Exogenous orienting of crossmodal attention in 3-D space: Support for a depth-aware crossmodal attentional system

Abstract

The aim of the present study was to investigate exogenous crossmodal orienting of attention in three-dimensional (3-D) space. Most studies in which the orienting of attention has been examined in 3-D space concerned either exogenous intramodal or endogenous crossmodal attention. Evidence for exogenous crossmodal orienting of attention in depth is lacking. Endogenous and exogenous attention are behaviorally different, suggesting that they are two different mechanisms. We used the orthogonal spatial-cueing paradigm and presented auditory exogenous cues at one of four possible locations in near or far space before the onset of a visual target. Cues could be presented at the same (valid) or at a different (invalid) depth from the target (radial validity), and on the same (valid) or on a different (invalid) side (horizontal validity), whereas we blocked the depth at which visual targets were presented. Next to an overall validity effect (valid RTs < invalid RTs) in horizontal space, we observed an interaction between the horizontal and radial validity of the cue: The horizontal validity effect was present only when the cue and the target were presented at the same depth. No horizontal validity effect was observed when the cue and the target were presented at different depths. These results suggest that exogenous crossmodal attention is “depth-aware,” and they are discussed in the context of the supramodal hypothesis of attention.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in to check access.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Notes

  1. 1.

    Ten participants who were not part of the main experiment sample (five female, five male; mean age = 25.70 years, SD = 3.13) were tested with a four-choice localization task in the same setup as in the main experiment. They were instructed to maintain fixation on a black fixation cross (0.5º × 0.5º, 0.20 cd/m2) presented on a light gray background (4.79 cd/m2) in near space (80 cm). The average accuracy of 3-D localization was significantly above chance (M = .55, SD = .16) [t(9) = −6.00, p < .001].

  2. 2.

    Note that we did not track eye movements, and therefore that we cannot draw any conclusions about whether any exogenous cueing effects were due to covert or overt exogenous orienting of attention. The results will still be able to indicate whether attention can be exogenously shifted in 3-D space in a crossmodal way.

References

  1. Andersen, R. A. (1997). Multimodal integration for the representation of space in the posterior parietal cortex. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 352, 1421–1428.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Atchley, P., Kramer, A. F., Andersen, G. J., & Theeuwes, J. (1997). Spatial cuing in a stereoscopic display: Evidence for a “depth-aware” attentional focus. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4, 524–529. doi:10.3758/BF03214343

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Bauer, D., Plinge, A., Ehrenstein, W. H., Rinkenauer, G., & Grosjean, M. (2011). Spatial orienting of attention in stereo depth. Psychological Research, 76, 730–735.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Berger, A., Henik, A., & Rafal, R. (2005). Competition between endogenous and exogenous orienting of visual attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134, 207–221. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.134.2.207

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Bronkhorst, A. W., & Houtgast, T. (1999). Auditory distance perception in rooms. Nature, 397, 517–520.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Chen, Q., Weidner, R., Vossel, S., Weiss, P. H., & Fink, G. R. (2012). Neural mechanisms of attentional reorienting in three-dimensional space. Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 13352–13362.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Cousineau, D. (2005). Confidence intervals in within-subject designs: a simpler solution to Loftus and Masson’s method. Tutorials in Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 1, 42–45.

    Google Scholar 

  8. Couyoumdjian, A., Di Nocera, F., & Ferlazzo, F. (2003). Functional representation of 3D space in endogenous attention shifts. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56A, 155–183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Downing, C. J., & Pinker, S. (1985). The spatial structure of visual attention. In M. I. Posner & O. S. M. Marin (Eds.), Attention and performance XI (pp. 171–187). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  10. Eimer, M., & Van Velzen, J. (2002). Crossmodal links in spatial attention are mediated by supramodal control processes: evidence from event-related potentials. Psychophysiology, 39, 437–449.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Farah, M. J., Wong, A. B., Monheit, M. A., & Morrow, L. A. (1989). Parietal lobe mechanisms of spatial attention: Modality-specific or supramodal? Neuropsychologia, 27, 461–470.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Frens, M. A., & Van Opstal, A. J. (1995). A quantitative study of auditory-evoked saccadic eye movements in two dimensions. Experimental Brain Research, 107, 103–117.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Gardner, J. L., Merriam, E. P., Movshon, J. A., & Heeger, D. J. (2008). Maps of visual space in human occipital cortex are retinotopic, not spatiotopic. Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 3988–3999.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Ho, C., & Spence, C. (2005). Assessing the effectiveness of various auditory cues in capturing a driver’s visual attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 11, 157–174.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Kim, Y., Gitelman, D. R., Nobre, A. C., Parrish, T. B., LaBar, K. S., & Mesulam, M. M. (1999). The large-scale neural network for spatial attention displays multifunctional overlap but differential asymmetry. NeuroImage, 9, 269–277.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Macaluso, E., & Driver, J. (2005). Multisensory spatial interactions: A window onto functional integration in the human brain. Trends in Neurosciences, 28, 264–271.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Macaluso, E., Frith, C. D., & Driver, J. (2002). Supramodal effects of cover spatial orienting triggered by visual or tactile events. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 389–401.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Spence, C., & Driver, J. (1997). Audiovisual links in exogenous covert spatial orienting. Perception & Psychophysics, 59, 1–22. doi:10.3758/BF03206843

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Spence, C., & MacDonald, J. (2004). The cross-modal consequences of the exogenous spatial orienting of attention. In G. A. Calvert, C. Spence, & B. E. Stein (Eds.), The handbook of multisensory processes (1st ed., pp. 3–26). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  20. Theeuwes, J., & Pratt, J. (2003). Inhibition of return spreads across 3-D space. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10, 616–620. doi:10.3758/BF03196523

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author Note

The authors thank Jesse Smit, Marieke Janssen, and Jorinde Duits for their assistance in collecting data. This research was funded by two grants from the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research: Grant Nos. 451-09-019 (to S.V.d.S.) and 451-10-013 (to T.C.W.N.).

Author information

Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Nathan Van der Stoep.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and Permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Van der Stoep, N., Nijboer, T.C.W. & Van der Stigchel, S. Exogenous orienting of crossmodal attention in 3-D space: Support for a depth-aware crossmodal attentional system. Psychon Bull Rev 21, 708–714 (2014). https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-013-0532-y

Download citation

Keywords

  • Cueing
  • Exogenous
  • Orthogonal
  • Space
  • 3-D
  • Attention