Psychonomic Bulletin & Review

, Volume 22, Issue 2, pp 366–377 | Cite as

Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory: a meta-analysis

  • Jacky Au
  • Ellen Sheehan
  • Nancy Tsai
  • Greg J. Duncan
  • Martin Buschkuehl
  • Susanne M. Jaeggi
Theoretical Review

Abstract

Working memory (WM), the ability to store and manipulate information for short periods of time, is an important predictor of scholastic aptitude and a critical bottleneck underlying higher-order cognitive processes, including controlled attention and reasoning. Recent interventions targeting WM have suggested plasticity of the WM system by demonstrating improvements in both trained and untrained WM tasks. However, evidence on transfer of improved WM into more general cognitive domains such as fluid intelligence (Gf) has been more equivocal. Therefore, we conducted a meta-analysis focusing on one specific training program, n-back. We searched PubMed and Google Scholar for all n-back training studies with Gf outcome measures, a control group, and healthy participants between 18 and 50 years of age. In total, we included 20 studies in our analyses that met our criteria and found a small but significant positive effect of n-back training on improving Gf. Several factors that moderate this transfer are identified and discussed. We conclude that short-term cognitive training on the order of weeks can result in beneficial effects in important cognitive functions as measured by laboratory tests.

Keywords

Cognitive training Transfer Plasticity 

Supplementary material

13423_2014_699_Fig5_ESM.jpg (123 kb)
ESM 1

(JPEG 122 kb)

13423_2014_699_Fig6_ESM.jpg (129 kb)
ESM 2

(JPEG 129 kb)

13423_2014_699_Fig7_ESM.jpg (124 kb)
ESM 3

(JPEG 123 kb)

13423_2014_699_Fig8_ESM.jpg (134 kb)
ESM 4

(JPEG 133 kb)

13423_2014_699_Fig9_ESM.jpg (115 kb)
ESM 5

(JPEG 114 kb)

13423_2014_699_Fig10_ESM.jpg (118 kb)
ESM 6

(JPEG 117 kb)

13423_2014_699_MOESM1_ESM.pdf (53 kb)
Table S1(PDF 52 kb)
13423_2014_699_MOESM2_ESM.pdf (80 kb)
Table S2(PDF 79 kb)
13423_2014_699_MOESM3_ESM.pdf (105 kb)
Table S3(PDF 105 kb)

References

  1. Ackerman, P. L., Beier, M. E., & Boyle, M. O. (2005). Working memory and intelligence: the same or different constructs? Psychological Bulletin, 131(1), 30–60. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.131.1.30 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Basak, C., Boot, W. R., Voss, M. W., & Kramer, A. F. (2008). Can training in a real-time strategy video game attenuate cognitive decline in older adults? Psychology and Aging, 23(4), 765–777.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Binet, A. (1909). Les idées modernes sur les enfants. Paris: Ernest Flammarion Paris.Google Scholar
  4. Bobko, P., Roth, P. L., & Bobko, C. (2001). Correcting the effect size of d for range restriction and unreliability. Organizational Research Methods, 4(1), 46–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Borenstein, M., Higgins, J., & Rothstein, H. (2005). Comprehensive meta-analysis version 2. Englewood: Biostat.Google Scholar
  6. Borenstein, M., Hedges, L., Higgins, J., & Rothstein, H. (2009). Introduction to Meta-Analysis. West Sussex: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Burgess, G. C., Gray, J. R., Conway, A. R., & Braver, T. S. (2011). Neural mechanisms of interference control underlie the relationship between fluid intelligence and working memory span. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 140(4), 674–692. doi:10.1037/a0024695 CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Buschkuehl, M., & Jaeggi, S. M. (2010). Improving intelligence: a literature review. Swiss Medical Weekly, 140(19–20), 266–272.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Buschkuehl, M., Hernandez-Garcia, L., Jaeggi, S. M., Bernard, J. A., & Jonides, J. (2014). Neural effects of short-term training on working memory. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 14(1), 147–160. doi:10.3758/s13415-013-0244-9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Cameron, J., Banko, K. M., & Pierce, W. D. (2001). Pervasive negative effects of rewards on intrinsic motivation: The myth continues. Behavior Analyst, 24(1), 1–44.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Caruso, D., Taylor, J., & Detterman, D. (1982). Intelligence research and intelligent policy. In D. Detterman & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), How and how much can intelligence be increased. Norwood: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  12. Dahlin, E., Backman, L., Neely, A. S., & Nyberg, L. (2009). Training of the executive component of working memory: subcortical areas mediate transfer effects. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 27(5), 405–419. doi:10.3233/RNN-2009-0492 PubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Deci, E. L., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R. M. (1999). A meta-analytic review of experiments examining the effects of extrinsic rewards on intrinsic motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 125(6), 627–668. discussion 692–700.Google Scholar
  14. Detterman, D. K. (1993). The case for prosecution: Transfer as an epiphenomenon. In D. K. D. R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Transfer on trial: Intelligence, cognition, and instruction. Norwood: Ablex Publishing Corporation.Google Scholar
  15. Dickens, W. T., & Flynn, J. R. (2001). Heritability estimates versus large environmental effects: the IQ paradox resolved. Psychology Review, 108(2), 346–369.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Duckworth, A. L., Peterson, C., Matthews, M. D., & Kelly, D. R. (2007). Grit: perseverance and passion for long-term goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92(6), 1087–1101. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.92.6.1087 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Dunst, C. J., Hamby, D. W., & Trivette, C. M. (2004). Guidelines for Calculating Effect Sizes for Practice-Based Research Syntheses. Centerscope, 3(1), 1–10.Google Scholar
  18. Egger, M., Davey Smith, G., Schneider, M., & Minder, C. (1997). Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ, 315(7109), 629–634.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Fritz, C. O., Morris, P. E., & Richler, J. J. (2012). Effect size estimates: Current use, calculations, and interpretation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141(1), 2-18. doi:10.1037/A0026092 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Galton, F. (1892). Hereditary genius: An inquiry into its laws and consequences. London: Macmillan and Co.Google Scholar
  21. Gottfredson, L. S. (1997). Why g matters: The complexity of everyday life. Intelligence, 24(1), 79–132. doi:10.1016/S0160-2896(97)90014-3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Gray, J. R., & Thompson, P. M. (2004). Neurobiology of intelligence: science and ethics. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5(6), 471–482. doi:10.1038/nrn1405 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Harrison, T. L., Shipstead, Z., Hicks, K. L., Hambrick, D. Z., Redick, T. S., & Engle, R. W. (2013). Working memory training may increase working memory capacity but not fluid intelligence. Psychological Science, 24(12), 2409–2419. doi:10.1177/0956797613492984 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Heinzel, S., Schulte, S., Onken, J., Duong, Q. L., Riemer, T. G., Heinz, A., & Rapp, M. A. (2014). Working memory training improvements and gains in non-trained cognitive tasks in young and older adults. Neuropsychology, Development, and Cognition. Section B, Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition, 21(2), 146–173. doi:10.1080/13825585.2013.790338 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Higgins, J. P., Thompson, S. G., Deeks, J. J., & Altman, D. G. (2003). Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ, 327(7414), 557–560. doi:10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557 CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Higgins JPT, Green S (editors). (2011). Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions Version 5.1.0 [updated March 2011]. The Cochrane Collaboration, 2011. Available from www.cochrane-handbook.org
  27. Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Jonides, J., & Perrig, W. J. (2008). Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105(19), 6829–6833. doi:10.1073/pnas.0801268105 CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Jaeggi, S. M., Studer-Luethi, B., Buschkuehl, M., Su, Y. F., Jonides, J., & Perrig, W. J. (2010). The relationship between n-back performance and matrix reasoning - implications for training and transfer. Intelligence, 38(6), 625–635. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2010.09.001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Shah, P., & Jonides, J. (2014). The role of individual differences in cognitive training and transfer. Memory and Cognition, 42(3), 464–480Google Scholar
  30. Kane, M. J., Hambrick, D. Z., & Conway, A. R. (2005). Working memory capacity and fluid intelligence are strongly related constructs: comment on Ackerman, Beier, and Boyle (2005). Psychological Bulletin, 131(1), 66–71.Google Scholar
  31. Ketay, S., Aron, A., & Hedden, T. (2009). Culture and attention: evidence from brain and behavior. Progress in Brain Research, 178, 79–92. doi:10.1016/S0079-6123(09)17806-8 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Kundu, B., Sutterer, D. W., Emrich, S. M., & Postle, B. R. (2013). Strengthened effective connectivity underlies transfer of working memory training to tests of short-term memory and attention. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(20), 8705–8715. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5565-12.2013 CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Leys, C., Ley, C., Klein, O., Bernard, P., & Licata, L. (2013). Detecting outliers: Do not use standard deviation around the mean, use absolute deviation around the median. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49(4), 764–766. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2013.03.013 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Muggleton, N. G., & Banissy, M. J. (2014). Culture and cognition. Cognitive Neuroscience, 5(1), 1–2. doi:10.1080/17588928.2014.885781 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Nisbett, R. E., & Norenzayan, A. (2002). Culture and Cognition. In D. L. Medin (Ed.), Stevens' Handbook of Experimental Psychology, Third EditionVol 3: Memory and Cognitive Processes (pp. 561–598). Wiley Online Library: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.Google Scholar
  36. Nisbett, R. E., Aronson, J., Blair, C., Dickens, W., Flynn, J., Halpern, D. F., & Turkheimer, E. (2012). Intelligence: new findings and theoretical developments. American Psychologist, 67(2), 130–159. doi:10.1037/a0026699 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Oberauer, K., Schulze, R., Wilhelm, O., & Suss, H. M. (2005). Working memory and intelligence--their correlation and their relation: comment on Ackerman, Beier, and Boyle. Psychological Bulletin, 131(1), 61–65. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.131.1.61. author reply 72–65.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Oelhafen, S., Nikolaidis, A., Padovani, T., Blaser, D., Koenig, T., & Perrig, W. J. (2013). Increased parietal activity after training of interference control. Neuropsychologia, 51(13), 2781–2790. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.08.012 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Orwin, R. G. (1983). A fail safe N for effect size in meta-analysis. Journal for Educational Statistics, 8(2), 157–159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Redick, T. S., Shipstead, Z., Harrison, T. L., Hicks, K. L., Fried, D. E., Hambrick, D. Z., & Engle, R. W. (2013). No evidence of intelligence improvement after working memory training: a randomized, placebo-controlled study. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 142(2), 359–379. doi:10.1037/a0029082 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Rosenthal, R. (1991). Meta-analytic procedures for social research. London: Sage.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Rudebeck, S. R., Bor, D., Ormond, A., O'Reilly, J. X., & Lee, A. C. (2012). A potential spatial working memory training task to improve both episodic memory and fluid intelligence. PLoS One, 7(11), e50431. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0050431 CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Salminen, T., Strobach, T., & Schubert, T. (2012). On the impacts of working memory training on executive functioning. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 166. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2012.00166 CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Salomon, G., & Perkins, D. N. (1989). Rocky Roads to Transfer - Rethinking Mechanisms of a Neglected Phenomenon. Educational Psychologist, 24(2), 113–142. doi:10.1207/s15326985ep2402_1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Schwarb, H. (2012). Optimized Cogntive Training: Investigating the Limits of Brain Training on Generalized Cognitive Function (Doctoral Dissertation). (Doctoral Dissertation), Georgia Institute of Technology.Google Scholar
  46. Schweizer, S., Hampshire, A., & Dalgleish, T. (2011). Extending brain-training to the affective domain: increasing cognitive and affective executive control through emotional working memory training. PLoS One, 6(9), e24372. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0024372 CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Shipstead, Z., Redick, T. S., & Engle, R. W. (2012). Is working memory training effective? Psychological Bulletin, 138(4), 628–654. doi:10.1037/a0027473 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Stankov, L. (1986). Kvashchev Experiment - Can We Boost Intelligence. Intelligence, 10(3), 209–230. doi:10.1016/0160-2896(86)90016-4 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Stepankova, H., Lukavsky, J., Buschkuehl, M., Kopecek, M., Ripova, D., & Jaeggi, S. M. (2014). The malleability of working memory and visuospatial skills: a randomized controlled study in older adults. Developmental Psychology, 50(4), 1049–1059. doi:10.1037/a0034913 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Stephenson, C. L., & Halpern, D. F. (2013). Improved matrix reasoning is limited to training on tasks with a visuospatial component. Intelligence, 41(5), 341–357. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2013.05.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Sterne, J. A., Gavaghan, D., & Egger, M. (2000). Publication and related bias in meta-analysis: power of statistical tests and prevalence in the literature. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 53(11), 1119–1129.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. Thompson, T. W., Waskom, M. L., Garel, K. L., Cardenas-Iniguez, C., Reynolds, G. O., Winter, R., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2013). Failure of working memory training to enhance cognition or intelligence. PLoS One, 8(5), e63614. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0063614 CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Tomic, W., & Klauer, K. J. (1996). On the effects of training inductive reasoning: How far does it transfer and how long do the effects persist? European Journal of Psychology of Education, 11(3), 283–299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Viera, A. J., & Garrett, J. M. (2005). Understanding interobserver agreement: the kappa statistic. Family Medicine, 37(5), 360–363.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  55. von Bastian, C. C., & Oberauer, K. (2013). Effects and mechanisms of working memory training: a review. Psychological Research. doi:10.1007/s00426-013-0524-6 Google Scholar
  56. Wiley, J., Jarosz, A. F., Cushen, P. J., & Colflesh, G. J. H. (2011). New Rule Use Drives the Relation Between Working Memory Capacity and Raven's Advanced Progressive Matrices. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 37(1), 256–263. doi:10.1037/A0021613 Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  • Jacky Au
    • 1
  • Ellen Sheehan
    • 1
  • Nancy Tsai
    • 1
  • Greg J. Duncan
    • 1
  • Martin Buschkuehl
    • 2
  • Susanne M. Jaeggi
    • 1
  1. 1.School of EducationUniversity of California, IrvineIrvineUSA
  2. 2.MIND Research InstituteIrvineUSA

Personalised recommendations