Memory & Cognition

, Volume 42, Issue 2, pp 237–246

Intentional forgetting: Note-taking as a naturalistic example

Article

Abstract

In the present study, we examined whether note-taking as a memory aid may provide a naturalistic example of intentional forgetting. In the first experiment, participants played Concentration, a memory card game in which the identity and location of pairs of cards need to be remembered. Before the game started, half of the participants were allowed to study the cards, and the other half made notes that were then unexpectedly taken away. No significant differences emerged between the two groups for remembering identity information, but the study group remembered significantly more location information than did the note-taking group. In a second experiment, we examined whether note-takers would show signs of proactive interference while playing Concentration repeatedly. The results indicated that they did not. The findings suggest that participants adopted an intentional-forgetting strategy when using notes to store certain types of information.

Keywords

Directed forgetting Memory Mnemonics 

References

  1. Basden, B. H., Basden, D. R., & Gargano, G. J. (1993). Directed forgetting in implicit and explicit memory tests: A comparison of methods. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 603–616.Google Scholar
  2. Bjork, R. A. (1970). Positive forgetting: The noninterference of items intentionally forgotten. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 9, 255–268. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(70)80059-7 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bjork, R. A. (1972). Theoretical implications of directed forgetting. In A. W. Melton & W. Martin (Eds.), Coding processes in human memory (pp. 217–235). Washington, DC: Winston.Google Scholar
  4. Block, R. A. (1971). Effects of instructions to forget in short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 89, 1–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Delaney, P. F., Nghiem, K. N., & Wladum, E. R. (2009). The selective directed forgetting effect: Can people forget only part of a text? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 1542–1550.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Di Vesta, F. J., & Gray, G. S. (1972). Listening and note-taking. Journal of Educational Psychology, 63, 8–14.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Donald, M. (1991). Origins of the modern mind: Three stages in the evolution of culture and cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  8. Earles, J. L., & Kersten, A. W. (2002). Directed forgetting of actions by younger and older adults. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 383–388.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Eskritt, M. (2005). Children’s and adult’s use of notation and its influence on memory strategies. In S. Shohov (Ed.), Advances in psychology (pp. 33–38). New York, NY: Nova Sciences.Google Scholar
  10. Eskritt, M., & Lee, K. (2002). “Remember where you last saw that card”: Children’s production of external symbols as a memory aid. Developmental Psychology, 38, 254–266. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.38.2.254 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Eskritt, M., & Lee, K. (2007). The impact of notation on cognition & its development: Theoretical perspectives and empirical evidence. In E. Teubal, J. Dockrell, & L. Tolchinsky (Eds.), Notational knowledge: Developmental and historical perspectives (pp. 233–256). Rotterdam, The Netherlands: Sense Publishers.Google Scholar
  12. Eskritt, M., Lee, K., & Donald, M. (2001). The influence of symbolic literacy on memory: Testing Plato’s hypothesis. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55, 39–50.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Eskritt, M., & McLeod, K. (2008). Children’s note taking as a mnemonic tool. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 101, 52–74.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Fawcett, J. M. (2013). The production effect benefits performance in between-subject designs: A meta-analysis. Acta Psychologica, 142, 1–5.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Fawcett, J. M., Taylor, T. L., & Nadel, L. (2013). Intentional forgetting diminishes memory for continuous events. Memory, 21, 675–694. doi:10.1080/09658211.2012.748078 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Forrin, N. D., Macleod, C. M., & Ozubko, J. D. (2012). Widening the boundaries of the production effect. Memory & Cognition, 40, 1046–1055. doi:10.3758/s13421-012-0210-8 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Geiselman, R. E., Bjork, R. A., & Fishman, D. L. (1983). Disrupted retrieval in directed forgetting: A link with posthypnotic amnesia. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 112, 58–72.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Goernert, P. N., Corenblum, B., & Otani, H. (2011). Directed forgetting of faces: The role of response criterion. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 1930–1938.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Golding, J. M., & Keenan, J. M. (1985). Direct forgetting and memory for directions to a destination. American Journal of Psychology, 98, 579–590.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Golding, J. M., & Long, D. L. (1998). There’s more to intentional forgetting than directed forgetting: An integrative review. In J. M. Golding & C. M. MacLeod (Eds.), Intentional forgetting: Interdisciplinary approaches (pp. 1–57). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  21. Gottlob, L. R., Golding, J. M., & Hauselt, W. J. (2006). Directed forgetting of a single item. Journal of General Psychology, 133, 67–80.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Hauswald, A., & Kissler, J. (2008). Directed forgetting of complex pictures in an item method paradigm. Memory, 16, 797–809.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Hourihan, K. L., & MacLeod, C. M. (2008). Directed forgetting meets the production effect: Distinctive processing is resistant to intentional forgetting. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 242–246. doi:10.1037/1196-1961.62.4.242 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Hourihan, K. L., Ozubko, J. D., & MacLeod, C. M. (2009). Directed forgetting of visual symbols: Evidence for nonverbal selective rehearsal. Memory & Cognition, 37, 1059–1068. doi:10.3758/MC.37.8.1059 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Johnson, H. M. (1994). Processes of successful intentional forgetting. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 274–292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Joormann, J., Hertel, P. T., Brozovich, F., & Gotlib, I. H. (2005). Remembering the good, forgetting the bad: Intentional forgetting of emotional material in depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 640–648. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.114.4.640 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Joslyn, S. L., & Oakes, M. A. (2005). Directed forgetting of autobiographical events. Memory & Cognition, 33, 577–587. doi:10.3758/BF03195325 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Korfine, L., & Hooley, J. M. (2000). Directed forgetting of emotional stimuli in borderline personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 214–221.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Lee, Y., Lee, H., & Fawcett, J. M. (2013). Intentional forgetting reduces color-naming interference: Evidence from item-method directed forgetting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39, 220–236.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Lehman, M., & Malmberg, K. J. (2011). Overcoming the effects of intentional forgetting. Memory & Cognition, 39, 335–347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. MacLeod, C. M. (1975). Long-term recognition and recall following directed forgetting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 1, 271–279.Google Scholar
  32. MacLeod, C. M. (1998). Directed forgetting. In J. M. Golding & C. M. MacLeod (Eds.), Intentional forgetting: Interdisciplinary approaches (pp. 1–57). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  33. MacLeod, C. M., & Daniels, K. A. (2000). Direct versus indirect tests of memory: Directed forgetting meets the generation effect. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 7, 354–359.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Quinlan, C. K., Taylor, T. L., & Fawcett, J. M. (2010). Directed forgetting: Comparing pictures and words. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 41–46.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Sahakyan, L. (2004). Destructive effects of “forget” instructions. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 555–559.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Sahakyan, L., & Kelley, C. M. (2002). A contextual change account of directed forgetting effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28, 1064–1072. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.28.6.1064 PubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Shapiro, S., Lindsey, C., & Krishnan, H. S. (2006). Intentional forgetting as a facilitator for recalling new product attributes. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Applied, 12, 251–263. doi:10.1037/1076-898X.12.4.251 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Sparrow, B., Liu, J., & Wegner, D. M. (2011). Google effects on memory: Cognitive consequences of having information at our fingertips. Science, 333, 776–778. doi:10.1126/science.1207745 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Standing, L., Conezio, J., & Haber, R. N. (1970). Perception and memory for pictures: Single-trial learning of 2500 visual stimuli. Psychonomic Science, 19, 73–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Sutton, J. (2010). Exograms and interdisciplinarity: History, the extended mind, and the civilizing process. In R. Menary (Ed.), The extended mind (pp. 189–225). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Tekcan, A. I., Taş, A. C., Topçuoğlu, V., & Yücel, B. (2008). Memory bias in anorexia nervosa: Evidence from directed forgetting. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 39, 369–380.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Underwood, B. J. (1957). Interference and forgetting. Psychological Review, 64, 49–60. doi:10.1037/h0044616 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyMount St. Vincent UniversityHalifaxCanada

Personalised recommendations