Skip to main content
Log in

The drawing effect: Evidence for costs and benefits using pure and mixed lists

  • Published:
Memory & Cognition Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Drawing a referent of a to-be-remembered word often results in better recognition and recall of this word relative to a control task in which the word is written, a pattern dubbed the drawing effect. Although this effect is not always found in pure lists, we report three experiments in which the drawing effect emerged in both pure- and mixed-lists on recognition and recall tests, though the effect was larger in mixed lists. Our experiments then compared drawing effects on memory between pure- and mixed-list contexts to determine whether the larger mixed-list drawing effect reflected a benefit to draw items, a cost to write items, or a combination. In delayed recognition and free-recall tests, a mixed-list benefit emerged for draw items in which memory for mixed-list draw items was greater than pure-list draw items. This mixed-list drawing benefit was accompanied by a mixed-list writing cost compared to pure-list write items, indicating that the mixed-list drawing effect does not operate cost-free. Our findings of a pure-list drawing effect are consistent with a memory strength account, however, the larger drawing effect in mixed lists suggest that participants may also deploy a distinctiveness heuristic to aid retrieval of drawn items.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Because hit and false alarm rates were perfect for many participants (1.00 or 0.00, respectively), we focus our analyses on raw recognition rates versus a hits-minus-false-alarms correction or dꞌ using signal detection. A signal-detection analysis requires a correction for asymptotic values (e.g., MacMillan & Creelman, 1988), and most participants would have required at least one corrected score to compute dꞌ due to ceiling hits or floor false alarms which would have distorted the dataset.

References

  • Balota, D.A., Yap, M.J., Hutchison, K.A. et al. The English lexicon project. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 445–459.

  • Begg, I., & Roe, H. (1988). On the inhibition of reading by generating. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 42, 325–336.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Begg, I., & Snider, A. (1987). The generation effect: Evidence for generalized inhibition. Journal Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13, 553–563.

    Google Scholar 

  • Begg, I., Snider, A., Foley, F., & Goddard, R. (1989). The generation effect is no artifact: Generating makes words distinctive. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 977–989.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bertsch, S., Pesta, B. J., Wiscott, R., & McDaniel, M. A. (2007). The generation effect: A meta-analytic review. Memory & Cognition, 35, 201–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bodner, G. E., Taikh, A., & Fawcett, J. M. (2014). Assessing the costs and benefits of production in recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21, 149–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brysbaert, M., & New, B. (2009). Moving beyond Kučera and Francis: A critical evaluation of current word frequency norms and the introduction of a new and improved word frequency measure for American English. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 977–990.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brysbaert, M., Warriner, A. B., & Kuperman, V. (2014). Concreteness ratings for 40 thousand generally known English word lemmas. Behavior Research Methods, 46, 904–911.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burnett, A. N., & Bodner, G. E. (2014). Learnin’ ‘bout my generation? Evaluating the effects of generation on encoding, recall, and metamemory across study-test experiences. Journal of Memory and Language, 75, 1–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conway, M. A., & Gathercole, S. E. (1987). Modality and long-term memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 26, 341–361.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Craik, F. I. M., & Lockhart, R. S. (1972). Levels of processing: A framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 11, 671–684.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crowder, R. G. (1976). Principles of Learning and Memory. Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • deWinstanley, P. A., & Bjork, E. L. (2004). Processing strategies and the generation effect: Implications for making a better reader. Memory & Cognition, 32, 945–955.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Engelkamp, J., & Dehn, D. M. (2000). Item and order information in subject-performed tasks and experimenter-performed tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 671–682.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Engelkamp, J., & Krumnacker, H. (1980). Image- and motor-processes in the retention of verbal materials. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie, 27, 511–533.

    Google Scholar 

  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). GPower 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fawcett, J. M. (2013). The production effect benefits performance in between-subjects designs: A meta-analysis. Acta Psychologica, 142, 1–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fernandes, M. A., Wammes, J. D., & Meade, M. E. (2018). The surprisingly powerful influence of drawing on memory. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27(5), 302–308.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Forrin, N. D., MacLeod, C. M., & Ozubko, J. D. (2012). Widening the boundaries of the production effect. Memory & Cognition, 40, 1046–1055.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hege, A. C. G., & Dodson, C. S. (2004). Why distinctive information reduces false memories: Evidence for both impoverished relational-encoding and distinctiveness heuristic accounts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30(4), 787–795.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hopkins, R. H., & Edwards, R. E. (1972). Pronunciation effects in recognition memory. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 11, 534–537.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Howard, M. W., & Kahana, M. J. (1999). Contextual variability and serial position effects in free recall. Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition25(4), 923-941.

  • Huff, M. J., & Bodner, G. E. (2019). Item-specific and relational processing both improve recall accuracy in the DRM paradigm. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72, 1493–1506.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huff, M. J., Bodner, G. E., & Fawcett, J. M. (2015). Effects of distinctive encoding on correct and false memory: A meta-analytic review of costs and benefits and their origins in the DRM paradigm. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22, 349–365.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Icht, M., Mama, M., & Algom, D. (2014). The production effect in memory: Multiple species of distinctiveness. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 886.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, A. C., & Pyc, M. A. (2014). The production effect: Costs and benefits in free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40, 300–305.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jonker, T. R., Levene, M., & MacLeod, C. M. (2014). Testing the item-order account of design effects using the production effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40, 441–448.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jonker, T. R., Wammes, J. D., & MacLeod, C. M. (2019). Drawing enhances item information but undermines sequence information in memory. Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 45, 689–699.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kuperman, V., Stadthagen-Gonzalez, H., & Brysbaert, M. (2012). Age-of-acquisition ratings for 30,000 English words. Behavior Research Methods, 44, 978–990.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, C. M. (1975). Release from proactive interference: Insufficiency of an attention account. The American Journal of Psychology, 88, 459–465.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, C. M., & Bodner, G. E. (2017). The production effect in memory. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 26, 390–395.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, C. M., Gopie, N., Hourihan, K. L., Neary, K. R., & Ozubko, J. D. (2010). The production effect: Delineation of a phenomenon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36, 671–685.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Macmillan, N. A., & Creelman, C. D. (1991). Detection Theory: A User’s Guide. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McDaniel, M. A., & Bugg, J. M. (2008). Instability in memory phenomena: A common puzzle and a unifying explanation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 237–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (1986). Bizarre imagery as an effective memory aid: The importance of distinctiveness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 12, 54–65.

    Google Scholar 

  • McDaniel, M. A., Waddill, P. J., & Einstein, G. O. (1988). A contextual account of the generation effect: A three-factor theory. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 521–536.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meade, M. E., Wammes, J. D., & Fernandes, M. A. (2018). Drawing as an encoding tool: Memorial benefits in younger and older adults. Experimental Aging Research, 44, 369–396.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Murdock, B. B. (1962). The serial position effect of free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 482–488.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nairne, J. S., Thompson, S. R., & Pandeirada, J. N. S. (2007). A990.daptive memory: Survival processing enhances retention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 263–273.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Namias, J. M., Huff, M. J., Smith, A., & Maxwell, N. P. (2022). Drawing individual images benefits recognition accuracy in the Deese-Roediger-McDermott paradigm. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 75, 1571–1582.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pexman, P. M., Muraki, E., Sidhu, D. M., Siakaluk, P. D., & Yap, M. J. (2019). Quantifying sensorimotor experience: Body-object interaction ratings for more than 9,000 English words. Behavior Research Methods, 51, 453–466.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Polyn, S. M., Norman, K. A., & Kahana, M. J. (2009). A context maintenance and retrieval model of organizational processes in free recall. Psychological Review, 116, 129–156.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, B. R. T., MacLeod, C. M., & Fernandes, M. A. (2022). The enactment effect: A systematic review and meta-analysis of behavioral, neuroimaging, and patient studies. Psychological Bulletin, 148, 397–434.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L., & McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 803–814.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D. L., Israel, L., & Racine, C. (1999). Suppressing false recognition in younger and older adults: The distinctiveness heuristic. Journal of Memory and Language, 40, 1–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schonfield, D., & Robertson, B.-A. (1966). Memory storage and aging. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 20, 228–236.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slamecka, N. J., & Graf, P. (1978). The generation effect: Delineation of a phenomenon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 4, 592–604.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slamecka, N. J., & Katsaiti, L. T. (1987). The generation effect as an artifact of selective displaced rehearsal. Journal of Memory and Language, 26, 589–607.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Doorn, J., & ven den Bergh, D., Böhm, U., Dablander, F., Derks, K., Draws, T. … Wagenmakers, E.-J. (2021). The JASP guidelines for conducting and reporting a Bayesian analysis. Behavior Research Methods, 28, 813–826.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wahlheim, C. N., & Huff, M. J. (2015). Age differences in the focus of retrieval: Evidence from dual-list free recall. Psychology and Aging, 30, 768–780.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wammes, J. D., Meade, M. E., & Fernandes, M. A. (2016). The drawing effect: Evidence for reliable and robust memory benefits in free recall. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69, 1752–1776.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wammes, J. D., Meade, M. E., & Fernandes, M. A. (2017). Learning terms and definitions: Drawing and the role of elaborative encoding. Acta Psychologica, 179, 104–113.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wammes, J. D., Meade, M. E., & Fernandes, M. A. (2018). Creating a recollection-based memory though drawing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 44, 734–751.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wickens, D. D., Born, D. G., & Allen, C. K. (1963). Proactive inhibition and item similarity in short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 2, 440–445.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yonelinas, A. P. (2002). The nature of recollection and familiarity. A review of 30 years of research. Journal of Memory and Language, 46, 441–517.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors’ note

Data collected were used for partial fulfillment of the Honor’s thesis requirements for P.P. Subject level data reported are available on our Open Science Framework page (osf.io/5wx4m).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mark J. Huff.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Open practices statement

Data for Experiments 1, 2, and 3 are available at osf.io/5wx4m. Study materials are available upon request.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Huff, M.J., Namias, J.M. & Poe, P. The drawing effect: Evidence for costs and benefits using pure and mixed lists. Mem Cogn (2024). https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-024-01551-6

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-024-01551-6

Keywords

Navigation