Learning & Behavior

, Volume 45, Issue 4, pp 330–334 | Cite as

Creating a common terminology for play behavior to increase cross-disciplinary research

Article

Abstract

Historically, play behavior has been difficult to define. This likely stems from the number of different species, types of play, and context under which it occurs. In 2016, the Chicago Zoological Society – Brookfield Zoo hosted the Psychonomic Society leading edge workshop on the evolutionary and psychological significance of play. Sixteen experts attended from the diverse fields of African ethnology, animal behavior, animal science, animal welfare, cognitive psychology, cognitive zoology, comparative psychology, cultural anthropology, developmental psychology, educational psychology, ethology, neuroscience, primatology, and zoology. Approximately half of the participants studied human play and the other half studied non-human play. Before the workshop, participants were asked to send in either their personal definition of play or the one that they cite in peer-reviewed literature. Definitions were then reviewed to determine characteristics of play inclusive of all disciplines. The goal of the current study was not to do a literature review on play behavior, but to come up with a list of characteristics across all forms of play that could be used as a common terminology moving forward. Hopefully the results of this workshop and the current article will help to increase cross-disciplinary research in the field of play.

Keywords

Animal Behavior Common language Evolutionary benefit Human Play 

Notes

Acknowledgements

I would like to thank all of the participants of the 2016 Psychonomic Society Workshop held at the Chicago Zoological Society – Brookfield Zoo. This includes Alex de Voogt, Cindy Dell Clark, Vladimir Dinets, Tracy Gleason, Fernand Gobet, Artin Göncü, Martine Hausberger, Heather Hill, Angeline Lillard, Mathias Osvath, Elisabetta Palagi, Jaak Panksepp, Sergio Pellis, Jeff Rushen, and Marek Spinka. I would also like to thank the Psychonomic Society for funding the workshop and Louis Shomette for all his assistance. Finally, I would like to thank Stan Kuczaj (1951-2016) for making the workshop possible. He will be forever missed and his dedication to the field will never be forgotten.

References

  1. Auersperg, A. M., van Horik, J. O., Bugnyar, T., Kacelnik, A., Emery, N. J., & von Bayern, A. M. (2015). Combinatory actions during object play in psittaciformes (Diopsittaca nobilis, Pionites melanocephala, Cacatua goffini) and corvids (Corvus corax, C. monedula, C. moneduloides). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 129(1), 62.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Barabanov, V., Gulimova, V., Berdiev, R., & Saveliev, S. (2015). Object play in thick-toed geckos during a space experiment. Journal of Ethology, 33(2), 109–115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Burghardt, G. M. (2005). The genesis of animal play: Testing the limits. Mit Press.Google Scholar
  4. Burghardt, G. M. (2015). Play in fishes, frogs and reptiles. Current Biology, 25(1), R9–R10.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Burghardt, G. M., Dinets, V., & Murphy, J. B. (2015). Highly repetitive object play in a cichlid fish (Tropheus duboisi). Ethology, 121(1), 38–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Century, J., Cassata, A., Rudnick, M., & Freeman, C. (2012). Measuring enactment of innovations and the factors that affect implementation and sustainability: Moving toward common language and shared conceptual understanding. The Journal of Behavioral Health Services & Research, 39(4), 343–361.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Cordoni, G., & Palagi, E. (2016). Aggression and hierarchical steepness inhibit social play in adult wolves. Behaviour, 153(6–7), 749–766.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Dinets, V. (2015). Play behavior in crocodilians. Animal Behavior and Cognition, 2(1), 49–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Edmonds, W. A., & Kennedy, T. D. (2016). An Applied Guide to Research Designs: Quantitative, Qualitative, and Mixed Methods. Sage Publications.Google Scholar
  10. Fagan, R. (1981). Animal play behavior. Oxford: Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
  11. García, F., Bertoa, M. F., Calero, C., Vallecillo, A., Ruíz, F., Piattini, M., & Genero, M. (2006). Towards a consistent terminology for software measurement. Information and Software Technology, 48(8), 631–644.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Gleason, T. R. (2013). Imaginary Relationships 17. The oxford handbook of the development of imagination, 251.Google Scholar
  13. Harding, E. J., Paul, E. S., & Mendl, M. (2004). Animal behaviour: Cognitive bias and affective state. Nature, 427(6972), 312.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Jurasinski, G., Retzer, V., & Beierkuhnlein, C. (2009). Inventory, differentiation, and proportional diversity: A consistent terminology for quantifying species diversity. Oecologia, 159(1), 15–26.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Kalueff, A. V., Gebhardt, M., Stewart, A. M., Cachat, J. M., Brimmer, M., Chawla, J. S., … & Gaikwad, S. (2013). Towards a comprehensive catalog of zebrafish behavior 1.0 and beyond. Zebrafish, 10(1), 70–86.Google Scholar
  16. Kuba, M. J., Gutnick, T., & Burghardt, G. M. (2014). Learning from play in octopus. Cephalopod cognition, 57.Google Scholar
  17. Kuczaj, S. A., & Eskelinen, H. C. (2014). Why do dolphins play? Animal Behavior and Cognition, 1(2), 113–127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Lillard, A. S. (2014). The development of play. In L. S. Liben, U. Müller, R. M. Lerner, L. S. Liben, U. Müller, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science: Cognitive processes (7th ed, Vol. 2, pp. 425–468). Hoboken: Wiley.Google Scholar
  19. Mendl, M., Burman, O. H., Parker, R. M., & Paul, E. S. (2009). Cognitive bias as an indicator of animal emotion and welfare: Emerging evidence and underlying mechanisms. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 118(3), 161–181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Muir, I., Munroe-Chandler, K., & Guerrero, M. (2016). A literature review of children's active play. Journal of Exercise, Movement, and Sport, 48(1), 203.Google Scholar
  21. Osvath, M., Osvath, H., & Bååth, R. (2014). An exploration of play behaviors in raven nestlings. Animal Behavior and Cognition, 1(2), 157–165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Pellis, S. M., & Iwaniuk, A. N. (2000). Adult–adult play in primates: Comparative analyses of its origin, distribution and evolution. Ethology, 106(12), 1083–1104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Pellis, S. M., & Pellis, V. C. (2012). Play-fighting during early childhood and its role in preventing later chronic aggression. Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development, 2012, 1–4.Google Scholar
  24. Proctor, H. (2012). Animal sentience: Where are we and where are we heading? Animals, 2(4), 628–639.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  25. Rubin, K. H., Fein, G. G., & Vandenberg, B. (1983). Play. Handbook of Child Psychology, 4, 693–774.Google Scholar
  26. Zylinski, S. (2015). Fun and play in invertebrates. Current Biology, 25(1), R10–R12.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Chicago Zoological Society – Brookfield ZooBrookfieldUSA

Personalised recommendations