Psychonomic Bulletin & Review

, Volume 24, Issue 5, pp 1364–1374 | Cite as

Generating explanations via analogical comparison

Brief Report

Abstract

Generating explanations can be highly effective in promoting learning in both adults and children. Our interest is in the mechanisms that underlie this effect and in whether and how they operate in early learning. In adult reasoning, explanation may call on many subprocesses—including comparison, counterfactual reasoning, and reasoning by exclusion; but it is unlikely that all these processes are available to young children. We propose that one process that may serve both children and adults is comparison. In this study, we asked whether children would use the results of a comparison experience when asked to explain why a model skyscraper was stable. We focused on a challenging principle—that diagonal cross-bracing lends stability to physical structures (Gentner et al., Cognitive Science, 40, 224–240, 2016). Six-year-olds either received no training or interacted with model skyscrapers in one of three different conditions, designed to vary in their potential to invite and support comparison. In the Single Model condition, children interacted with a single braced model. In the comparison conditions (Low Alignability and High Alignability), children compared braced and unbraced models. Following experience with the models, children were asked to explain why the braced model was stable. They then received two transfer tasks. We found that children who received highly alignable pairs were most likely to (a) produce brace-based explanations and (b) transfer the brace principle to a dissimilar context. This provides evidence that children can benefit from analogical comparison in generating explanations and also suggests limitations on this ability.

Keywords

Explanation Comparison Analogy Learning 

References

  1. Alfieri, L., Nokes-Malach, T. J., & Schunn, C. D. (2013). Learning through case comparisons: A meta-analytic review. Educational Psychologist, 48(2), 87–113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Amsterlaw, J., & Wellman, H. M. (2006). Theories of mind in transition: A microgenetic study of the development of false belief understanding. Journal of Cognition and Development, 7(2), 139–172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Augier, L., & Thibaut, J. P. (2013). The benefits and costs of comparisons in a novel object categorization task: Interactions with development. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20(2), 1126–1132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Benjamin, N., Haden, C. A., & Wilkerson, E. (2010). Enhancing building, conversation, and learning through caregiver-child interactions in a children’s museum. Developmental Psychology, 46(2), 502–515.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Bonawitz, E. B., van Schijndel, T., Friel, D., & Schulz, L. (2012). Balancing theories and evidence in children’s exploration, explanations, and learning. Cognitive Psychology, 64(4), 215–234.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Chi, M. T. H. (2009). Active-constructive-interactive: A conceptual framework for differentiating learning activities. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1, 73–105.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Chi, M. T. H., Bassok, M., Lewis, M., Reimann, P., & Glaser, R. (1989). Self-explanations: How students study and use examples in learning to solve problems. Cognitive Science, 13, 145–182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Chi, M. T. H., de Leeuw, N., Chiu, M., & LaVancher, C. (1994). Eliciting self-explanations improves understanding. Cognitive Science, 18, 439–477.Google Scholar
  9. Chin-Parker, S., & Bradner, A. (2010). Background shifts affect explanatory style: How a pragmatic theory of explanation accounts for background effects in the generation of explanations. Cognitive Processing, 11, 227–249.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Christie, S., & Gentner, D. (2010). Where hypotheses come from: Learning new relations by structural alignment. Journal of Cognition and Development, 11(3), 356–373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Cimpian, A. (2015). The inherence heuristic: Generating everyday explanations. In R. Scott & S. Kosslyn (Eds.), Emerging trends in the social and behavioral sciences (pp. 1–15). Hoboken: Wiley.Google Scholar
  12. Cimpian, A., & Salomon, E. (2014). The inherence heuristic: An intuitive means of making sense of the world, and a potential precursor to psychological essentialism. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 37(5), 461–480.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Clement, C. A., & Gentner, D. (1991). Systematicity as a selection constraint in analogical mapping. Cognitive Science, 15(1), 89–132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Collins, A., & Gentner, D. (1987). How people construct mental models. In D. Holland & N. Quinn (Eds.), Cultural models in language and thought (pp. 243–265). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Crowley, K., & Siegler, R. S. (1999). Explanation and generalization in young children’s strategy learning. Child Development, 70, 304–316.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Day, S., & Gentner, D. (2007). Nonintentional analogical inference in text comprehension. Memory and Cognition, 35, 39–49.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Doumas, L. A. A., & Hummel, J. E. (2013). Comparison and mapping facilitate relation discovery and predication. PLOS ONE, 8(6), e63889. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0063889 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  18. Edwards, B. J., Williams, J. J., & Lombrozo, T. (2013). Effects of explanation and comparison on category learning. In M. Knauff, M. Pauen, N. Sebanz, & I. Wachsmuth (Eds.), Proceedings of the 35th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Austin: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
  19. Falkenhainer, B., Forbus, K. D., & Gentner, D. (1989). The structure-mapping engine: Algorithm and examples. Artificial Intelligence, 41, 1–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Fonseca, B. A., & Chi, M. T. H. (2011). Instruction based on self-explanation. In R. E. Mayer & P. A. Alexander (Eds.), The handbook of research on learning and instruction (pp. 296–321). New York: Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
  21. Forbus, K. D., Ferguson, R. W., Lovett, A., & Gentner, D. (2016). Extending SME to handle large-scale cognitive modeling. Cognitive Science. doi:10.1111/cogs.12377
  22. Forbus, K. D., Gentner, D., & Law, K. (1995). MAC/FAC: A model of similarity-based retrieval. Cognitive Science, 19, 141–205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Gentner, D. (1983). Structure-mapping: A theoretical framework for analogy. Cognitive Science, 7, 155–170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Gentner, D., & Hoyos, C. (2017). Analogy and abstraction. Topics in Cognitive Science. Google Scholar
  25. Gentner, D., & Kurtz, K. (2006). Relations, objects, and the composition of analogies. Cognitive Science, 30, 609–642.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Gentner, D., Levine, S. C., Dhillon, S., Ping, R., Bradley, C., Isaia, A., & Honke, G. (2016). Rapid learning in a children’s museum via analogical comparison. Cognitive Science, 40, 224–240.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Gentner, D., Loewenstein, J., & Thompson, L. (2003). Learning and transfer: A general role for analogical encoding. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(2), 393–408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Gentner, D., & Markman, A. B. (1994). Structural alignment in comparison: No difference without similarity. Psychological Science, 5(3), 152–158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Gentner, D., & Markman, A. (1997). Structure mapping in analogy and similarity. American Psychologist, 52, 45–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Gentner, D., & Medina, J. (1998). Similarity and the development of rules. Cognition, 65, 263–297.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Gentner, D., & Namy, L. (1999). Comparison in the development of categories. Cognitive Development, 14, 487–513.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Gentner, D., & Namy, L. (2006). Analogical processes in language learning. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15(6), 297–301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Gentner, D., & Toupin, C. (1986). Systematicity and surface similarity in the development of analogy. Cognitive Science, 10, 277–300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Gick, M. L., & Holyoak, K. J. (1983). Schema induction and analogical transfer. Cognitive Psychology, 15(1), 1–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Goldstone, R. L., Day, S., & Son, J. Y. (2010). Comparison. In B. Glatzeder, V. Goel, & A. von Müller (Eds.), Towards a theory of thinking: Building blocks for a conceptual framework (pp. 103–122). Berlin: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Higgins, E. J., & Ross, B. H. (2011). Comparisons in category learning: How best to compare for what. In L. Carlson, C. Holscher, & T. Shipley (Eds.), Proceedings of the 33rd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Austin: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
  37. Hilton, D. J., & Slugoski, B. R. (1986). Knowledge-based causal attribution: The abnormal conditions focus model. Psychological Review, 93(1), 75–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Hitchcock, C., & Knobe, J. (2009). Cause and norm. Journal of Philosophy, 11(11), 587–612.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Kahneman, D., & Miller, D. T. (1986). Norm theory: Comparing reality to its alternatives. Psychological Review, 93(2), 136–153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Kotovsky, L., & Gentner, D. (1996). Comparison and categorization in the development of relational similarity. Child Development, 67, 2797–2822.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Kurtz, K. J., Miao, C., & Gentner, D. (2001). Learning by analogical bootstrapping. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 10(4), 417–446.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Landy, D., & Hummel, J. E. (2010). Explanatory reasoning for inductive confidence. In S. Ohlsson & R. Catrambone (Eds.), Proceedings of the 32nd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 2894–2899). Austin: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
  43. Legare, C. H. (2012). Exploring explanation: Explaining inconsistent evidence informs exploratory, hypothesis-testing behavior in young children. Child Development, 83(1), 173–185.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Legare, C. H. (2014). The contributions of explanation and exploration to children’s scientific reasoning. Child Development Perspectives, 8, 101–106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Legare, C. H., Gelman, S. A., & Wellman, H. M. (2010). Inconsistency with prior knowledge triggers children’s causal explanatory reasoning. Child Development, 81(3), 929–944.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  46. Legare, C. H., & Lombrozo, T. (2014). Selective effects of explanation on learning during early childhood. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 126, 198–212.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Lombrozo, T. (2012). Explanation and abductive inference. In K. J. Holyoak & R. G. Morrison (Eds.), Oxford handbook of thinking and reasoning (pp. 260–276). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  48. Lombrozo, T., & Carey, S. (2006). Functional explanation and the function of explanation. Cognition, 99, 167–204.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  49. Markman, A. B. (1997). Constraints on analogical inference. Cognitive Science, 21(4), 373–418.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Markman, A. B., & Gentner, D. (1993). Splitting the differences: A structural alignment view of similarity. Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 517–535.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Markman, A. B., & Gentner, D. (1996). Commonalities and differences in similarity comparisons. Memory & Cognition, 24(2), 235–249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Nokes-Malach, T. J., VanLehn, K., Belenky, D., Lichtenstein, M., & Cox, G. (2013). Coordinating principles and examples through analogy and self-explanation. European Journal of Education of Psychology, 28(4), 1237–1263.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Olson, D. R. (1970). Cognitive development: The child’s acquisition of diagonality. New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  54. Phillips, J., Luguri, J. B., & Knobe, J. (2015). Unifying morality’s influence on non-moral judgments: The relevance of alternative possibilities. Cognition, 145, 30–42.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  55. Renkl, A. (2014). Toward an instructionally oriented theory of example-based learning. Cognitive Science, 38, 1–37.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Richey, J. E., & Nokes-Malach, T. J. (2015). Comparing four instructional techniques for promoting robust learning. Educational Psychology Review, 27, 181–218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Richey, J. E., Zepeda, C. D., & Nokes-Malach, T. J. (2015). Transfer effects of prompted and self-reported analogical comparison and self-explanation. In D. C. Noelle, R. Dale, A. S. Warlaumont, J. Yoshimi, T. Matlock, C. D. Jennings, & P. P. Maglio (Eds.), Proceedings of the 37th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society. Austin: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
  58. Richland, L. E., Zur, O., & Holyoak, K. (2007). Cognitive supports for analogies in the mathematics classroom. Science, 316, 1128–1129.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  59. Rittle-Johnson, B. (2006). Promoting transfer: Effects of self-explanation and direct instruction. Child Development, 77(1), 1–15.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  60. Rittle-Johnson, B., & Star, J. R. (2007). Does comparing solution methods facilitate conceptual and procedural knowledge? An experimental study on learning to solve equations. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99(3), 561–574.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Rittle-Johnson, B., & Star, J. R. (2009). Compared with what? The effects of different comparisons on conceptual knowledge and procedural flexibility for equation solving. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(3), 529–544.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Rittle-Johnson, B., & Star, J. R. (2011). The power of comparison in learning and instruction: Learning outcomes supported by different types of comparisons. In J. P. Mestre & B. H. Ross (Eds.), Psychology of learning and motivation: Cognition in education (Vol. 55, pp. 199–225). San Diego: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  63. Sagi, E., Gentner, D., & Lovett, A. (2012). What difference reveals about similarity. Cognitive Science, 36(6), 1019–1050.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  64. Schwartz, D. L., Chase, C. C., Oppezzo, M. A., & Chin, D. B. (2011). Practicing versus inventing with contrasting cases: The effects of telling first on learning and transfer. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103(4), 759–775.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Sidney, P. G., Hattikudur, S., & Alibali, M. W. (2015). How do contrasting cases and self-explanation promote learning? Evidence from fraction division. Learning and Instruction, 40, 29–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Siegler, R. S. (2002). Microgenetic studies of self-explanations. In N. Granott & J. Parziale (Eds.), Microdevelopment: Transition processes in development and learning (pp. 31–58). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Spellman, B. A., & Holyoak, K. J. (1996). Pragmatics in analogical mapping. Cognitive Psychology, 31(3), 307–346.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  68. van Fraassen, B. C. (1980). The scientific image. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Weiner, B. (1985). “Spontaneous” causal thinking. Psychological Bulletin, 97(1), 74–84.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  70. Williams, J. J., & Lombrozo, T. (2010). The role of explanation in discovery and generalization: Evidence from category learning. Cognitive Science, 34, 776–806.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  71. Williams, J. J., & Lombrozo, T. (2013). Explanation and prior knowledge interact to guide learning. Cognitive Psychology, 66, 55–84.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  72. Wong, R. M. F., Lawson, M. J., & Keeves, J. (2002). The effects of self-explanation training on students’ problem solving in high-school mathematics. Learning and Instruction, 12(2), 233–262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Xu, F. (2016). Preliminary thoughts on a rational constructivist approach to cognitive development: Primitives, symbols, learning, and thinking. In D. Barner & A. S. Baron (Eds.), Core knowledge and concept change (pp. 11–28). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyNorthwestern UniversityEvanstonUSA

Personalised recommendations