Learning & Behavior

, Volume 45, Issue 4, pp 335–354 | Cite as

Learning to play: A review and theoretical investigation of the developmental mechanisms and functions of cetacean play

  • Heather M. Hill
  • Sarah Dietrich
  • Briana Cappiello
Article

Abstract

Play is a behavioral phenomenon most commonly observed in the young of both solitary and social species. Documentation of play in cetaceans varies across species and settings. Cetacean play behavioral repertoires include a broad range of actions, such as the manipulation of diverse objects, blowing bubbles, chasing conspecifics, and swimming in spirals through the water. As is common in research on animal play, cetacean play has been grouped into categories by its form, including locomotor play, object play, and different variations of social play, such as affiliative games, play fighting, and socio-sexual play. Research has primarily focused on recording the topography of cetacean play and the demographics of the individuals engaging in play. However, these classifications are insufficient to address the possible developmental and societal functions of cetacean play behaviors, or the mechanisms with which play behaviors are spread between conspecifics and acquired by young members of cetacean populations. This article applies several developmental and social learning theories in order to organize current knowledge and guide future research.

Keywords

Cetacean Social learning Development Play 

Notes

Author note

This article would not exist if not for the legacy of Stan Kuczaj and his continuous efforts to merge the worlds of human development and animal cognition into a cohesive and rich discipline. This article is truly a reflection of Stan’s passions and love of science. We thank the Psychonomic Society for the opportunity and its support in bringing together some of the world’s foremost thinkers to the same table for three days to hash out what we currently understand about the evolutionary perspective on play, through the Society’s Leading Edge Series. This article has also provided a remarkable experience for H.M.H., who was afforded the opportunity to participate in the workshop and the development of this paper with Stan’s unexpected passing. We hope the ideas in this article resonate with those Stan was seeking to promote when he joined forces with Lance Miller and Alex de Voogt to explore the fun yet challenging topic of what play is and what it means through the workshop. We also thank Kathleen Dudzinski for her helpful comments on a previous draft of this paper.

References

  1. Asper, E. D., Young, W. G., & Walsh, M. T. (1988). Observations on the birth and development of a captive-born Killer whale Orcinus orca. International Zoo Yearbook, 27, 295–304.Google Scholar
  2. Baird, R. W. (2000). The killer whale. In J. Mann, R. C. Connor, P. L. Tyack, & H. Whitehead (Eds.), Cetacean societies: Field studies of dolphins and whales (pp. 127–153). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  3. Bakeman, R., & Brownlee, J. R. (1980). The strategic use of parallel play: A sequential analysis. Child Development, 51, 873–878.Google Scholar
  4. Baker, C. S., & Herman, L. M. (1984). Aggressive behavior between humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae) wintering in Hawaiian waters. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 62, 1922–1937.Google Scholar
  5. Baldwin, J. D., & Baldwin, J. D. (1978). The primate contribution to the study of play. In M. A. Salter (Ed.), Anthropological perspectives (pp. 53–68). New York, NY: Leisure Press.Google Scholar
  6. Barber, T. M. (2016). Variety and use of objects carried by provisioned wild Australian humpback dolphins (Sousa sahulensis) in Tin Can Bay, Queensland, Australia. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 29, 1–21. Retrieved from http://escholarship.org/uc/item/8pt5z83j Google Scholar
  7. Bauer, E. B., & Smuts, B. B. (2007). Cooperation and competition during dyadic play in domestic dogs, Canis familiaris. Animal Behavior, 73, 489–499.Google Scholar
  8. Bekoff, M. (1972). The development of social interaction, play, and metacommunication in mammals: An ethological perspective. Quarterly Review of Biology, 47, 412–434.Google Scholar
  9. Bender, C. E. (2012). Social play as a tool for developing social-cognitive skills in a wild population of Atlantic spotted dolphins (Stenella frontalis). Unpublished PhD thesis, Florida Atlantic University, Boca Raton, FL.Google Scholar
  10. Bender, C. E., Herzing, D. L., & Bjorklund, D. F. (2009). Evidence of teaching in Atlantic spotted dolphins (Stenella frontalis) by mother dolphins foraging in the presence of their calves. Animal Cognition, 12, 43–53.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Blackmore, S. (1998). Imitation and the definition of a meme. Journal of Memetics-Evolutionary Models of Information Transmission, 2, 159–170.Google Scholar
  12. Blois-Heulin, C., Rochais, C., Camus, S., Fureix, C., Lemasson, A., Lunel, C., & Hausberger, M. (2015). Animal welfare: Could adult play be a false friend. Animal Behavior and Cognition, 2, 156–185.Google Scholar
  13. Brooker, E., Blaise, M., & Edwards, S. (Eds.). (2014). SAGE handbook of play and learning in early childhood. New York, NY: Sage.Google Scholar
  14. Burghardt, G. M. (2005). The genesis of animal play: Testing the limits. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, Bradford Books.Google Scholar
  15. Cairns, R. B. (1976). The ontogeny and phylogeny of social interactions. In M. E. Hahn & E. C. Simmel (Eds.), Communicative behavior and evolution (pp. 115–139). New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  16. Chechina, O. N. (2009). Formation of behavior in bottle-nosed dolphin newborns under conditions of an oceanarium: Heterochrony of involvement of various types of afferentiation. Biology Bulletin, 36, 489–492.Google Scholar
  17. Clapham, P. J. (2000). The humpback whale. In J. Mann, R. C. Connor, P. L. Tyack, & H. Whitehead (Eds.), Cetacean societies: Field studies of dolphins and whales (pp. 173–196). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  18. Cockcroft, V., & Ross, G. J. B. (1990). Observations on the early development of a captive bottlenose dolphin calf. In S. Leatherwood & R. R. Reeves (Eds.), The bottlenose dolphin (pp. 461–478). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  19. Connor, R. C. (2000). Group living in whales and dolphins. In J. Mann, R. C. Connor, P. L. Tyack, & H. Whitehead (Eds.), Cetacean societies: Field studies of dolphins and whales (pp. 199–218). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  20. Connor, R. C., Heithaus, M. R., & Barre, L. M. (2001). Complex social structure, alliance stability and mating access in a bottlenose dolphin “super-alliance”. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 268, 263–267.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  21. Connor, R. C., & Krützen, M. (2015). Male dolphin alliances in Shark Bay: Changing perspectives in a 30-year study. Animal Behaviour, 103, 223–235.Google Scholar
  22. Connor, R. C., & Peterson, D. M. (1994). The lives of whales and dolphins. New York, NY: Holt.Google Scholar
  23. Connor, R. C., Wells, R. S., Mann, J., & Read, A. J. (2000). The bottlenose dolphin. In J. Mann, R. C. Connor, P. L. Tyack, & H. Whitehead (Eds.), Cetacean societies: Field studies of dolphins and whales (pp. 91–125). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  24. de Vere, A. J., & Kuczaj, S. A. (2016). Where are we in the study of animal emotions? Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 7, 354–362.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Deakos, M. H., Branstetter, B. K., Maxxuca, L., Fertl, D., & Mobley, J. R. (2010). Two unusual interactions between a bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) and a humpback whale (Megaptera noraeangliae) in Hawaiian waters. Aquatic Mammals, 36, 121–128.Google Scholar
  26. Deutsch, S., Pearson, H., & Würsig, B. (2014). Development of leaps in dusky dolphin (Lagenorhynchus obscurus) calves. Behaviour, 151, 1555–1577.Google Scholar
  27. Dibble, D. S., Van Alstyne, K. R., & Ridgway, S. (2016). Dolphins signal success by producing a victory squeal. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 29, 1–10. Retrieved from http://escholarship.org/uc/item/89w2680r Google Scholar
  28. Dietrich, S., Garza, S., Hill, H., & Aibel, S. (2013, July). Oral exploratory interaction: “The mouth game”—A developmental precursor to aggressive threats or courting behavior? Paper presented at the Animal Behavior Society Meeting (ABS), Boulder, CO.Google Scholar
  29. Everitt, R. D., & Krogman, B. D. (1979). Sexual behavior of bowhead whales observed off the north coast of Alaska. Arctic, 32, 277–280.Google Scholar
  30. Fagen, R. (1981). Animal play behavior. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  31. Fagen, R. (1995). Animal play, games of angels, biology and brain. In A. D. Pellegrini (Ed.), The future of play theory. Albany, NY: New York University Press.Google Scholar
  32. Félix, F. (2015). Rope rubbing social play behavior recorded from wild bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) in Ecuador. Aquatic Mammals, 41, 222–225.Google Scholar
  33. Fellner, W., Bauer, G. B., & Harley, H. E. (2006). Cognitive implications of synchrony in dolphins: A review. Aquatic Mammals, 32, 511–516.Google Scholar
  34. Fertl, D., & Würsig, B. (1995). Coordinated feeding by Atlantic spotted dolphins (Stenella frontalis) in the Gulf of Mexico. Aquatic Mammals, 21, 3–5.Google Scholar
  35. Freeberg, T. M. (2000). Culture and courtship in vertebrates: A review of social learning and transmission of courtship systems and mating patterns. Behavioural Processes, 51, 177–192.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. George, E. M., & Noonan, M. (2016). Underwater bubbling in beluga whales (Delphinapterus leucas): Rates of production as a function of age, sex, and bubble type. Integrative and Comparative Biology, 56, E290–E290.Google Scholar
  37. Gewalt, W. (1989). Orinoco-freshwater-dolphins (Inia geoffrensis) using self-produced air bubble rings as toys. Aquatic Mammals, 15, 73–79.Google Scholar
  38. Glabicky, N., DuBrava, A., & Noonan, M. (2010). Social–sexual behavior seasonality in captive beluga whales (Delphinapterus leucas). Polar Biology, 33, 1145–1147.Google Scholar
  39. Goldman, J. A. (1981). Social participation of preschool children in same-versus mixed-age groups. Child Development, 52, 644–650.Google Scholar
  40. Gomendio, M. (1988). The development of different types of play in gazelles: implications for the nature and functions of play. Animal Behaviour, 36, 825–836.Google Scholar
  41. Greene, W. E., Melillo-Sweeting, K., & Dudzinski, K. M. (2011). Comparing object play in captive and wild dolphins. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 24, 292–306.Google Scholar
  42. Groos, K. (1898). The play of animals. New York, NY: D. Appleton.Google Scholar
  43. Groos, K. (1901). The play of man. London, UK: Heinemann.Google Scholar
  44. Guarino, S., Hill, H. M., & Sigman, J. (2016). Development of sociality and emergence of independence in a killer whale (Orcinus orca) calf from birth to 36 months. Zoo Biology, 36, 11–20.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Guinet, C. (1991). Intentional stranding apprenticeship and social play in killer whales (Orcinus orca). Canadian Journal of Zoology, 69, 2712–2716.Google Scholar
  46. Guinet, C., & Bouvier, J. (1995). Development of intentional stranding hunting techniques in killer whale (Orcinus orca) calves at Crozet Archipelago. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 73, 27–33.Google Scholar
  47. Hausberger, M., Fureix, C., Bourjade, M., Wessel-Robert, S., & Richard-Yris, M. A. (2012). On the significance of adult play: What does social play tell us about adult horse welfare? Naturwissenschaften, 99, 291–302.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Held, S. D., & Špinka, M. (2011). Animal play and animal welfare. Animal Behaviour, 81, 891–899.Google Scholar
  49. Herzing, D. L. (2005). Transmission mechanisms of social learning in dolphins: Underwater observations of free-ranging dolphins in the Bahamas. In F. Delfour & M. J. Dubois (Eds.), Autour de l’ethologie et de la cognition animale (pp. 185–193). Lyon, France: Presses Universitaires de Lyon.Google Scholar
  50. Herzing, D. L., & Johnson, C. M. (1997). Interspecific interactions between Atlantic spotted dolphins (Stenella frontalis) and bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) in the Bahamas, 1985–1995. Aquatic Mammals, 23, 85–99.Google Scholar
  51. Herzing, D. L., Moewe, K., & Brunnick, B. J. (2003). Interspecies interactions between Atlantic spotted dolphins, Stenella frontalis, and bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops truncatus, on Great Bahama Bank, Bahamas. Aquatic Mammals, 29, 335–341.Google Scholar
  52. Highfill, L. E., Yeater, D., & Kuczaj, S. A., II. (2016). Catch! Dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) ball tossing to humans is affected by human perspective. Aquatic Mammals, 42, 253–258.Google Scholar
  53. Hill, H. M. (2009). The behavioral development of two beluga calves during the first year of life. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 22, 234–253.Google Scholar
  54. Hill, H. M., Kahn, M. S., Brilliott, L. J., Roberts, B. M., Gutierrez, C., & Artz, S. (2011). Beluga (Delphinapterus leucas) Bubble bursts: Surprise, protection, or play?. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 24, 235–243.Google Scholar
  55. Hill, H. M., Campbell, C., Dalton, L., & Osborn, S. (2013). The first year of behavioral development and maternal care of beluga (Delphinapterus leucas) calves in human care. Zoo Biology, 32, 565–570.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Hill, H. M., Dietrich, S., Yeater, D., McKinnon, M., Miller, M., Aibel, S., & Dove, A. (2015). Developing a catalog of socio-sexual behaviors of beluga whales (Delphinapterus leucas). Animal Behavior and Cognition, 2, 105–123.Google Scholar
  57. Hill, H., Guarino, S., Crandall, S., Lenhart, E., & Dietrich, S. (2015). Young belugas diversify adult beluga (Delphinapterus leucas) behavior. Animal Behavior and Cognition, 2, 267–284.Google Scholar
  58. Hill, H., & Ramirez, D. (2014). Adults play but not like their young: The frequency and types of play by belugas (Delphinapterus leucas) in human care. Animal Behavior and Cognition, 1, 166–185.Google Scholar
  59. Hill, H. M., Yeater, D., Gallup, S., Guarino, S., Lacy, S., Dees, T., & Kuczaj, S. (2016). Responses to familiar and unfamiliar humans by belugas (Delphinapterus leucas), bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus), & Pacific white-sided dolphins (Lagenorhynchus obliquidens): A replication and extension. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 29, 1–21. Retrieved from http://escholarship.org/uc/item/48j4v1s8 Google Scholar
  60. Hughes, C., & Leekam, S. (2004). What are the links between theory of mind and social relations? Review, reflections and new directions for studies of typical and atypical development. Social Development, 13, 590–619.Google Scholar
  61. Jaakkola, K., Guarino, E., Rodriguez, M., Erb, L., & Trone, M. (2010). What do dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) understand about hidden objects? Animal Cognition, 13, 103–120.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  62. Janik, V. M. (2015). Play in dolphins. Current Biology, 25, R7–R8.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  63. Johnson, C. M., & Norris, K. S. (1994). Social behavior. In K. S. Norris, B. Würsig, R. S. Wells, & M. Würsig (Eds.), The Hawaiian spinner dolphin (pp. 243–286). Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
  64. Johnson, C. M., Sullivan, J., Buck, C. L., Trexel, J., & Scarpuzzi, M. (2015). Visible and invisible displacement with dynamic visual occlusion in bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp. Animal Cognition, 18, 179–193.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  65. Jones, B., & Kuczaj, S. A. (2014). Beluga (Delphinapterus leucas) novel bubble helix play behavior. Animal Behavior and Cognition, 1, 206–214.Google Scholar
  66. Kilborn, S. S. (1994). Object carrying in a captive beluga whale (Delphinapterus leucas) as possible surrogate behavior. Marine Mammal Science, 10, 496–501.Google Scholar
  67. Kirkpatrick, M., & Dugatkin, L. A. (1994). Sexual selection and the evolutionary effects of copying mate choice. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 34, 443–449.Google Scholar
  68. Krasnova, V. V., Chernetsky, A. D., Zheludkova, A. I., & Bel’kovich, V. M. (2014). Parental behavior of the beluga whale (Delphinapterus leucas) in natural environment. Biology Bulletin, 41, 349–356.Google Scholar
  69. Kuczaj, S. A., & Eskelinen, H. C. (2014). Why do dolphins play? Animal Behavior and Cognition, 1, 113–127.Google Scholar
  70. Kuczaj, S. A., & Frick, E. E. (2015, December). What do dolphins mean when they open their mouth? Poster presented at the meeting of the Society for Marine Mammalogy, San Francisco, CA.Google Scholar
  71. Kuczaj, S. A., & Highfill, L. E. (2005). Dolphin play: Evidence for cooperation and culture. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 405–406.Google Scholar
  72. Kuczaj, S. A., II, & Horback, K. M. (2012). Play and emotion. In S. Watanabe & S. Kuczaj (Eds.), Emotions of animals and humans (pp. 87–112). Tokyo, Japan: Springer Japan.Google Scholar
  73. Kuczaj, S. A., II, & Makecha, R. (2008). The role of play in the evolution and ontogeny of contextually flexible communication. In D. K. Oller & U. Griebel (Eds.), Evolution of communicative flexibility: Complexity, creativity, and adaptability in human and animal communication (pp. 253–277). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  74. Kuczaj, S. A., II, Makecha, R., Trone, M., Paulos, R. D., & Ramos, J. A. (2006). Role of peers in cultural innovation and cultural transmission: Evidence from the play of dolphin calves. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 19, 223–240.Google Scholar
  75. Kuczaj, S., Tranel, K., Trone, M., & Hill, H. (2001). Are animals capable of deception or empathy? Implications for animal consciousness and animal welfare. Animal Welfare, 10, S161–S174.Google Scholar
  76. Kuczaj, S. A., II, Winship, K. A., & Eskelinen, H. C. (2015). Can bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) cooperate when solving a novel task? Animal Cognition, 18, 543–550.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  77. Kuczaj, S. A., II, & Yeater, D. B. (2006). Dolphin imitation: Who, what, when, and why? Aquatic Mammals, 32, 413–422.Google Scholar
  78. Kuczaj, S. A., II, & Yeater, D. B. (2007). Observations of rough-toothed dolphins (Steno bredanensis) of the coast of Utila, Honduras. Journal of the Marine Biological, 87, 141–148.Google Scholar
  79. Laland, K. N. (1994a). A theoretical investigation of the role of social transmission in evolution. Ethology and Sociobiology, 13, 87–113.Google Scholar
  80. Laland, K. N. (1994b). On the evolutionary consequences of sexual imprinting. Evolution, 48, 477–489.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  81. Leca, J. B., Gunst, N., & Huffman, M. A. (2007). Age-related differences in the performance, diffusion, and maintenance of stone handling, a behavioral tradition in Japanese macaques. Journal of Human Evolution, 53, 691–708.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  82. Lillard, A. S. (2015). The development of play. In L. S. Liben & U. Mueller (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science processes (7th ed., Vol. 2, pp. 425–468). New York, NY: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
  83. Lomac-Macnair, K. S., Smultea, M. A., Cotter, M. P., Thissen, C., & Parker, L. (2015). Socio-sexual and probable mating behavior of Cook Inlet beluga whales, Delphinapterus leucas, observed from an aircraft. Marine Fisheries Review, 77, 32–40.Google Scholar
  84. Lopez, J. C., & Lopez, D. (1985). Killer whales (Orcinus orca) and their behavior of intentional stranding while hunting nearshore. Journal of Mammalogy, 66, 181–183.Google Scholar
  85. Lorenz, K. Z. (1958). The evolution of behavior. Scientific American, 199, 67–78.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  86. Lusseau, D. (2006). Why do dolphins jump? Interpreting the behavioural repertoire of bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp.) in Doubtful Sound, New Zealand. Behavioural Processes, 73, 257–265.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  87. Lyamin, O. I., Manger, P. R., Ridgway, S. H., Mukhametov, L. M., & Siegel, J. M. (2008). Cetacean sleep: An unusual form of mammalian sleep. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 32, 1451–1484. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.05.023 Google Scholar
  88. Mackey, A. D., Makecha, R. N., & Kuczaj, S. A., II. (2014). The development of social play in bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Animal Behavior and Cognition, 1, 19–35.Google Scholar
  89. Mann, J. (2006). Establishing trust: Socio-sexual behaviour and the development of male-male bonds among Indian Ocean bottlenose dolphins. In V. Sommer & P. L. Vasey (Eds.), Homosexual behaviour in animals (pp. 107–130). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  90. Mann, J., & Smuts, B. (1999). Behavioral development in wild bottlenose dolphin newborns (Tursiops sp.). Behaviour, 136, 529–566.Google Scholar
  91. Martin, A. R., da Silva, V. M. F., & Rothery, P. (2008). Object carrying as socio-sexual display in an aquatic mammal. Biology Letters, 4, 243–245.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  92. Mason, W. A. (1968). Early social deprivation in the nonhuman primates: Implications for human behavior. In D. C. Glass (Ed.), Environmental influence (pp. 70–101). New York, NY: Rockefeller University Press and Russell Sage Foundation.Google Scholar
  93. McBride, A., & Kritzler, H. (1951). Observations of pregnancy, parturition, and post-natal behavior in the bottlenose dolphin. Journal of Mammalogy, 32, 251–266.Google Scholar
  94. McCowan, B., Marino, L., Vance, E., Walke, L., & Reiss, D. (2000). Bubble ring play of bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus): Implications for cognition. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 114, 98–106.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  95. Mitchell, R. W. (1990). A theory of play. Interpretation and Explanation in the Study of Animal Behavior, 1, 197–227.Google Scholar
  96. Noren, S. R., Biedenbach, G., & Edwards, E. F. (2006). Ontogeny of swim performance and mechanics in bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Journal of Experimental Biology, 209, 4724–4731.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  97. Norris, K. S., Wursig, B., & Wells, R. S. (1994). The spinner dolphin. In K. S. Norris, B. Wursig, R. S. Wells, & M. Wursig (Eds.), The Hawaiian spinner dolphin. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
  98. Osvath, M., & Sima, M. (2014). Sub-adult ravens synchronize their play: A case of emotional contagion. Animal Behaviour and Cognition, 1, 197–205.Google Scholar
  99. Owen, K., Dunlop, R., & Donnelly, D. (2012). Seaweed interactions by humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae): A form of object play? Aquatic Mammals, 38, 418–422.Google Scholar
  100. Pace, D. S. (2000). Fluke-made bubble rings as toys in bottlenose dolphin calves (Tursiops truncatus). Aquatic Mammals, 26, 57–64.Google Scholar
  101. Pack, A. A., & Herman, L. M. (2004). Bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) comprehend the referent of both static and dynamic human gazing and pointing in an object-choice task. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 118, 160–171.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  102. Palagi, E. (2009). Adult play fighting and potential role of tail signals in ringtailed lemurs (Lemur catta). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 123, 1–9. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.123.1.1 PubMedGoogle Scholar
  103. Palagi, E., Burghardt, G. M., Smuts, B., Cordoni, G., Dall’Olio, S., Fouts, H. N., & Pellis, S. M. (2016). Rough-and-tumble play as a window on animal communication. Biological Reviews, 91, 311–327.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  104. Parten, M. (1932). Social participation among preschool children. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 27, 243–269.Google Scholar
  105. Paulos, R. D., Trone, M., & Kuczaj, S. A. (2010). Play in wild and captive cetaceans. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 23, 701–722.Google Scholar
  106. Pearson, H. C. (2017). Unravelling the function of dolphin leaps using the dusky dolphin (Lagenorhynchus obscurus) as a model species. Behaviour, 154, 563–581.Google Scholar
  107. Pellis, S. M., & McKenna, M. (1992). Intrinsic and extrinsic influences on play fighting in rats: Effects of dominance, partner playfulness, temperament and neonatal exposure to testosterone propionate. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 50, 135–145.Google Scholar
  108. Pellis, S., & Pellis, V. (2007). Rough-and-tumble play and the development of the social brain. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 95–98.Google Scholar
  109. Petit, O., Bertrand, B., & Thierry, B. (2008). Social play in crested and Japanese macaques: Testing the covariation hypothesis. Journal of Developmental Psychobiology, 50, 399–407.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  110. Piaget, J. (1951). Plays, dreams, and imitation in childhood. New York, NY: Norton.Google Scholar
  111. Provine, R. R. (1996a). Contagious yawning and laughter: Significance for sensory feature detection. In C. M. Heyes & B. G. Galef (Eds.), Social learning in animals: The roots of culture (p. 179-208). Academic Press: San Diego.Google Scholar
  112. Provine, R. R. (1996a). Laughter. American Scientist, 84, 38–45.Google Scholar
  113. Provine, R. R. (2005). Yawning: The yawn is primal, unstoppable and contagious, revealing the evolutionary and neural basis of empathy and unconscious behavior. American Scientist, 93, 532–539.Google Scholar
  114. Pryor, K. W., Lindbergh, J., Lindbergh, S., & Milano, R. (1990). A dolphin-human fishing cooperative in Brazil. Marine Mammal Science, 6, 77–82.Google Scholar
  115. Rendell, L., & Whitehead, H. (2001). Culture in whales and dolphins. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 309–324.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  116. Rose, N. (1992). The social dynamics of male killer whales, Orcinus orca (Doctoral dissertation). British Columbia University.Google Scholar
  117. Santos, M. C. D. O. (2010). Guiana dolphins (Sotalia guianensis) displaying beach hunting behavior in the Cananéia estuary, Brazil: Social context and conservation issues. Brazilian Journal of Oceanography, 58, 143–152.Google Scholar
  118. Sargeant, B. L., & Mann, J. (2009). Developmental evidence for foraging traditions in wild bottlenose dolphins. Animal Behaviour, 78, 715–721.Google Scholar
  119. Sargeant, B. L., Mann, J., Berggren, P., & Krützen, M. (2005). Specialization and development of beach hunting, a rare foraging behaviour, by wild bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp.). Canadian Journal of Zoology, 83, 1400–1410.Google Scholar
  120. Silber, G. K., & Fertl, D. (1995). Intentional beaching by bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) in the Colorado River Delta, Mexico. Aquatic Mammals, 21, 183–186.Google Scholar
  121. Similä, T., & Ugarte, F. (1993). Surface and underwater observations of cooperatively feeding killer whales in Northern Norway. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 71, 1494–1499.Google Scholar
  122. Smith, B. D., Thant, U. H., Lwin, J. M., & Shaw, C. D. (1997). Investigation of cetaceans in the Ayayarwady River and northern coastal waters of Myanmar. Asian Marine Biology, 14, 173–94.Google Scholar
  123. Smolker, R. A., Richards, A. F., Connor, R. C., Mann, J., & Berggren, P. (1997). Sponge-carrying by Indian Ocean bottlenose dolphins: Possible tool-use by a delphinid. Ethology, 103, 454–65.Google Scholar
  124. Spencer, H. (1873). The principles of psychology. Appleton: New York.Google Scholar
  125. Tayler, C. K., & Saayman, G. S. (1973). Imitative behaviour by Indian Ocean bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops aduncus) in captivity. Behaviour, 44, 286–298.Google Scholar
  126. Thieltges, H., Lemasson, A., Kuczaj, S., Böye, M., & Blois-Heulin, C. (2011). Visual laterality in dolphins when looking at (un)familiar humans. Animal Cognition, 14, 303–308. doi: 10.1007/s10071-010-0354-5 PubMedGoogle Scholar
  127. Thomas, P. O., & Taber, S. M. (1984). Mother–infant interaction and behavioral development in southern right whales, Eubalaena australis. Behaviour, 88, 42–60.Google Scholar
  128. Tschudin, A. J. (2001). “Mindreading” mammals? Attribution of belief tasks with dolphins. Animal Welfare, 10, 119–127.Google Scholar
  129. Tyack, P., & Whitehead, H. (1983). Male competition in large groups of wintering humpback whales. Behaviour, 83, 132–154.Google Scholar
  130. Varlinskaya, E. I., Spear, L. P., & Spear, N. E. (1999). Social behavior and social motivation in adolescent rats: Role of housing conditions and partner’s activity. Physiology & Behavior, 67, 475–482.Google Scholar
  131. Wade, P. R., Reeves, R. R., & Mesnick, S. L. (2012). Social and behavioural factors in cetacean responses to overexploitation: Are Odontocetes less “resilient” than Mysticetes? Journal of Marine Biology, 2012(567276), 1–15. doi: 10.1155/2012/567276 Google Scholar
  132. Weaver, A., & Kuczaj, S. (2016). Neither toy nor tool: Grass-wearing behavior among free-ranging bottlenose dolphins in Western Florida. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 29, 1–18. Retrieved from http://escholarship.org/uc/item/893417x3 Google Scholar
  133. Whitehead, A. N. (1985). Symbolism: Its meaning and effect. New York, NY: Fordham University Press.Google Scholar
  134. Würsig, B., & Würsig, M. (1980). Behavior and ecology of the dusky dolphin, Lagenorhynchus obscurus, in the South Atlantic. Fisheries Bulletin, 77, 871–890.Google Scholar
  135. Yando, R., Seitz, V., & Zigler, E. (1978). Imitation: A developmental perspective. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  136. Yeater, D., & Kuczaj, S. (2010). Observational learning in wild and captive dolphins. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 23, 379–385.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  • Heather M. Hill
    • 1
  • Sarah Dietrich
    • 2
  • Briana Cappiello
    • 3
  1. 1.St. Mary’s UniversitySan AntonioUSA
  2. 2.University at Buffalo, State University of New YorkBuffaloUSA
  3. 3.The University of Southern MississippiHattiesburgUSA

Personalised recommendations