Advertisement

Memory & Cognition

, Volume 42, Issue 2, pp 305–324 | Cite as

Pilgrims sailing the Titanic: Plausibility effects on memory for misinformation

  • Scott R. Hinze
  • Daniel G. Slaten
  • William S. Horton
  • Ryan Jenkins
  • David N. Rapp
Article

Abstract

People rely on information they read even when it is inaccurate (Marsh, Meade, & Roediger, Journal of Memory and Language 49:519–536, 2003), but how ubiquitous is this phenomenon? In two experiments, we investigated whether this tendency to encode and rely on inaccuracies from text might be influenced by the plausibility of misinformation. In Experiment 1, we presented stories containing inaccurate plausible statements (e.g., “The Pilgrims’ ship was the Godspeed”), inaccurate implausible statements (e.g., . . . the Titanic), or accurate statements (e.g., . . . the Mayflower). On a subsequent test of general knowledge, participants relied significantly less on implausible than on plausible inaccuracies from the texts but continued to rely on accurate information. In Experiment 2, we replicated these results with the addition of a think-aloud procedure to elicit information about readers’ noticing and evaluative processes for plausible and implausible misinformation. Participants indicated more skepticism and less acceptance of implausible than of plausible inaccuracies. In contrast, they often failed to notice, completely ignored, and at times even explicitly accepted the misinformation provided by plausible lures. These results offer insight into the conditions under which reliance on inaccurate information occurs and suggest potential mechanisms that may underlie reported misinformation effects.

Keywords

Memory False memory Text processing 

Notes

Author Note

Scott R. Hinze, Daniel G. Slaten, William S. Horton, Ryan Jenkins, and David N. Rapp, Northwestern University.

Scott R. Hinze is now in the Psychology Department at Virginia Wesleyan College.

Direct all correspondence to Scott Hinze, Department of Psychology, 1584 Wesleyan Dr., Virginia Wesleyan College, Norfolk, VA, 23502. E-mail: shinze@vwc.edu. Phone: 757-455-3288.

References

  1. Afflerbach, P. (2002). Verbal reports and protocol analysis. In M. L. Kamil, P. B. Mosenthal, P. D. Pearson, & R. Barr (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (Vol. III, pp. 87–103). Mahwah: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  2. Appel, M., & Richter, T. (2007). Persuasive effects of fictional narratives increase over time. Media Psychology, 10, 113–134.Google Scholar
  3. Barton, S. B., & Sanford, A. J. (1993). A case-study of anomaly detection: Shallow semantic processing and cohesion establishment. Memory and Cognition, 21, 477–487.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bottoms, H. C., Eslick, A. N., & Marsh, E. J. (2010). Memory and the Moses illusion: Failures to detect contradictions with stored knowledge yield negative memorial consequences. Memory, 18, 670–678.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Brainerd, C. J., Reyna, V. F., Wright, R., & Mojardin, A. H. (2003). Recollection rejection: False-memory editing in children and adults. Psychological Review, 110, 762–784.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Chaiken, S., Liberman, A., & Eagly, A. H. (1989). Heuristic and systematic processing within and beyond the persuasion context. In J. S. Uleman & J. A. Barch (Eds.), Unintended thought (pp. 212–252). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  7. Connell, L., & Keane, M. T. (2004). What plausibly affects plausibility? Concept–coherence and distributional word–coherence as factors influencing plausibility judgements. Memory and Cognition, 32, 185–197.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Connell, L., & Keane, M. T. (2006). A model of plausibility. Cognitive Science, 30, 95–120.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Coté, N., & Goldman, S. R. (1999). Building representations of informational text: Evidence from children’s think-aloud protocols. In H. van Oostendorp & S. R. Goldman (Eds.), The construction of mental representations during reading (pp. 169–193). Mahwah: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  10. Fazio, L. K., Barber, S. J., Rajaram, S., Ornstein, P. A., & Marsh, E. J. (2013). Creating illusions of knowledge: Learning errors that contradict prior knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 1–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Fazio, L. K., & Marsh, E. J. (2008). Slowing presentation speed increases illusions of knowledge. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 180–185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Gallo, D. A. (2004). Using recall to reduce false recognition: Diagnostic and disqualifying monitoring. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 30, 120–128.Google Scholar
  13. Gallo, D. A. (2010). False memories and fantastic beliefs: 15 years of the DRM illusion. Memory & Cognition, 38, 833–848.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Gerrig, R. J. (1993). Experiencing narrative worlds. New Haven: Yale Univ. Press.Google Scholar
  15. Gerrig, R. J., & McKoon, G. (2001). Memory processes and experiential continuity. Psychological Science, 12, 81–85.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Gerrig, R. J., & Prentice, D. A. (1991). The representation of fictional information. Psychological Science, 2, 336–340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Gilbert, D. T. (1991). How mental systems believe. American Psychologist, 46, 107–119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Gilbert, D. T., Krull, D. S., & Malone, P. S. (1990). Unbelieving the unbelievable: Some problems in the rejection of false information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 601–613.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Gilbert, D. T., Tafarodi, R. W., & Malone, P. S. (1993). You can’t not believe everything you read. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 221–233.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Green, M. C., & Brock, T. C. (2000). The role of transportation in the persuasiveness of public narratives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 701–721.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Isberner, M. B., & Richter, T. (2013a). Can readers ignore implausibility? Evidence for nonstrategic monitoring of event-based plausibility in language comprehension. Acta Psychologica, 142, 15–22.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Isberner, M. B., & Richter, T. (2013b). Comprehension and validation: Separable stages of information processing? A case for epistemic monitoring in language comprehension. In D. N. Rapp & J. Braasch (Eds.) Processing Inaccurate Information: Theoretical and Applied Perspectives from Cognitive Science and the Educational Sciences. MIT Press.Google Scholar
  23. Kendeou, P., & van den Broek, P. (2005). The effects of readers’ misconceptions on comprehension of scientific text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 235–245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Kendeou, P., & van den Broek, P. (2007). The effects of prior knowledge and text structure on comprehension processes during reading of scientific texts. Memory & Cognition, 35, 1567–1577.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Landauer, T. K., Foltz, P. W., & Laham, D. (1998). Introduction to latent semantic analysis. Discourse Processes, 25, 259–284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Landauer, T., & Kintsch, W. (2006). Latent Semantic Analysis. [Online Computer Software]. Accessed May 9, 2013, from http://lsa.colorado.edu/
  27. Loftus, E. (1979). Reactions to blatantly contradictory information. Memory & Cognition, 7, 368–374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Magliano, J. P., & Millis, K. K. (2003). Assessing reading skill with a think-aloud procedure and latent semantic analysis. Cognition and Instruction, 21, 251–283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Magliano, J. P., Trabasso, T., & Graesser, A. C. (1999). Strategic processes during comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 615–629.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Marsh, E. J. (2004). Stimuli for creating false beliefs about the world. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 36, 650–655.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Marsh, E. J., Balota, D. A., & Roediger, H. L. (2005). Learning facts from fiction: Effects of healthy aging and early-stage dementia of the Alzheimer type. Neuropsychology, 19, 115–129.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Marsh, E. J., & Fazio, L. K. (2006). Learning errors from fiction: Difficulties in reducing reliance on fictional stories. Memory & Cognition, 34, 1141–1149.Google Scholar
  33. Marsh, E. J., Meade, M. L., & Roediger, H. L. (2003). Learning facts from fiction. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 519–536.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Marsh, E. J., & Umanath, S. (2013). Knowledge neglect: Failures to notice contradictions in stored knowledge. In D. N. Rapp & J. Braasch (Eds.) Processing Inaccurate Information: Theoretical and Applied Perspectives from Cognitive Science and the Educational Sciences. MIT Press.Google Scholar
  35. Mazzoni, G. A. L., Loftus, E. F., & Kirsch, I. (2001). Changing beliefs about implausible autobiographical events: A little plausibility goes a long way. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 7, 51–59.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. McKoon, G., & Ratcliff, R. (1992). Inference during reading. Psychological Review, 99, 440–466.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. McNamara, D. S. (2004). SERT: Self-explanation reading training. Discourse Processes, 38, 1–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Myers, J. L., & O’Brien, E. J. (1998). Accessing the discourse representation during reading. Discourse Processes, 26, 131–157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Myers, J. L., O’Brien, E. J., Albrecht, J. E., & Mason, R. A. (1994). Maintaining global coherence during reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 876–886.Google Scholar
  40. Nelson, T. O., & Narens, L. (1980). Norms of 300 general-information questions: Accuracy of recall, latency of recall and feeling-of-knowing ratings. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 21, 403–420.Google Scholar
  41. O’Brien, E. J., Rizzella, M. L., Albrecht, J. E., & Halleran, J. G. (1998). Updating a situation model: A memory-based text processing view. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 1200–1210.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Odegard, T. N., & Lampinen, J. M. (2006). Memory editing: Knowledge, criteria, and alignment. Memory, 14, 777–787.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Park, H., & Reder, L. M. (2004). Moses illusion: Implications for human cognition. In R. F. Pohl (Ed.), Cognitive illusions (pp. 275–291). Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  44. Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). Communication and persuasion. Central and peripheral routes to attitude change. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
  45. Pezdek, K., Blandon-Gitlin, I., & Gabbay, P. (2006). Imagination and memory: Does imagining implausible events lead to false autobiographical memories? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 764–769.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Pezdek, K., Finger, K., & Hodge, D. (1997). Planting false childhood memories: The role of event plausibility. Psychological Science, 8, 437–441.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Pickering, M. J., & Traxler, M. J. (1998). Plausibility and recovery from garden paths: An eye-tracking study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 24, 940–961.Google Scholar
  48. Prentice, D. A., Gerrig, R. J., & Bailis, D. S. (1997). What readers bring to the processing of fictional texts. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4, 416–420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Rapp, D. N. (2008). How do readers handle incorrect information during reading? Memory & Cognition, 36, 688–701.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Rapp, D. N., Hinze, S. R., Kohlhepp, K., & Ryskin, R. A. (2013a). Reducing reliance on inaccurate information. Memory & Cognition.Google Scholar
  51. Rapp, D. N., Hinze, S. R., Slaten, D. G., & Horton, W. S. (2013b). Amazing stories: Acquiring and avoiding inaccurate information from fiction. Discourse Processes.Google Scholar
  52. Reder, L. M. (1982). Plausibility judgments vs. fact retrieval: Alternative strategies for sentence verification. Psychological Review, 89, 250–280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Richter, T., Schroeder, S., & Wohrmann, B. (2009). You don’t have to believe everything you read: Background knowledge permits fast and efficient validation of information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 538–558.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Schroeder, S., Richter, T., & Hoever, I. (2008). Getting a picture that is both accurate and stable: Situation models and epistemic validation. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 237–259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Singer, M. (2006). Verification of text ideas during reading. Journal of Memory and Language, 54, 574–591.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Sparks, J. R., & Rapp, D. N. (2011). Readers’ reliance on source credibility in the service of comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 37, 230–247.Google Scholar
  57. van Oostendorp, H., & Kok, I. (1990). Failing to notice errors in sentences. Languages and Cognitive Processes, 5, 105–113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Wheeler, S. C., Green, M. C., & Brock, T. C. (1999). Fictional narratives change beliefs: Replications of Prentice, Gerrig, and Bailis (1997) with mixed corroboration. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 6, 136–141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Wyer, R. S., & Radvansky, G. A. (1999). The comprehension and validation of social information. Psychological Review, 106, 89–118.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  • Scott R. Hinze
    • 1
    • 2
  • Daniel G. Slaten
    • 1
  • William S. Horton
    • 1
  • Ryan Jenkins
    • 1
  • David N. Rapp
    • 1
  1. 1.Northwestern UniversityEvanstonUSA
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyVirginia Wesleyan CollegeNorfolkUSA

Personalised recommendations