Anderson, J. A. (1973). A theory for the recognition of items from short memorized lists. Psychological Review, 80, 417–438.
Article
Google Scholar
Anderson, J. R. (1974). Retrieval of propositional information from long-term memory. Cognitive Psychology, 6, 451–474.
Article
Google Scholar
Anderson, J. R. (1983). A spreading activation theory of memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Behavior, 22, 261–295.
Article
Google Scholar
Anderson, J. R., & Bower, G. H. (1972). Recognition and retrieval processes in free recall. Psychological Review, 79, 97–123.
Article
Google Scholar
Anderson, M. C., & Neely, J. H. (1996). Interference and inhibition in memory retrieval. In E. L. Bjork & R. A. Bjork (Eds.), Memory: Handbook of perception and cognition (2nd ed., pp. 237–313). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Google Scholar
Anderson, J. R., & Reder, L. M. (1999). The fan effect: New results and new theories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128, 186–197.
Article
Google Scholar
Barnes, J. M., & Underwood, B. J. (1959). "Fate" of first-list associations in transfer theory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 97–105.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Berger, S. A., Hall, L. K., & Bahrick, H. P. (1999). Stabilizing access to marginal and submarginal knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 5, 438–447.
Article
Google Scholar
Bertsch, S., Pesta, B. J., Wiscott, R., & McDaniel, M. A. (2007). The generation effect: A metanalytic review. Memory & Cognition, 35, 201–210.
Article
Google Scholar
Bjork, R. A. (1994). Institutional impediments to effective training. In D. Druckman & R. A. Bjork (Eds.), Learning, remembering, believing: Enhancing human performance (pp. 295–306). Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
Google Scholar
Butler, A. C., Karpicke, J. D., & III Roediger, H. L. (2008). Correcting a metacognitive error: Feedback increases retention of low confidence correct responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 918–928.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Butterfield, B., & Mangels, J. A. (2003). Neural correlates of error detection and correction in a semantic retrieval task. Cognitive Brain Research, 17, 793–817.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Butterfield, B., & Metcalfe, J. (2006). The correction of errors committed with high confidence. Metacognition and Learning, 1, 69–84.
Article
Google Scholar
Carpenter, S. K. (2009). Cue strength as a moderator of the testing effect: The benefits of elaborative retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 1563–1569.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Carpenter, S. K. (2011). Semantic information activated during retrieval contributes to later retention: Support for the mediator effectiveness hypothesis of the testing effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37(6), 1547–1552
Google Scholar
Carpenter, S. K., & DeLosh, E. L. (2006). Impoverished cue support enhances subsequent retention: Support for the elaborative retrieval explanation of the testing effect. Memory & Cognition, 34, 268–276.
Article
Google Scholar
Carpenter, S. K., & Olson, K. M. (in press). Are pictures good for learning new vocabulary in a foreign language? Only if you think they are not. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition.
Clare, L., & Jones, R. S. P. (2008). Errorless learning in the rehabilitation of memory impairment: A critical review. Neuropsychology Review, 1, 1–23.
Article
Google Scholar
Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82, 407–428.
Google Scholar
Collins, A. M., & Quillian, M. R. (1972). Experiments on semantic memory and language comprehension. In L. Gregg (Ed.), Cognition and learning (pp. 117–138)). New York: Wiley.
Craik, F. I. M., & Lockhart, R. S. (1972). Levels of processing: A framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 671–684.
Article
Google Scholar
Craik, F. I. M., & Tulving, E. (1975). Depth of processing and the retention of words in episodic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 10, 268–294.
Article
Google Scholar
Cunningham, D., & Anderson, R. C. (1968). Effects of practice time within prompting and confirmation presentation procedures on paired associate learning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 7, 613–616.
Article
Google Scholar
Eich, J. M. (1982). A composite holographic associative recall model. Psychological Review, 89, 627–661.
Article
Google Scholar
Fazio, L. K., Huelser, B. J., Johnson, A., & Marsh, E. J. (2010). Receiving right/wrong feedback: Consequences for learning. Memory, 18, 335–350.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Fazio, L. K., & Marsh, E. J. (2009). Surprising feedback improves later memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 88–92.
Article
Google Scholar
Gillund, G., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1984). A retrieval model for both recognition and recall. Psychological Review, 91, 1–67.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Glaser, R. (1990). The reemergence of learning theory within instructional research. American Psychologist, 45, 29–39.
Article
Google Scholar
Grimadli, P. J. & Karpicke, J. D. (in press). When and why do retrieval attempts enhance subsequent encoding? Memory & Cognition.
Guthrie, E. (1952). The psychology of learning (Revth ed.). New York: Harper.
Google Scholar
Hintzman, D. L. (1984). MINERVA 2: A simulation model of human memory. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 16, 96–101.
Article
Google Scholar
Huelser, B. J. & Metcalfe, J. (2011, November). Performance monitoring offsets, but does not eliminate, the metacognitive illusion that errors hurt learning. Poster presented at the 52nd annual meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Seattle, WA.
Izawa, C. (1967). Function of test trials in paired-associate learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 75, 194–209.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Izawa, C. (1970). Optimal potentiating effects and forgetting-prevention effects of tests in paired-associate learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 83, 340–344.
Article
Google Scholar
Jarvis, B. G. (2004). DirectRT (Version 2004.1.0.55) [computer software]. New York: Empirisoft Corporation.
Kane, J. H., & Anderson, R. C. (1978). Depth of processing and interference effects in the learning and remembering of sentences. Journal of Educational Psychology, 70, 626–635.
Article
Google Scholar
Kang, S. H. K., Pashler, H., Cepeda, N. J., Rohrer, D., Carpenter, S. K., & Mozer, M. C. (2011). Does incorrect guessing impair fact learning? Journal of Educational Psychology., 131, 48–59.
Article
Google Scholar
Koriat, A. (1997). Monitoring one's own knowledge during study: A cue-utilization approach to judgments of learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 126, 349–370.
Article
Google Scholar
Koriat, A. (2008). Easy comes, easy goes? The link between learning and remembering and its exploitation in metacognition. Memory & Cognition, 36, 416–428.
Article
Google Scholar
Koriat, A., & Ma’ayan, H. (2005). The effects of encoding fluency and retrieval fluency on judgments of learning. Journal of Memory and Language, 52, 478–492.
Article
Google Scholar
Kornell, N., Hays, M. J., & Bjork, R. A. (2009). Unsuccessful retrieval attempts enhance subsequent learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 989–998.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Kornell, N., & Son, L. K. (2009). Learners’ choices and beliefs about self-testing. Memory, 17, 493–501.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Landauer, T. K., & Dumais, S. T. (1997). A solution to Plato’s problem: The latent semantic analysis theory of acquisition, induction, and representation of knowledge. Psychological Review, 104, 211–240.
Article
Google Scholar
Landauer, T. K., Foltz, P. W., & Laham, D. (1998). Introduction to latent semantic analysis. Discourse Processes, 25, 259–284.
Article
Google Scholar
Loftus, E. F. (1979). Eyewitness testimony. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Google Scholar
McDaniel, M. A., Agarwal, P. K., Huelser, B. J., McDermott, K. B., & Roediger, H. L., III. (2011). Test-enhanced learning in a middle school science classroom: The effects of quiz frequency and placement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103, 399–414.
Article
Google Scholar
McGeoch, J. A. (1942). The psychology of human learning. New York: Longmans.
Google Scholar
Melton, A. W., & Irwin, J. M. (1940). The influence of degree of interpolated learning on retroactive inhibition and the overt transfer of specific responses. American Journal of Psychology, 53, 173–203.
Article
Google Scholar
Metcalfe, J. (1990). Composite holographic associative recall model (CHARM) and blended memories in eyewitness testimony. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 119, 145–160.
Article
Google Scholar
Metcalfe, J. (2009). Metacognitive judgments and control of study. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 159–163.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Metcalfe, J., & Finn, B. (2008). Evidence that judgments of learning are causally related to study choice. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 174–179.
Article
Google Scholar
Metcalfe, J., & Kornell, N. (2007). Principles of cognitive science in education: The effects of generation, errors and feedback. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 225–229.
Article
Google Scholar
Metcalfe, J., Kornell, N., & Finn, B. (2009). Delayed versus immediate feedback in children's and adults' vocabulary learning. Memory & Cognition, 37, 1077–1087.
Article
Google Scholar
Miele, D. B., & Molden, D. C. (2010). Naïve theories of intelligence and the role of processing fluency in perceived comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139, 535–557.
Article
Google Scholar
Mulligan, N. W., & Lozito, J. P. (2005). Self-generation and memory. In B. H. Ross (Ed.), Psychology of learning and motivation (pp. 175–214). San Diego: Elsevier.
Google Scholar
Nelson, T. O., & Dunlosky, J. (1991). When people’s judgments of learning (JOLs) are extremely accurate at predicting subsequent recall: The “delayed-JOL-effect.” Psychological Science, 5, 207–213.
Nelson, D. L., McEvoy, C. L., & Schreiber, T. A. (1998). The University of South Florida word association, rhyme, and word fragment norms. Retrieved from http://w3.usf.edu/FreeAssociation/
Osgood, C. E. (1949). The similarity paradox in human learning: A resolution. Psychological Review, 56, 132–143.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Parlow, J., & Berlyne, D. E. (1971). The effect of prior guessing on incidental learning of verbal associations. Journal of Structural Learning, 2, 55–65.
Google Scholar
Pashler, H., Cepeda, N. J., Wixted, J. T., & Rohrer, D. (2005). When does feedback facilitate learning of words? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 3–8.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Pashler, H., Zarow, G., & Triplett, B. (2003). Is temporal spacing of tests helpful even when it inflates error rates? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 1051–1057.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Pavio, A., Yuille, J. C., & Madigan, S. A. (1968). Concreteness, imagery, and meaningfulness values for 925 nouns. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 76, 1–25.
Article
Google Scholar
Pieschl, S. (2009). Metacognitive calibration—an extended conceptualization and potential applications. Metacognition Learning, 4, 3–31.
Article
Google Scholar
Pyc, M. A., & Rawson, K. A. (2010). Why testing improves memory: Mediator effectiveness hypothesis. Science, 330, 335.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Rawson, K. A., & Dunlosky, J. (2002). Are performance predictions for text based on ease of processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28, 69–80.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Rhodes, M. G., & Castel, A. D. (2008). Memory predictions are influenced by perceptual information: Evidence for metacognitive illusions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137, 615–625.
Article
Google Scholar
Richland, L. E., Kornell, N., & Kao, L. S. (2009). The pretesting effect: Do unsuccessful retrieval attempts enhance learning? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 15, 243–257.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Slamecka, N. J., & Graf, P. (1978). The generation effect: Delineation of a phenomenon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 4, 592–604.
Article
Google Scholar
Slamecka, N. J., & Fevreiski, J. (1983). The generation effect when generation fails. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22, 153–163.
Article
Google Scholar
Son, L. K. (2004). Spacing one’s study: Evidence for a metacognitive control strategy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 601–604.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Son, L. K., & Kornell, N. (2008). Research on the allocation of study time: Key studies from 1890 to the present (and beyond). In J. Dunlosky & R. A. Bjork (Eds.), A handbook of memory and metamemory (pp. 333–351). Hillsdale, NJ: Psychology Press.
Google Scholar
Son, L. K., & Metcalfe, J. (2000). Metacognitive and control strategies in study-time allocation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 204–221.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Soraci, S. A., Jr., Carlin, M. T., Chechile, R. A., Franks, J. J., Wills, T., & Watanabe, T. (1999). Encoding variability and cuing in generative processing. Journal of Memory & Language, 41, 541–559.
Article
Google Scholar
Stone, N. J. (2000). Exploring the relationship between calibration and self-regulated learning. Educational Psychology Review, 12, 437–475.
Article
Google Scholar
Thiede, K. W., Anderson, M. C. M., & Therriault, D. (2003). Accuracy of metacognitive monitoring affects learning of texts. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 66–73.
Article
Google Scholar
Veksler, V. D., Grintsvayg, A., Lindsey, R., & Gray, W. D. (2007). A proxy for all your semantic needs. In D. S. McNamara & J. G. Trafton (Eds.), Proceedings of the 29th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (p. 1878). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society. Retrieved from http://cwl-projects.cogsci.rpi.edu/msr/
Webb, L. W. (1917). Transfer of training and retroaction: A comparative study. Psychological Monographs, 24, 1–90.
Article
Google Scholar
Winkielman, P., Schwarz, N., Fazendeiro, T., & Reber, R. (2003). The hedonic marking of processing fluency: Implications for evaluative judgment. In J. Musch & K. C. Klauer (Eds.), The psychology of evaluation: Affective processes in cognition and emotion (pp. 189–217). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Google Scholar