Psychonomic Bulletin & Review

, Volume 21, Issue 3, pp 701–707 | Cite as

Attentional capture by evaluative stimuli: Gain- and loss-connoting colors boost the additional-singleton effect

Brief Report

Abstract

In a valence induction task, one color acquired positive valence by indicating the chance to win money (in the case of fast and correct responses), and a different color acquired negative valence by indicating the danger to lose money (in the case of slow or incorrect responses). In the additional-singleton trials of a visual search task, the task-irrelevant singleton color was either the positive one, the negative one, or one of two neutral colors. We found an additional-singleton effect (i.e., longer RTs with a singleton color than in the no-singleton control condition). This effect was significantly increased for the two valent colors (with no differences between them) relative to the two neutral colors (with no differences between them, either). This result favors the hypothesis that the general relevance of stimuli elicits attentional capture, rather than the negativity bias hypothesis.

Keywords

Attentional capture Additional singleton Valence Relevance Emotion 

References

  1. Anderson, B. A., Laurent, P. A., & Yantis, S. (2011). Value-driven attentional capture. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108, 10367–10371. doi:10.1073/pnas.1104047108 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Anderson, B. A., & Yantis, S. (2013). Persistence of value-driven attentional capture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39, 6–9. doi:10.1037/a0030860 PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Bermeitinger, C., Kuhlmann, M., & Wentura, D. (2012). Reading a standing wave: Figure–ground-alternation masking of primes in evaluative priming. Consciousness and Cognition, 21, 1109–1121. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2012.03.015 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Brosch, T., Sander, D., Pourtois, G., & Scherer, K. R. (2008). Beyond fear: Rapid spatial orienting toward positive emotional stimuli. Psychological Science, 19, 362–370.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Brosch, T., & Sharma, D. (2005). The role of fear-relevant stimuli in visual search: A comparison of phylogenetic and ontogenetic stimuli. Emotion, 5, 360–364.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Buchner, A., Rothermund, K., Wentura, D., & Mehl, B. (2004). Valence of distractor words increases the effects of irrelevant speech on serial recall. Memory & Cognition, 32, 722–731.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Fazio, R. H., Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Powell, M. C., & Kardes, F. R. (1986). On the automatic activation of attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 229–238. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.50.2.229 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Fox, E., Russo, R., & Dutton, K. (2002). Attentional bias for threat: Evidence for delayed disengagement from emotional faces. Cognition and Emotion, 16, 355–379.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Hickey, C., Chelazzi, L., & Theeuwes, J. (2010). Reward changes salience in human vision via the anterior cingulate. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 11096–11103. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1026-10.2010 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Hickey, C., Chelazzi, L., & Theeuwes, J. (2011). Reward has a residual impact on target selection in visual search, but not on the suppression of distractors. Visual Cognition, 19, 117–128. doi:10.1080/13506285.2010.503946 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Notebaert, L., Crombez, G., Van Damme, S., De Houwer, J., & Theeuwes, J. (2011). Signals of threat do not capture, but prioritize, attention: A conditioning approach. Emotion, 11, 81–89. doi:10.1037/a0021286 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Nummenmaa, L., Hyönä, J., & Calvo, M. G. (2009). Emotional scene content drives the saccade generation system reflexively. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 305–323. doi:10.1037/a0013626 PubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Öhman, A., Flykt, A., & Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: Detecting the snake in the grass. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 466–478. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.130.3.466 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Öhman, A., & Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness: Toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychological Review, 108, 483–522.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Olivers, C. N. L., Meijer, F., & Theeuwes, J. (2006). Feature-based memory-driven attentional capture: Visual working memory content affects visual attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 1243–1265. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.32.5.1243 PubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Petrova, K., & Wentura, D. (2012). Upper-lower visual field asymmetries in oculomotor inhibition of emotional distractors. Vision Research, 62, 209–219.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Purkis, H. M., Lester, K. J., & Field, A. P. (2011). But what about the Empress of Racnoss? The allocation of attention to spiders and Doctor Who in a visual search task is predicted by fear and expertise. Emotion, 11, 1484–1488.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Rothermund, K. (2003). Motivation and attention: Incongruent effects of feedback on the processing of valence. Emotion, 3, 223–238.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Rothermund, K., Voss, A., & Wentura, D. (2008). Counter-regulation in affective attentional biases: A basic mechanism that warrants flexibility in emotion and motivation. Emotion, 8, 34–46. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.8.1.34 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Theeuwes, J. (1992). Perceptual selectivity for color and form. Perception & Psychophysics, 51, 599–606. doi:10.3758/BF03211656 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Tukey, J. W. (1977). Exploratory data analysis. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
  22. Wentura, D., & Degner, J. (2010). Practical guide to sequential priming and related tasks. In B. Gawronski & B. K. Payne (Eds.), Handbook of implicit social cognition: Measurement, theory, and applications (pp. 95–116). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  23. Wentura, D., Rothermund, K., & Bak, P. (2000). Automatic vigilance: The attention-grabbing power of approach- and avoidance-related social information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 1024–1037.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Wentura, D., Voss, A., & Rothermund, K. (2009). Playing TETRIS for science: Counter-regulatory affective processing in a motivationally “hot” context. Acta Psychologica, 131, 171–177. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.05.008 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Yiend, J. (2010). The effects of emotion on attention: A review of attentional processing of emotional information. Cognition and Emotion, 24, 3–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  • Dirk Wentura
    • 1
  • Philipp Müller
    • 1
  • Klaus Rothermund
    • 2
  1. 1.Department of PsychologySaarland UniversitySaarbrückenGermany
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyUniversity of JenaJenaGermany

Personalised recommendations