Advertisement

Psychonomic Bulletin & Review

, Volume 22, Issue 3, pp 766–771 | Cite as

Remembering episodic memories is not necessary for forgetting of negative words: Semantic retrieval can cause forgetting of negative words

  • Masanori Kobayashi
  • Yoshihiko Tanno
Brief Report

Abstract

Retrieval of a memory can induce forgetting of other related memories, which is known as retrieval-induced forgetting. Although most studies have investigated retrieval-induced forgetting by remembering episodic memories, this also can occur by remembering semantic memories. The present study shows that retrieval of semantic memories can lead to forgetting of negative words. In two experiments, participants learned words and then engaged in retrieval practice where they were asked to recall words related to the learned words from semantic memory. Finally, participants completed a stem-cued recall test for the learned words. The results showed forgetting of neutral and negative words, which was characteristic of semantic retrieval-induced forgetting. A certain degree of overlapping features, except same learning episode, is sufficient to cause retrieval-induced forgetting of negative words. Given the present results, we conclude that retrieval-induced forgetting of negative words does not require recollection of episodic memories.

Keywords

Cognitive and attentional control Inhibition and memory Memory Episodic memory 

Notes

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by a grant from the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science. We thank Midori Ohkita, Jun Moriya, Mingming Lin, Yoshiko Honma, and Kenji Ikeda for their thoughtful comments on a draft of this manuscript and Motoyuki Sanada for help with data collection.

Supplementary material

13423_2014_719_MOESM1_ESM.pdf (187 kb)
ESM 1 (PDF 187 kb)

References

  1. Amir, N., Badour, C. L., & Freese, B. (2009). The effect of retrieval on recall of information in individuals with posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 23, 535–540. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2008.10.012 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Amir, N., Coles, M. E., Brigidi, B., & Foa, E. B. (2001). The effect of practice on recall of emotional information in individuals with generalized social phobia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 76–82. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.110.1.76 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Anderson, M. C. (2003). Rethinking interference theory: Executive control and the mechanisms of forgetting. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 415–445. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2003.08.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Anderson, M. C., Bjork, R. A., & Bjork, E. L. (1994). Remembering can cause forgetting: Retrieval dynamics in long-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 1063–1087. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.20.5.1063 PubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Anderson, M. C., & Spellman, B. A. (1995). On the status of inhibitory mechanisms in cognition: Memory retrieval as a model case. Psychological Review, 102, 68–100. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.102.1.68 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Barber, S. J., & Mather, M. (2012). Forgetting in context: The effects of age, emotion, and social factors on retrieval-induced forgetting. Memory & Cognition, 40, 874–888. doi: 10.3758/s13421-012-0202-8 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Barnier, A. J., Hung, L., & Conway, M. A. (2004). Retrieval-induced forgetting of emotional and unemotional autobiographical memories. Cognition & Emotion, 18, 457–477. doi: 10.1080/0269993034000392 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bäuml, K.-H. (2002). Semantic generation can cause episodic forgetting. Psychological Science, 13, 356–360. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00464 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Brainard, D. H. (1997). The Psychophysics Toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10, 433–436. doi: 10.1163/156856897X00357 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Cumming, G. (2014). The new statistics: Why and how. Psychological Science, 25, 7–29. doi: 10.1177/0956797613504966 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Dehli, L., & Brennen, T. (2008). Does retrieval-induced forgetting occur for emotional stimuli? Cognition & Emotion, 23, 1056–1068. doi: 10.1080/02699930802285221 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Harris, C. B., Sharman, S. J., Barnier, A. J., & Moulds, M. L. (2010). Mood and retrieval‐induced forgetting of positive and negative autobiographical memories. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 413, 399–413. doi: 10.1002/acp CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Kensinger, E. A. (2004). Remembering emotional experiences: The contribution of valence and arousal. Reviews in the Neurosciences, 15, 241–252. doi: 10.1515/REVNEURO.2004.15.4.241 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Kobayashi, M., & Tanno, Y. (2013). Retrieval-induced forgetting of words with negative emotionality. Memory, 21, 313–323. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2012.725741 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Kuhbandner, C., Bäuml, K.-H., & Stiedl, F. (2009). Retrieval-induced forgetting of negative stimuli: The role of emotional intensity. Cognition & Emotion, 23, 817–830. doi: 10.1080/02699930802204768 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Miyaji, Y., & Yama, H. (2002). Takaikakuristu de kyokioku wo seiseisuru DRM paradaimu notameno nihongorisuto no sakusei [Making Japanese lists which induce false memory at high probability of the DRM paradigm]. The Japanese Journal of Psychonomic Science, 21, 21–25.Google Scholar
  17. Pelli, D. G. (1997). The Video Toolbox software for visual psychophysics: Transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision, 10, 437–442. doi: 10.1163/156856897X00366 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Raaijmakers, J. G. W., & Jakab, E. (2013). Rethinking inhibition theory: On the problematic status of the inhibition theory for forgetting. Journal of Memory and Language, 68, 98–122. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2012.10.002 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Simmons, J., Nelson, L., & Simonsohn, U. (2011). False-positive psychology: Undisclosed flexibility in data collection and analysis allows presenting anything. Psychological Science, 22, 1359–1366. doi: 10.1177/0956797611417632 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Smith, R. E., & Hunt, R. R. (2000). The influence of distinctive processing on retrieval-induced forgetting. Memory & Cognition, 28, 503–508. doi: 10.3758/BF03201240 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Spitzer, B., & Bäuml, K.-H. (2007). Retrieval-induced forgetting in item recognition: Evidence for a reduction in general memory strength. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 863–875. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.33.5.863 PubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Storm, B. C., & Jobe, T. A. (2012). Retrieval-induced forgetting predicts failure to recall negative autobiographical memories. Psychological Science, 23, 1356–1363. doi: 10.1177/0956797612443837 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Takahashi, M. (2001). Istuwari no kioku no jikken notameno joudouka no tyousa [Experimental tests with the Japanese emotional word lists that create false memories]. Seishin Studies, 96, 127–150.Google Scholar
  24. Teasdale, J. D., Fennell, M. J., Hibbert, G. A., & Amies, P. L. (1984). Cognitive therapy for major depressive disorder in primary care. The British Journal of Psychiatry: The Journal of Mental Science, 144, 400–406. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/6372925 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Tempel, T., & Wippich, W. (2012). The processing of inter-item relations as a moderating factor of retrieval-induced forgetting. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 8, 218–225. doi: 10.2478/v10053-008-0117-x
  26. Wessel, I., & Hauer, B. J. A. (2006). Retrieval‐induced forgetting of autobiographical memory details. Cognition & Emotion, 20, 430–447. doi: 10.1080/02699930500342464 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Williams, J. M., & Scott, J. (1988). Autobiographical memory in depression. Psychological Medicine, 18, 689–695. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/3186869 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Graduate School of Arts and SciencesThe University of TokyoTokyoJapan
  2. 2.Graduate School of Environmental StudiesNagoya UniversityNagoya-shiJapan

Personalised recommendations