Lexical decision task
Accuracy and response time data from the lexical decision task were each analyzed in a noise by word frequency ANOVA. There was no effect of noise on accuracy (M
none = 96.8%, se = 1.1%, M
low = 95.1%, se = .1.8%, M
high = 97.2%, se = 0.7%), F < 1; but accuracy was higher for high-frequency words (M
H = 98.5%, se = 0.8%, M
L = 94.3%, se = 1.0%), F(1, 35) = 26.50, p < 0.001. For response time (based on correct responses), the main effects of noise (M
none = 893 ms, se = 41, M
low = 977 ms, se = 50, M
high = 1,195 ms, se = 67), F(2, 70) = 14.58, p < 0.001, and word frequency (M
H = 926ms, se = 43, M
L = 1,116 ms, se = 45), F(1, 35) = 53.79, p < 0.001, were reliable; but the interaction was not, F < 1. These findings indicated that readers could precisely identify isolated words in noise, but that noise decreased lexical processing speed, an effect likely to occur at the character recognition stage of processing (Pelli, Farell, & Moore, 2003).
Reading time
Reading time per word increased as noise increased, F(2, 70) = 14.10, p < 0.001, with reading times of 561 ms (se = 25), 581ms (se = 22), and 637 ms (se = 29) for no-, low-, and high-noise conditions, respectively.
Patterns of resource allocation
Regression analysis of reading times onto linguistic features was used to decompose the reading time into attention (time) allocated to component processes (e.g., Lorch, & Myers, 1990) underlying sentence understanding. In order to estimate resources allocated to different computations needed for sentence processing, each word in the sentence was coded in terms of an array of word-level and textbase-level features. The word-level features included the number of syllables and log word frequency of each word in the sentence, in order to estimate the time allocated to orthographic decoding and to word meaning access, respectively. Conceptual integration of the textbase was operationalized as the responsiveness of reading time to two variables. First, each word was dummy-coded (0/1) for whether it was a newly introduced noun concept in the sentence. An increase in reading time as a function of this variable provides an estimate of the time allocated to immediate processing of conceptual information (35.1% of the words were coded as new concepts). Second, the cumulative conceptual load at sentence boundaries was calculated by multiplying the total number of concepts introduced in the sentence by the dummy-coded variable for the sentence-final word (1 out of 18 = 5.6% of the total words), to estimate conceptual integration at sentence wrap-up (Haberlandt et al., 1986; Stine-Morrow et al., 2010). Reading times for each participant within each visual noise condition were decomposed by regressing them onto these four features to operationalize attention allocated to various levels of sentence processing. Note that using the product term to isolate wrap-up term produces a coefficient that represents the time per new concept allocated at that point; thus, estimates of resource allocation for immediacy in conceptual integration and wrap-up were on the same scale. This collection of variables represented essential sources of engagement for word and textbase processing; these variables were also minimally correlated to avoid multicollinearity.
Individual parameters
The coefficients from individual regressions were trimmed such that regression coefficients greater than 3SD away from the group mean within each condition were replaced with that mean (<2.1% data). Means and standard errors for individual parameters are presented in Table 1. Each coefficient may be interpreted as an estimate of time allocated to a particular type of computation while controlling for the impact of other demands. For example, referring to Table 1, reading time in the no-noise increased by 30 ms per syllable, controlling for the effects of word frequency, new concepts and sentence boundary wrap-up on reading time.
Table 1 Mean allocation parameters (ms) (and standard errors) as a function of visual noise (parameters were compared to zero in single-sample t tests)
Consistent with the Effortfulness Hypothesis, the effect of word frequency on resource allocation was exaggerated by noise, F(2, 70) = 3.50, p < 0.05, and the sentence wrap-up effect was reduced, F(2, 70) = 3.62, p < 0.05. With noise, more time was allocated to lexical access and less time was allocated to conceptual integration. There were also nonsignificant trends for increased allocation to orthographic decoding and decreased allocation for processing new concepts with increasing noise, F < 1 for both.
Construct-level analyses
To specifically test the key implication of the Effortfulness Hypothesis that word-level and textbase-level processing would diverge in noise, composite scores were created in order to get reliable estimates of word-level and textbase-level processing at the construct level. We obtained the index of word-level processing by averaging z-scores of syllable and reverse-coded log word frequency parameters and the index of textbase-level processing by averaging z-scores of new concept and sentence wrap-up parameters. These indices were analyzed in a 3 (noise intensity) × 2 (level of processing) repeated measures ANOVAFootnote 2, in which both noise and level of sentence processing were within-subject variables. The interaction between noise and level was reliable, F(2, 70) = 5.21, p < 0.01 (Fig. 1). Consistent with the Effortfulness Hypothesis, noise tended to increase word-level processing, F(2, 70) = 2.96, p = 0.06, and to significantly decrease textbase-level processing, F(2, 70) = 3.95, p < 0.05.
Recall performance
A propositional analysis of the 24 sentences yielded 172 propositions. Visual noise did not have an effect on overall propositional recall, F1/2 < 1. To assess the effects of noise on recall in a more fine-grained way, we examined the quality of recall with a memorability analysisFootnote 3 (Hartley, 1993; Rubin, 1985; Stine & Wingfield, 1988). We divided the propositions into three memorability groups based on the normative probability of recall in the no-noise condition. Recall was then analyzed in 3 (Noise: none, low, high) × 3 (MemorabilityFootnote 4: low, medium, high) repeated measures ANOVA. Noise did not affect overall recall, F1/2 < 1. However, the Noise by Memorability interaction was highly reliable in both subject and item analyses, F1(4, 136) = 3.21, p < 0.05; F2(4, 338) = 4.80, p = .001. Visual noise tended to disproportionally disrupt recall of more core propositions, F1(2, 70) = 1.70, p = 0.19; F2(2, 100) = 4.52, p < 0.05, and concomitantly increased recall of less central propositions in text, F1(2, 70) = 3.99, p < 0.05; F2(2, 114) = 5.87, p < 0.01, whereas recall of moderately memorable propositions remained unaffected, F1/2 < 1 (Table 2).Footnote 5
Table 2 Mean recall (%) (and standard errors) as a function of visual noise and propositional memorability