Abstract
Melody recognition entails the encoding of pitch intervals between successive notes. While it has been shown that a whole melodic sequence is better encoded than the sum of its constituent intervals, the underlying reasons have remained opaque. Here, we compared listeners’ accuracy in encoding the relative pitch distance between two notes (for example, C, E) of an interval to listeners accuracy under the following three modifications: (1) doubling the duration of each note (C – E –), (2) repetition of each note (C, C, E, E), and (3) adding a preceding note (G, C, E). Repeating (2) or adding an extra note (3) improved encoding of relative pitch distance when the melodic sequences were transposed to other keys, but lengthening the duration (1) did not improve encoding relative to the standard two-note interval sequences. Crucially, encoding accuracy was higher with the four-note sequences than with long two-note sequences despite the fact that sensory (pitch) information was held constant. We interpret the results to show that re-forming the Gestalts of two-note intervals into two-note “melodies” results in more accurate encoding of relational pitch information due to a richer structural context in which to embed the interval.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.



References
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker. B., Walker S. (2013). lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4. R package version 1.0-5. http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4
Bigand, E., Poulin, B., Tillmann, B., Madurell, F., & D'Adamo, D. A. (2003). Sensory versus cognitive components in harmonic priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 29(1), 159–171. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.29.1.159
Collins, T., Tillmann, B., Barrett, F. S., Delbe, C., & Janata, P. (2014). A combined model of sensory and cognitive representations underlying tonal expectations in music: From audio signals to behavior. Psychological Review, 121(1), 33–65. doi:10.1037/a0034695
Cuddy, L. L., & Cohen, A. J. (1976). Recognition of transposed melodic sequences. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 28(May), 255–270.
Deutsch, D. (1969). Music recognition. Psychological Review, 76(3), 300–307.
Dowling, W. J. (1986). Context effects on melody recognition - scale-step versus interval representations. Music Perception, 3(3), 281–296.
Dowling, W. J., Bartlett, J. C., Halpern, A. R., & Andrews, M. W. (2008). Melody recognition at fast and slow tempos: Effects of age, experience, and familiarity. Perception & Psychophysics, 70(3), 496–502. doi:10.3758/Pp.70.3.496
Edworthy, J. (1985). Interval and contour in melody processing. Music Perception, 2(3), 375–388.
Enns, J. T., & Prinzmetal, W. (1984). The role of redundancy in the object-line effect. Perception & Psychophysics, 35(1), 22–32.
Jaeger, T. F. (2008). Categorical data analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards Logit Mixed Models. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 434–446. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2007.11.007
Jones, M. R. (1976). Time, our lost dimension - toward a new theory of perception, attention, and memory. Psychological Review, 83(5), 323–355.
Jones, M. R., Moynihan, H., MacKenzie, N., & Puente, J. (2002). Temporal aspects of stimulus-driven attending in dynamic arrays. Psychological Science, 13(4), 313–319.
Koelsch, S., & Siebel, W. A. (2005). Towards a neural basis of music perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(12), 578–584. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.10.001
Krumhansl, C. L. (1990). Cognitive foundations of musical pitch. New York: Oxford University Press.
Kubovy, M., & Van Valkenburg, D. (2001). Auditory and visual objects. [Research Support, U.S. Gov't, P.H.S. Review]. Cognition, 80(1-2), 97–126.
Leman, M. (2000). An auditory model of the role of short-term memory in probe-tone ratings. Music Perception, 17(4), 481–509.
Mcclelland, J. L. (1978). Perception and masking of wholes and parts. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 4(2), 210–223.
Neuhaus, C., & Knösche, T. R. (2006). Processing of rhythmic and melodic gestalts—An ERP study. Music Perception, 24(2), 209–222. doi:10.1525/Mp.2006.24.2.209
Plantinga, J., & Trainor, L. J. (2005). Memory for melody: Infants use a relative pitch code. Cognition, 98(1), 1–11. doi:10.1016/J.Cognition.2004.09.008
Richler, J. J., Cheung, O. S., & Gauthier, I. (2011). Holistic processing predicts face recognition. [Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural Research Support, U.S. Gov't, Non-P.H.S.]. Psychological Science, 22(4), 464–471. doi:10.1177/0956797611401753
Schindler, A., Herdener, M., & Bartels, A. (2012). Coding of melodic Gestalt in human auditory cortex. Cerebral Cortex. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhs289
Stewart, L., Overath, T., Warren, J. D., Foxton, J. M., & Griffiths, T. D. (2008). fMRI evidence for a cortical hierarchy of pitch pattern processing. [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't]. PloS One, 3(1), e1470. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0001470
Trainor, L. J., McDonald, K. L., & Alain, C. (2002). Automatic and controlled processing of melodic contour and interval information measured by electrical brain activity. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(3), 430–442. doi:10.1162/089892902317361949
Warrier, C. M., & Zatorre, R. J. (2002). Influence of tonal context and timbral variation on perception of pitch. [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't]. Perception & Psychophysics, 64(2), 198–207.
Williams, A., & Weisstein, N. (1978). Line segments are perceived better in a coherent context than alone - object-line effect in visual-perception. Memory & Cognition, 6(2), 85–90.
Winter, B. (2013). Linear models and linear mixed effects models in R with linguistic applications. arXiv:1308.5499. [http://arxiv.org/pdf/1308.5499.pdf]
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to thank Cory Kendrick, Kevin Miller, and Samuel Lloyd for their great help on data collection. We thank Bodo Winter for providing the helpful tutorial on the linear mixed effects modeling in R and his advice on the analysis for our study via personal communication with us. Yune-Sang Lee’s special thanks go to Prof. Jay Hull for his enormous and unconditional help on other statistical analyses. Lastly, we are truly grateful to the reviewing editor—Dr. Bob McMurray—and two anonymous reviewers for their great comments and suggestions.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Lee, YS., Janata, P., Frost, C. et al. Melody recognition revisited: influence of melodic Gestalt on the encoding of relational pitch information. Psychon Bull Rev 22, 163–169 (2015). https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-014-0653-y
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-014-0653-y
Keywords
- Music
- Melody
- Gestalt
- Interval
- Pitch
- Recognition