Advertisement

Memory & Cognition

, Volume 42, Issue 8, pp 1273–1284 | Cite as

Waiting for feedback helps if you want to know the answer: the role of curiosity in the delay-of-feedback benefit

  • Kellie M. Mullaney
  • Shana K. CarpenterEmail author
  • Courtney Grotenhuis
  • Steven Burianek
Article

Abstract

When participants answer a test question and then receive feedback of the correct answer, studies have shown that the feedback is more effective when it is delayed by several seconds rather than provided immediately (e.g., Brackbill & Kappy, Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 55, 14–18, 1962; Schroth, Contemporary Educational Psychology, 17, 78–82, 1992). Despite several demonstrations of this delay-of-feedback benefit, a theoretical explanation for this finding has not yet been developed. The present study tested the hypothesis that brief delays of feedback are beneficial because they encourage anticipation of the upcoming feedback. In Experiment 1, participants answered obscure trivia questions, and before receiving the answer, they rated their curiosity to know the answer. The answer was then provided either immediately or after a 4-s delay. A later final test over the same questions revealed a significant delay-of-feedback benefit, but only for items that had been rated high in curiosity. Experiment 2 replicated this same effect and showed that the delay-of-feedback benefit only occurs when feedback is provided after a variable, unpredictable time duration (either 2, 4, or 8 s) rather than after a constant duration (always 4 s). These findings demonstrate that the delay-of-feedback effect appears to be greatest under conditions in which participants are curious to know the answer and when the answer is provided after an unpredictable time interval.

Keywords

Delay of feedback Curiosity Memory 

Notes

Author note

This study was conducted as part of the requirements for the degree of Master of Science by the first author. Portions of this study were presented at the annual meeting of the Psychonomic Society (Seattle, WA, November 2011). We thank Ashley Adams, Syamim Hasim, Lauren Miller, Courtney Tapp, and Andrew Woods for their assistance with data collection and scoring.

References

  1. Auble, P. M., & Franks, J. J. (1978). The effects of effort toward comprehension on recall. Memory & Cognition, 6, 20–25. doi: 10.3758/BF03197424 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Berlyne, D. E. (1954). An experimental study of human curiosity. British Journal of Psychology, 45, 256–265.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Berlyne, D. E. (1966). Conditions of prequestioning and retention of meaningful material. Journal of Educational Psychology, 57, 128–132.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Botham, N. (2006). The book of useless information. New York, NY: Penguin.Google Scholar
  5. Brackbill, Y. (1964). The impairment of learning under immediate reinforcement. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1, 199–207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Brackbill, Y., Bravos, A., & Starr, R. H. (1962a). Delayed-improved retention of a difficult task. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 55, 947–952.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Brackbill, Y., Isaacs, R. B., & Smelkinson, N. (1962b). Delay of reinforcement and the retention of unfamiliar, meaningless material. Psychological Reports, 11, 553–554.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Brackbill, Y., & Kappy, M. S. (1962). Delay of reinforcement and retention. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 55, 14–18.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Bull, S. G., & Dizney, H. F. (1973). Epistemic-curiosity-arousing prequestions: Their effect on long-term retention. Journal of Educational Psychology, 65, 45–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Butler, A. C., Fazio, L. K., & Marsh, E. J. (2011). The hypercorrection effect persists over a week, but high-confidence errors return. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 1238–1244. doi: 10.3758/s13423-011-0173-y CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Butler, A. C., Karpicke, J. D., & Roediger, H. L., III. (2007). The effect of type and timing of feedback on learning from multiple-choice tests. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 13, 273–281. doi: 10.1037/1076-898X.13.4.273 PubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Butler, A. C., & Roediger, H. L., III. (2008). Feedback enhances the positive effects and reduces the negative effects of multiple-choice testing. Memory & Cognition, 36, 604–616. doi: 10.3758/MC.36.3.604 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Butterfield, B., & Metcalfe, J. (2001). Errors committed with high confidence are hypercorrected. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 1491–1494. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.27.6.1491 PubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Butterfield, B., & Metcalfe, J. (2006). The correction of errors committed with high confidence. Metacognition and Learning, 1, 69–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Carpenter, S. K. (2012). Testing enhances the transfer of learning. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 279–283. doi: 10.1177/0963721412452728 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Carpenter, S. K., Sachs, R. E., Martin, B., Schmidt, K., & Looft, R. (2012). Learning new vocabulary in German: The effects of inferring word meanings, type of feedback, and time of test. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 81–86. doi: 10.3758/s13423-011-0185-7 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Carpenter, S. K., & Vul, E. (2011). Delaying feedback by three seconds benefits retention of face–name pairs: The role of active anticipatory processing. Memory & Cognition, 37, 1211–1221. doi: 10.3758/s13421-011-0092-1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Dunlosky, J., Rawson, K. A., Marsh, E. J., Nathan, M. J., & Willingham, D. T. (2013). Improving students’ learning with effective learning techniques: Promising directions from cognitive and educational psychology. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 14, 4–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Fazio, L. K., Huelser, B. J., Johnson, A., & Marsh, E. J. (2010). Receiving right/wrong feedback: Consequences for learning. Memory, 18, 335–350.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  20. Fazio, L. K., & Marsh, E. J. (2009). Surprising feedback improves later memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 88–92. doi: 10.3758/PBR.16.1.88 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Finn, B., & Metcalfe, J. (2010). Scaffolding feedback to maximize long-term error correction. Memory & Cognition, 38, 951–961. doi: 10.3758/MC.38.7.951 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Huelser, B. J., & Metcalfe, J. (2012). Making related errors facilitates learning, but learners do not know it. Memory & Cognition, 40, 514–527. doi: 10.3758/s13421-011-0167-z CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Kang, M. J., Hsu, M., Krajbich, I. M., Loewenstein, G., McClure, S. M., Wang, J. T., & Camerer, C. F. (2009). The wick in the candle of learning: Epistemic curiosity activates reward circuitry and enhances memory. Psychological Science, 20, 963–973. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02402.x PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Kang, S. H. K., Pashler, H., Cepeda, N. J., Rohrer, D., Carpenter, S. K., & Mozer, M. C. (2011). Does incorrect guessing impair fact learning? Journal of Educational Psychology, 103, 48–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Karpicke, J. D., & Blunt, J. R. (2011). Retrieval practice produces more learning than elaborative studying with concept mapping. Science, 331, 772–775. doi: 10.1126/science.1199327 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Kornell, N. (2014). Attempting to answer a meaningful question enhances subsequent learning even when feedback is delayed. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40, 106–114.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Kornell, N., Hays, M. J., & Bjork, R. A. (2009). Unsuccessful retrieval attempts enhance subsequent learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 989–998. doi: 10.1037/a0015729 PubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Kronlund, A., & Whittlesea, B. W. A. (2006). Remembering after a perception of discrepancy: Out with the old, in with the two. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 1174–1184. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.32.5.1174 PubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Kulhavy, R. W., & Anderson, R. C. (1972). Delay-retention effect with multiple-choice tests. Journal of Educational Psychology, 63, 505–512.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Kulik, J. A., & Kulik, C. C. (1988). Timing of feedback and verbal learning. Review of Educational Research, 58, 79–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Lhyle, K. G., & Kulhavy, R. W. (1987). Feedback processing and error correction. Journal of Educational Psychology, 79, 320–322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Maw, W. H., & Maw, E. W. (1961). Information recognition by children with high and low curiosity. Educational Research Bulletin, 40, 197–224.Google Scholar
  33. Metcalfe, J., Kornell, N., & Finn, B. (2009). Delayed versus immediate feedback in children’s and adults’ vocabulary learning. Memory & Cognition, 37, 1077–1087. doi: 10.3758/MC.37.8.1077 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Mullet, H. G., Butler, A. C., Verdin, B., von Borries, R., & Marsh, E. J. (2014). Delaying feedback promotes transfer of knowledge despite student preferences to receive feedback immediately. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition. doi: 10.1016/j.jarmac.2014.05.001
  35. Parlow, J., & Berlyne, D. E. (1971). The effect of prior guessing on incidental learning of verbal associations. Journal of Structural Learning, 2, 55–65.Google Scholar
  36. Pashler, H., Cepeda, N. J., Wixted, J. T., & Rohrer, D. (2005). When does feedback facilitate learning of words? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 3–8. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.31.1.3 PubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Peeck, J., van den Bosch, A. B., & Kreupeling, W. J. (1985). Effects of informative feedback in relation to retention of initial responses. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 10, 303–313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Perin, C. T. (1943). A quantitative investigation of the delay of reinforcement gradient. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 37–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Pubols, B. H. (1958). Delay of reinforcement, response perseveration, and discrimination reversal. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56, 32–40.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Roediger, H. L., III, & Butler, A. C. (2011). The critical role of retrieval practice in long-term learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15, 20–27. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.09.003 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Sarter, M., Givens, B., & Bruno, J. P. (2001). The cognitive neuroscience of sustained attention: Where top-down meets bottom-up. Brain Research Reviews, 35, 146–160.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Sassenrath, J. M., & Yonge, G. D. (1969). Effects of delayed information feedback and feedback cues in learning on delayed retention. Journal of Educational Psychology, 60, 174–177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Schroth, M. L. (1992). The effects of delay of feedback on a delayed concept formation transfer task. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 17, 78–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Schroth, M. L., & Lund, E. (1993). Role of delay of feedback on subsequent pattern recognition transfer tasks. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 18, 15–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Sturges, P. T., Sarafino, E. P., & Donaldson, P. L. (1968). Delay-retention effect and informative feedback. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 78, 357–358.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Surber, J. R., & Anderson, R. C. (1975). Delay-retention effect in natural classroom settings. Journal of Educational Psychology, 67, 170–173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Swinnen, S. P., Schmidt, R. A., Nicholson, D. E., & Shapiro, D. C. (1990). Information feedback for skill acquisition: Instantaneous knowledge of results degrades learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 706–716. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.16.4.706 Google Scholar
  48. Tarpy, R. M., & Sawabini, F. L. (1974). Reinforcement delay: A selective review of the last decade. Psychological Bulletin, 81, 984–997.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Tobin, K. (1987). The role of wait time in higher cognitive level learning. Review of Educational Research, 57, 69–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Zaromb, F. M., Karpicke, J. D., & Roediger, H. L., III. (2010). Comprehension as a basis for metacognitive judgments: Effects of effort after meaning on recall and metacognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36, 552–557. doi: 10.1037/a0018277 PubMedGoogle Scholar
  51. Zaromb, F. M., & Roediger, H. L., III. (2009). The effects of “effort after meaning” on recall: Differences in within- and between-subjects designs. Memory & Cognition, 37, 447–463. doi: 10.3758/MC.37.4.447 CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  • Kellie M. Mullaney
    • 1
  • Shana K. Carpenter
    • 1
    Email author
  • Courtney Grotenhuis
    • 1
  • Steven Burianek
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyIowa State UniversityAmesUSA

Personalised recommendations