient brain scanning techniques.

When comparing cooperation with competition, only theta-band behavioral coherence remained significant. This suggested that theta coherence might be a unique component underling cooperation versus competition difference, while alpha coherence might be related to other cognitive components (e.g., attention) shared between tasks. The theta-band rhythms have been implicated in prediction (Huang et al., 2015), cognitive control (see Cavanagh & Frank, 2014, for a review), and behavioral adaptation (Billeke et al., 2012; Cavanagh et al., 2010), all of which are essential for cooperation. However, alpha-band behavioral (e.g., Fiebelkorn et al., 2013; Re et al., 2019; Song et al., 2014) and neural (e.g., Bollimunta, Mo, Schroeder, & Ding, 2011; Rohenkohl & Nobre, 2011) oscillations have been consistently associated with attention, which could be equally engaged in both cooperation and competition. Indeed, by design, in the present study, participants need to attend to “ready” and “go” signal stimuli in both cooperation and competition tasks, leading to a comparable amount of attention involved in both conditions. These explanations are strengthened by our finding that only theta-band coherence significantly correlated with leader-follower relationship in cooperation, while alpha-band coherence provided no such behavioral relevance.

Leader-follower relationships were used to assess the behavioral relevance of behavioral coherence because studies showed that performance was better when theta-band leader-follower roles were established (Fairhurst et al., 2014; Jiang et al., 2015; Konvalinka et al., 2014). Indeed, as shown by balance index (see Fig. 4b), in many dyads, the relationship between the two participants was not balanced regarding response speed: one participant had more trials with faster response than his partner. We assume that the more cooperative pairs of participants spontaneously adopted a leader-follower strategy when they tried to adapt to each other. Consistent with previous brain-imaging hyperscanning studies (Jiang et al., 2015; Konvalinka et al., 2014), our results showed that better cooperative performance (tagger by higher winning score) was associated with clearer leader/follower roles (tagged by lower balance index). Balance index, in turn, was negatively correlated with theta-band interpersonal behavioral coherence, indicating that dyads with a clearer leader-follower relationship tended to have higher phase coherence. In other words, theta-band fluctuations of members of a dyad tended to be better synchronized when the dyad had a clear leader. Thus, we suggest that interpersonal behavioral coherence could be used as a potential marker for leader-follower structure in cooperation, which has been suggested as a key factor impacting cooperation quality (Fairhurst et al., 2014; Jiang et al., 2015; Konvalinka et al., 2014).

In conclusion, the findings reported here provide the first evidence for interpersonal synchronization of theta-band behavioral fluctuations during cooperation and its relevance to the leader-follower relationship in cooperation. The present study shed new light on the rapidly expanding field of behavioral oscillations, by extending its application beyond perceptual behavioral rhythms revealed by individual-level amplitude spectrum to more complex social interactions revealed by dyad-level interpersonal phase coherence. Our findings pinpoint a unique role of theta-band interpersonal behavioral coherence in cooperation and provide “mirrored” evidence of interbrain synchrony directly in behavioral dynamics.