The capture of attention by entirely irrelevant pictures of calorie-dense foods

Article

Abstract

Inborn preference for palatable energy-dense food is thought to be an evolutionary adaptation. One way this preference manifests itself is through the control of visual attention. In the present study, we investigated how attentional capture is influenced by changes in naturally occurring goal-states, in this case desire for energy-dense foods (typically high fat and/or high sugar). We demonstrate that even when distractors are entirely irrelevant, participants were significantly more distracted by energy-dense foods compared with non-food objects and even low-energy foods. Additionally, we show the lability of these goal-states by having a separate set of participants consume a small amount of calorie-dense food prior to the task. The amount of distraction by the energy-dense food images in this case was significantly reduced and no different than distraction by images of low-energy foods and images of non-food objects. While naturally occurring goal-states can be difficult to ignore, they also are highly flexible.

Keywords

Attentional capture Irrelevant distractors Energy-dense foods 

References

  1. Anderson, B. A., Laurent, P. A., & Yantis, S. (2013). Reward predictions bias attentional selection. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 262. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00262 PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  2. Bacon, W. F., & Egeth, H. E. (1994). Overriding stimulus-driven attentional capture. Perception & Psychophysics, 55(5), 485-496.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Blechert, J., Meule, A., Busch, N. A., & Ohla, K. (2014). Food-pics: an image database for experimental research on eating and appetite. Frontiers in Psychology, 5. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00617
  4. Brainard, D. H. (1997). The Psychophysics Toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10(4), 433-436. https://doi.org/10.1163/156856897x00357 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Branton, A., Akhavan, T., Gladanac, B., Pollard, D., Welch, J., Rossiter, M., & Bellissimo, N. (2014). Pre-meal video game playing and a glucose preload suppress food intake in normal weight boys. Appetite, 83, 256–262.Google Scholar
  6. Castellanos, E. H., Charboneau, E., Dietrich, M. S., Park, S., Bradley, B. P., Mogg, K., & Cowan, R. L. (2009). Obese adults have visual attention bias for food cue images: Evidence for altered reward system function. International Journal of Obesity, 33(9), 1063.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Drewnowski, A. (1997a). Taste preferences and food intake. Annual Review of Nutrition, 17(1), 237-253.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Drewnowski, A. (1997b). Why do we like fat? Journal of the American Dietetic Association, 97(7), S58-S62.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G* Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175-191.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Fishbach, A., Friedman, R. S., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2003). Leading us not into temptation: Momentary allurements elicit overriding goal activation. Journal of personality and social psychology, 84(2), 296.Google Scholar
  11. Folk, C. L. (2013). Dissociating compatibility effects and distractor costs in the additional singleton paradigm. Frontiers in Psychology, 4 434.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  12. Folk, C. L., & Remington, R. (1998). Selectivity in distraction by irrelevant featural singletons: evidence for two forms of attentional capture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human perception and performance, 24(3), 847.Google Scholar
  13. Folk, C. L., Remington, R. W., & Johnston, J. C. (1992). Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18(4), 1030.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Folk, C. L., Remington, R. W., & Johnston, J. C. (1993). Contingent attentional capture: A reply to Yantis (1993). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Peiformance, 19, 682–685. Google Scholar
  15. Forster, S., & Lavie, N. (2008). Failures to ignore entirely irrelevant distractors: The role of load. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Applied, 14(1), 73-83. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-898X.14.1.73 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  16. Forster, S., & Lavie, N. (2011). Entirely irrelevant distractors can capture and captivate attention. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(6), 1064-1070. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-011-0172-z CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Gibson, B. S., & Kelsey, E. M. (1998). Stimulus-driven attentional capture is contingent on attentional set for displaywide visual features. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24(3), 699.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Graves, T., & Egeth, H. E. (2015). When does feature search fail to protect against attentional capture? Visual cognition, 23(9–10), 1098–1123.Google Scholar
  19. Hall, P. A. (2016). Executive-control processes in high-calorie food consumption. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 25(2), 91-98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Herman, C. P., & Mack, D. (1975). Restrained and unrestrained eating. Journal of personality, 43(4), 647–660.Google Scholar
  21. Hodsoll, S., Viding, E., & Lavie, N. (2011). Attentional capture by irrelevant emotional distractor faces. Emotion, 11(2), 346.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Hoek, H. W. (2006). Incidence, prevalence and mortality of anorexia nervosa and other eating disorders. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 19(4), 389-394.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. James, W. (1890/1950). The Principles of Psychology. New York: Dover Publications.Google Scholar
  24. Jansen, A., & Van den Hout, M. (1991). On being led into temptation:“Counterregulation” of dieters after smelling a “preload”. Addictive behaviors, 16(5), 247–253.Google Scholar
  25. Lavie, N. (1995). Perceptual load as a necessary condition for selective attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(3), 451.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Leber, A. B., & Egeth, H. E. (2006). It’s under control: Top-down search strategies can override attentional capture. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13(1), 132-138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Lleras, A., Buetti, S., & Mordkoff, J. T. (2013). When do the effects of distractors provide a measure of distractibility. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 59, 261-316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Lynn, J., & Shin, M. (2015). Strategic top-down control versus attentional bias by previous reward history. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 77(7), 2207–2216.Google Scholar
  29. Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., Hyare, H., & Lee, S. (1998). Selective attention to food-related stimuli in hunger: Are attentional biases specific to emotional and psychopathological states, or are they also found in normal drive states? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36(2), 227-237.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Most, S. B., Chun, M. M., Widders, D. M., & Zald, D. H. (2005). Attentional rubbernecking: Cognitive control and personality in emotion-induced blindness. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12(4), 654-661.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Most, S. B., & Wang, L. (2011). Dissociating spatial attention and awareness in emotion-induced blindness. Psychological Science, 22(3), 300-305.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Neimeijer, R. A., de Jong, P. J., & Roefs, A. (2013). Temporal attention for visual food stimuli in restrained eaters. Appetite, 64, 5-11.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Nijs, I. M., & Franken, I. H. (2012). Attentional processing of food cues in overweight and obese individuals. Current Obesity Reports, 1(2), 106-113.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  34. Ogden, C. L., Carroll, M. D., Kit, B. K., & Flegal, K. M. (2014). Prevalence of childhood and adult obesity in the United States, 2011-2012. JAMA, 311(8), 806-814. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2014.732 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  35. Olszanowski, M., Pochwatko, G., Kuklinski, K., Scibor-Rylski, M., Lewinski, P., & Ohme, R. K. (2014). Warsaw set of emotional facial expression pictures: a validation study of facial display photographs. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1516. Google Scholar
  36. Papies, E. K., Stroebe, W., & Aarts, H. (2008). The allure of forbidden food: On the role of attention in self-regulation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(5), 1283–1292.Google Scholar
  37. Piech, R. M., Pastorino, M. T., & Zald, D. H. (2010). All I saw was the cake. Hunger effects on attentional capture by visual food cues. Appetite, 54(3), 579-582.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Pool, E., Brosch, T., Delplanque, S., & Sander, D. (2014). Where is the chocolate? Rapid spatial orienting toward stimuli associated with primary rewards. Cognition, 130(3), 348-359.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Ristic, J., & Landry, M. (2015). Combining attention: a novel way of conceptualizing the links between attention, sensory processing, and behavior. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 77(1), 36-49.Google Scholar
  40. Robinson, M. J., & Berridge, K. C. (2013). Instant transformation of learned repulsion into motivational “wanting”. Current Biology, 23(4), 282-289.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  41. Rogers, P. J., & Hill, A. J. (1989). Breakdown of dietary restraint following mere exposure to food stimuli: interrelationships between restraint, hunger, salivation, and food intake. Addictive behaviors, 14(4), 387–397.Google Scholar
  42. Shafran, R., Lee, M., Cooper, Z., Palmer, R. L., & Fairburn, C. G. (2007). Attentional bias in eating disorders. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 40(4), 369-380.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  43. Theeuwes, J. (1990). Perceptual selectivity is task dependent: Evidence from selective search. Acta Psychologica, 74(1), 81-99.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Theeuwes, J. (1994). Stimulus-driven capture and attentional set: Selective search for color and visual abrupt onsets. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20(4), 799.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Theeuwes, J. (2010). Top–down and bottom–up control of visual selection. Acta Psychologica, 135(2), 77-99.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Theeuwes, J., & Van der Stigchel, S. (2006). Faces capture attention: Evidence from inhibition of return. Visual Cognition, 13(6), 657-665.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Toepel, U., Knebel, J.-F., Hudry, J., le Coutre, J., & Murray, M. M. (2009). The brain tracks the energetic value in food images. NeuroImage, 44(3), 967-974.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Van Dillen, L. F., Papies, E. K., & Hofmann, W. (2013). Turning a blind eye to temptation: How cognitive load can facilitate self-regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104(3), 427.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  49. Veenstra, E. M., de Jong, P. J., Koster, E. H., & Roefs, A. (2010). Attentional avoidance of high-fat food in unsuccessful dieters. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 41(3), 282-288.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Vuilleumier, P., & Schwartz, S. (2001). Emotional facial expressions capture attention. Neurology, 56(2), 153-158.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  51. Werthmann, J., Roefs, A., Nederkoorn, C., & Jansen, A. (2013). Desire lies in the eyes: Attention bias for chocolate is related to craving and self-endorsed eating permission. Appetite, 70, 81-89.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. Werthmann, J., Roefs, A., Nederkoorn, C., Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., & Jansen, A. (2011). Can (not) take my eyes off it: Attention bias for food in overweight participants. Health Psychology, 30(5), 561.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Yantis, S., & Jonides, J. (1984). Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: Evidence from visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 10(5), 601-621.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Psychological and Brain SciencesJohns Hopkins UniversityBaltimoreUSA

Personalised recommendations