Dodson, C. S., & Johnson, M. K. (1993). Rate of false source attributions depends on how questions are asked. The American Journal of Psychology, 106, 541–557.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Durso, F. T., & Johnson, M. K. (1979). Facilitation in naming and categorizing repeated pictures and words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 5, 449–459.
Article
Google Scholar
Fazendeiro, T., Winkielman, P., Luo, C., & Lorah, C. (2005). False recognition across meaning, language, and stimulus format: Conceptual relatedness and the feeling of familiarity. Memory & Cognition, 33, 249–260. doi:10.3758/BF03195314
Article
Google Scholar
Feustel, T. C., Shiffrin, R. M., & Salasoo, A. (1983). Episodic and lexical contributions to the repetition effect in word identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 112, 309–346.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Friese, U., Supp, G. G., Hipp, J. F., Engel, A. K., & Gruber, T. (2012). Oscillatory MEG gamma band activity dissociates perceptual and conceptual aspects of visual object processing: A combined repetition/conceptual priming study. NeuroImage, 59, 861–871. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.073
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Garoff-Eaton, R. J., Slotnick, S. D., & Schacter, D. L. (2006). Not all false memories are created equal: The neural basis of false recognition. Cerebral Cortex, 16, 1645–1652.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Gottesman, C. V. (2011). Mental layout extrapolations prime spatial processing of scenes. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 37, 382–395.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Heaps, C. M., & Nash, M. (2001). Comparing recollective experience in true and false autobiographical memories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 920–930.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Henkel, L. A., & Franklin, N. (1998). Reality monitoring of physically similar and conceptually related objects. Memory & Cognition, 26, 659–673.
Article
Google Scholar
Hintzman, D. L. (1988). Judgments of frequency and recognition memory in a multiple-trace memory model. Psychological Review, 95, 528–551. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.95.4.528
Article
Google Scholar
Hirshman, E., Snodgrass, J. G., Mindes, J., & Feenan, K. (1990). Conceptual priming in fragment completion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 634–647.
Article
Google Scholar
Horner, A. J., & Henson, R. N. (2011). Stimulus–response bindings code both abstract and specific representations of stimuli: Evidence from a classification priming design that reverses multiple levels of response representations. Memory & Cognition, 39, 1457–1471.
Article
Google Scholar
Intraub, H., & Hoffman, J. E. C. (1992). Reading and visual memory: Remembering scenes that were never seen. The American Journal of Psychology, 105, 101–114.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Israel, L., & Schacter, D. L. (1997). Pictorial encoding reduces false recognition of semantic associates. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4, 577–581. doi:10.3758/BF03214352
Article
Google Scholar
Jacoby, L. L., & Dallas, M. (1981). On the relationship between autobiographical memory and perceptual learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 110, 306–340. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.110.3.306
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Johnson, M. K., Hashtroudi, S., & Lindsay, D. S. (1993). Source monitoring. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 3–28. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.114.1.3
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Johnson, M. K., Suengas, A. G., Foley, M. A., & Raye, C. L. (1988). Phenomenal characteristics of memories for perceived and imagined autobiographical events. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 117, 371–376.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Johnston, W. A., Dark, V. J., & Jacoby, L. L. (1985). Perceptual fluency and recognition judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11, 3–11.
Article
Google Scholar
Kherif, F., Josse, G., & Price, C. J. (2011). Automatic top-down processing explains common left occipito-temporal responses to visual words and objects. Cerebral Cortex, 21, 103–114.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Koutstaal, W., Verfaellie, M., & Schacter, D. L. (2001). Recognizing identical versus similar categorically related common objects: Further evidence for degraded gist-representations in amnesia. Neuropsychology, 15, 268–289.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Lampinen, J. M., Meier, C. R., Arnal, J. D., & Leding, J. K. (2005). Compelling untruths: Content borrowing and vivid false memories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 954–963. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.31.5.954
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Lampinen, J. M., Odegard, T. N., & Bullington, J. L. (2003). Qualities of memories for performed and imagined actions. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17, 881–893. doi:10.1002/acp. 916
Article
Google Scholar
Lindsay, D. S., & Johnson, M. K. (1989). The eyewitness suggestibility effect and memory for source. Memory & Cognition, 17, 349–358.
Article
Google Scholar
Lyle, K. B., & Johnson, M. K. (2006). Importing perceived features into false memories. Memory, 14, 197–213.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Lyle, K. B., & Johnson, M. K. (2007). Source misattributions may increase the accuracy of source judgments. Memory & Cognition, 35, 1024–1033.
Article
Google Scholar
Mather, M., Henkel, L. A., & Johnson, M. K. (1997). Evaluating characteristics of false memories: Remember/know judgments and memory characteristics questionnaire compared. Memory & Cognition, 25, 826–837.
Article
Google Scholar
McDermott, K. B. (1997). Priming on perceptual implicit memory tests can be achieved through presentation of associates. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4, 582–586.
Article
Google Scholar
Miller, M. B., & Gazzaniga, M. S. (1998). Creating false memories for visual scenes. Neuropsychologia, 36, 513–520.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Norman, K. A., & Schacter, D. L. (1997). False recognition in younger and older adults: Exploring the characteristics of illusory memories. Memory & Cognition, 25, 838–848.
Article
Google Scholar
Scarborough, D. L., Gerard, L., & Cortese, C. (1979). Accessing lexical memory: The transfer of word repetition effects across task and modality. Memory & Cognition, 7, 3–12. doi:10.3758/BF03196928
Article
Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L., & Slotnick, S. D. (2004). The cognitive neuroscience of memory distortion. Neuron, 44, 1–20.
Article
Google Scholar
Schooler, J. W., Gerhard, D., & Loftus, E. F. (1986). Qualities of the unreal. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 12, 171–181.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Stark, C. E. L., & McClelland, J. L. (2000). Repetition priming of words, pseudowords, and nonwords. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 945–972.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Stark, C. E. L., Okado, Y., & Loftus, E. F. (2010). Imaging the reconstruction of true and false memories using sensory reactivation and the misinformation paradigms. Learning & Memory, 17, 485–488.
Article
Google Scholar
VanRullen, R., & Koch, C. (2003). Competition and selection during visual processing of natural scenes and objects. Journal of Vision, 3(1), 75–85. doi:10.1167/3.1.8
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Weinstein, Y., & Shanks, D. R. (2008). Perceptual representations in false recognition and priming of pictures. Memory & Cognition, 36, 1415–1428. doi:10.3758/MC.36.8.1415
Article
Google Scholar
Weinstein, Y., & Shanks, D. R. (2010). Rapid induction of false memory for pictures. Memory, 18, 533–542.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Weldon, M. S., Roediger, H. L., III, Beitel, D. A., & Johnston, T. R. (1995). Perceptual and conceptual processes in implicit and explicit tests with picture fragment and word-fragment cues. Journal of Memory and Language, 34, 268–285.
Article
Google Scholar