Alós-Ferrer, C., Garagnani, M., & Hügelschäfer, S. (2016). Cognitive reflection, decision biases, and response times. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1402.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Bar-Hillel, M. (1980). The base-rate fallacy in probability judgments. Acta Psychologica, 44(3), 211–233.
Article
Google Scholar
Baron, J., & Hershey, J. C. (1988). Outcome bias in decision evaluation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(4), 569–579. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.4.569
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Bartolo, R., & Averbeck, B. B. (2021). Inference as a fundamental process in behavior. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 38, 8–13.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Behrens, T. E., Hunt, L. T., Woolrich, M. W., & Rushworth, M. F. (2008). Associative learning of social value. Nature, 456(7219), 245–249.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
Benrimoh, D., Parr, T., Vincent, P., Adams, R. A., & Friston, K. (2018). Active inference and auditory hallucinations. Computational Psychiatry (Cambridge, Mass.), 2, 183–204. https://doi.org/10.1162/cpsy_a_00022
Article
Google Scholar
Birnbaum, M. H. (1983). Base rates in Bayesian inference: Signal detection analysis of the cab problem. The American Journal of Psychology, 96(1), 85–94.
Article
Google Scholar
Dasgupta, I., Schulz, E., Goodman, N. D., & Gershman, S. J. (2018). Remembrance of inferences past: Amortization in human hypothesis generation. Cognition, 178, 67–81.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Dasgupta, I., Schulz, E., Tenenbaum, J. B., & Gershman, S. J. (2020). A theory of learning to infer. Psychological Review, 127(3), 412–441. https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000178
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
De Berker, A. O., Rutledge, R. B., Mathys, C., Marshall, L., Cross, G. F., Dolan, R. J., & Bestmann, S. (2016). Computations of uncertainty mediate acute stress responses in humans. Nature Communications, 7(1), 1–11.
Article
Google Scholar
De Lange, F. P., Heilbron, M., & Kok, P. (2018). How do expectations shape perception? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(9), 764–779.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Devaine, M., Hollard, G., & Daunizeau, J. (2014). Theory of mind: did evolution fool us? PLoS One, 9(2), e87619.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
Diaconescu, A. O., Stecy, M., Kasper, L., Burke, C. J., Nagy, Z., Mathys, C., & Tobler, P. N. (2020). Neural arbitration between social and individual learning systems. ELife, 9. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.54051
Diederich, A., Wyszynski, M., & Ritov, I. (2018). Moderators of framing effects in variations of the Asian Disease problem: Time constraint, need and disease type. Judgment and Decision making, 13(6), 529.
Google Scholar
Eldar, E., Rutledge, R. B., Dolan, R. J., & Niv, Y. (2016). Mood as representation of momentum. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(1), 15–24.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
Eldar, E., Felso, V., Cohen, J. D., & Niv, Y. (2021). A pupillary index of susceptibility to decision biases. Nature Human Behaviour. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-020-01006-3
Epley, N., & Gilovich, T. (2006). The anchoring-and-adjustment heuristic: Why the adjustments are insufficient. Psychological Science, 17(4), 311–318.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Fields, C., & Glazebrook, J. F. (2020). Information flow in context-dependent hierarchical Bayesian inference. Journal of Experimental & Theoretical Artificial Intelligence, 34(1), 111–142.
Article
Google Scholar
Fradkin, I., Adams, R. A., Parr, T., Roiser, J. P., & Huppert, J. D. (2020a). Searching for an anchor in an unpredictable world: A computational model of obsessive compulsive disorder. Psychological Review, 127(5), 672–699. https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000188
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Fradkin, I., Ludwig, C., Eldar, E., & Huppert, J. D. (2020b). Doubting what you already know: Uncertainty regarding state transitions is associated with obsessive compulsive symptoms. PLoS Computational Biology, 16(2), e1007634.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
Friston, K. (2012). The history of the future of the Bayesian brain. Neuroimage, 62(2), 1230–1233.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Fudenberg, D., Levine, D. K., & Maniadis, Z. (2012). On the robustness of anchoring effects in WTP and WTA experiments. American Economic Journal: Microeconomics, 4(2), 131–145.
Google Scholar
Geisler, W. S., Perry, J. S., Super, B. J., & Gallogly, D. P. (2001). Edge co-occurrence in natural images predicts contour grouping performance. Vision Research, 41(6), 711–724.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Gigerenzer, G. (2018). The bias in behavioral economics. Review of Behavioral Economics, 5(3-4), 303–336.
Article
Google Scholar
Gigerenzer, G., & Brighton, H. (2009). Homo heuristicus: Why biased minds make better inferences. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1(1), 107–143.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Gigerenzer, G., Hell, W., & Blank, H. (1988). Presentation and content: The use of base rates as a continuous variable. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14(3), 513.
Google Scholar
Gigerenzer, G. E., Hertwig, R. E., & Pachur, T. E. (2011). Heuristics: The foundations of adaptive behavior. Oxford University Press.
Book
Google Scholar
Gilovich, T., Vallone, R., & Tversky, A. (1985). The hot hand in basketball: On the misperception of random sequences. Cognitive Psychology, 17(3), 295–314.
Article
Google Scholar
Gilovich, T., Griffin, D., & Kahneman, D. (2002). In T. Gilovich, D. Griffin, & D. Kahneman (Eds.), Heuristics and biases: the psychology of intuitive judgment. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511808098
Chapter
Google Scholar
Glaze, C. M., Filipowicz, A. L. S., Kable, J. W., Balasubramanian, V., & Gold, J. I. (2018). A bias–variance trade-off governs individual differences in on-line learning in an unpredictable environment. Nature Human Behaviour, 2(3), 213–224. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0297-4
Article
Google Scholar
Griffiths, T. L., Chater, N., Kemp, C., Perfors, A., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2010). Probabilistic models of cognition: exploring representations and inductive biases. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(8), 357–364. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.05.004
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Grüne-Yanoff, T. (2017). Reflections on the 2017 Nobel memorial prize awarded to Richard Thaler. Erasmus Journal for Philosophy and Economics, 10(2), 61–75.
Article
Google Scholar
Gul, S., Krueger, P. M., Callaway, F., & Griffiths, T. L. (2018). Discovering rational heuristics for risky choice. In: The 14th biannual conference of the German Society for Cognitive Science, GK.
Guomei, Z., & Qicheng, J. (2003). Psychologist Daniel Kahneman wins 2002 Nobel prize in economics. Advances in Psychological Science, 11(01), 1.
Google Scholar
Gweon, H. (2021). Inferential social learning: Cognitive foundations of human social learning and teaching. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 25(10), 896–910.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Hertwig, R., Leuker, C., Pachur, T., Spiliopoulos, L., & Pleskac, T. J. (2021). Studies in ecological rationality. Topics in Cognitive Science. https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12567
Hesp, C., Smith, R., Parr, T., Allen, M., Friston, K. J., & Ramstead, M. J. D. (2021). Deeply felt affect: the emergence of valence in deep active inference. Neural Computation, 33(2), 398–446.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
Hintzman, D. L. (1976). Repetition and memory. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 10, 47–91.
Article
Google Scholar
Igou, E. R., & Bless, H. (2007). On undesirable consequences of thinking: Framing effects as a function of substantive processing. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 20(2), 125–142.
Article
Google Scholar
Ioannidis, K., Offerman, T., & Sloof, R. (2020). On the effect of anchoring on valuations when the anchor is transparently uninformative. Journal of the Economic Science Association, 6(1), 77–94.
Article
Google Scholar
Jacowitz, K. E., & Kahneman, D. (1995). Measures of anchoring in estimation tasks. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21(11), 1161–1166.
Article
Google Scholar
John, P. (2018). How far to nudge?: Assessing behavioural public policy. Edward Elgar Publishing.
Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, Fast and Slow. Penguin/Robinson.
Google Scholar
Kahneman, D., Slovic, S. P., Slovic, P., & Tversky, A. (Eds.). (1982). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Cambridge University Press.
Google Scholar
Kemp, C., Perfors, A., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2007). Learning overhypotheses with hierarchical Bayesian models. Developmental Science, 10(3), 307–321.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Keramati, M., Smittenaar, P., Dolan, R. J., & Dayan, P. (2016). Adaptive integration of habits into depth-limited planning defines a habitual-goal-directed spectrum. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 113(45), 12868–12873.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
Knill, D. C., & Pouget, A. (2004). The Bayesian brain: the role of uncertainty in neural coding and computation. Trends in Neurosciences, 27(12), 712–719.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Krynski, T. R., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2007). The role of causality in judgment under uncertainty. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136(3), 430.
Article
Google Scholar
Lawson, R. P., Mathys, C., & Rees, G. (2017). Adults with autism overestimate the volatility of the sensory environment. Nature Neuroscience, 20(9), 1293–1299.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
Lee, M. D., & Newell, B. R. (2011). Using hierarchical Bayesian methods to examine the tools of decision-making. Judgement and Decision Making, 6(8), 832–842.
Google Scholar
Levin, I. P., Schneider, S. L., & Gaeth, G. J. (1998). All frames are not created equal: A typology and critical analysis of framing effects. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 76(2), 149–188.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Lieder, F., Griffiths, T. L., & Hsu, M. (2018a). Overrepresentation of extreme events in decision making reflects rational use of cognitive resources. Psychological Review, 125(1), 1–32.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Lieder, F., Griffiths, T. L., Huys, M., & Q. J., and Goodman, N. D. (2018b). Empirical evidence for resource-rational anchoring and adjustment. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(2), 775–784.
Article
Google Scholar
Mandel, D. R. (2014). Do framing effects reveal irrational choice? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(3), 1185.
Article
Google Scholar
Manohar, S., Lockwood, P., Drew, D., Fallon, S. J., Chong, T. T.-J., Jeyaretna, D. S., … Husain, M. (2021). Reduced decision bias and more rational decision making following ventromedial prefrontal cortex damage. Cortex, 138, 24–37.
Miller, J. B., & Sanjurjo, A. (2018). Surprised by the hot hand fallacy? A truth in the law of small numbers. Econometrica: Journal of the Econometric Society, 86(6), 2019–2047. https://doi.org/10.3982/ECTA14943
Article
Google Scholar
Moore, F. R., Filippou, D., & Perrett, D. I. (2011). Intelligence and attractiveness in the face: Beyond the attractiveness halo effect. Journal of Evolutionary Psychology, 9(3), 205–217. https://doi.org/10.1556/JEP.9.2011.3.2
Article
Google Scholar
Nestler, S., Blank, H., & von Collani, G. (2008). Hindsight bias doesn’t always come easy: Causal models, cognitive effort, and creeping determinism. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34(5), 1043.
PubMed
Google Scholar
Pezzulo, G., Rigoli, F., & Friston, K. J. (2018). Hierarchical active inference: A theory of motivated control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(4), 294–306. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2018.01.009
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Piray, P., & Daw, N. D. (2020). A simple model for learning in volatile environments. PLoS Computational Biology, 16(7), e1007963. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1007963
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Polanía, R., Woodford, M., & Ruff, C. C. (2019). Efficient coding of subjective value. Nature Neuroscience, 22(1), 134–142. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-018-0292-0
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Powers, A. R., Mathys, C., & Corlett, P. R. (2017). Pavlovian conditioning-induced hallucinations result from overweighting of perceptual priors. Science, 357(6351), 596–600. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aan3458
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Qiu, C., Luu, L., & Stocker, A. A. (2020). Benefits of commitment in hierarchical inference. Psychological Review, 127(4), 622–639. https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000193
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Raoelison, M., & De Neys, W. (2019). Do we de-bias ourselves?: The impact of repeated presentation on the bat-and-ball problem. Judgment and Decision making, 14(2), 170.
Google Scholar
Reed, E. J., Uddenberg, S., Suthaharan, P., Mathys, C. D., Taylor, J. R., Groman, S. M., & Corlett, P. R. (2020). Paranoia as a deficit in non-social belief updating. ELife, 9. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.56345
Ritzwoller, D. M., & Romano, J. P. (2022). Uncertainty in the hot hand fallacy: Detecting streaky alternatives to random Bernoulli sequences. The Review of Economic Studies, 89(2), 976–1007.
Article
Google Scholar
Rozenkrantz, L., D’Mello, A. M., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2021). Enhanced rationality in autism spectrum disorder. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 25(8), 685–696.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Sanborn, A. N., & Chater, N. (2016). Bayesian brains without probabilities. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(12), 883–893.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Schmidt, A. T. (2017). The power to nudge. American Political Science Review, 111(2), 404–417.
Article
Google Scholar
Schustek, P., Hyafil, A., & Moreno-Bote, R. (2019). Human confidence judgments reflect reliability-based hierarchical integration of contextual information. Nature Communications, 10(1), 5430. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-019-13472-z
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1983). Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: informative and directive functions of affective states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45(3), 513.
Article
Google Scholar
Schwartz, A. (2015). Regulating for rationality. Stanford Law Review, 67, 1373.
Google Scholar
Sher, S., & McKenzie, C. R. M. (2006). Information leakage from logically equivalent frames. Cognition, 101, 467–494.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Siegel, J. Z., Mathys, C., Rutledge, R. B., & Crockett, M. J. (2018). Beliefs about bad people are volatile. Nature Human Behaviour, 2(10), 750–756.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Simmons, J. P., LeBoeuf, R. A., & Nelson, L. D. (2010). The effect of accuracy motivation on anchoring and adjustment: Do people adjust from provided anchors? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(6), 917.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Simon, H. A. (1979). Rational decision making in business organizations. The American Economic Review, 69(4), 493–513.
Google Scholar
Slovic, P., Finucane, M. L., Peters, E., & MacGregor, D. G. (2007). The affect heuristic. European Journal of Operational Research, 177(3), 1333–1352. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejor.2005.04.006
Article
Google Scholar
Smith, R., Thayer, J. F., Khalsa, S. S., & Lane, R. D. (2017). The hierarchical basis of neurovisceral integration. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 75, 274–296. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.02.003
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Strack, F., & Mussweiler, T. (1997). Explaining the enigmatic anchoring effect: Mechanisms of selective accessibility. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(3), 437–446.
Article
Google Scholar
Summerfield, C., & Tsetsos, K. (2012). Building Bridges between Perceptual and Economic Decision-Making: Neural and Computational Mechanisms. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 6, 70. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2012.00070
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Tenenbaum, J. B., Kemp, C., Griffiths, T. L., & Goodman, N. D. (2011). How to grow a mind: statistics, structure, and abstraction. Science, 331(6022), 1279–1285. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1192788
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Thaler, R. H. (2018a). Nudge, not sludge. Science, 361(6401), 431–431.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Thaler, R. H. (2018b). From cashews to nudges: The evolution of behavioral economics. American Economic Review, 108(6), 1265–1287.
Article
Google Scholar
Thorndike, E. L. (1920). A constant error in psychological ratings. Journal of Applied Psychology, 4(1), 25.
Article
Google Scholar
Todd, P. M., & Gigerenzer, G. E. (2012). Ecological rationality: Intelligence in the world. Oxford University Press.
Book
Google Scholar
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases: Biases in judgments reveal some heuristics of thinking under uncertainty. Science, 185(4157), 1124–1131.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1980). Causal schemas in judgments under uncertainty. In M. Fishbein (Ed.), Progress in Social Psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 49–72). Lawrence Erlbaum.
Google Scholar
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981a). Evidential impact of base rates. Stanford University.
Book
Google Scholar
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981b). The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science, 211(4481), 453–458.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1983). Extensional versus intuitive reasoning: The conjunction fallacy in probability judgment. Psychological Review, 90(4), 293.
Article
Google Scholar
van Ravenzwaaij, D., Moore, C. P., Lee, M. D., & Newell, B. R. (2014). A hierarchical Bayesian modeling approach to searching and stopping in multi-attribute judgment. Cognitive Science, 38(7), 1384–1405. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12119
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Wade, T. J., & DiMaria, C. (2003). Weight halo effects: Individual differences in perceived life success as a function of women’s race and weight. Sex Roles, 48(9), 461–465.
Article
Google Scholar
Williams, D. (2020). Epistemic irrationality in the Bayesian brain. The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science, 72(4), 913–938.
Article
Google Scholar
Wilson, T. D., Houston, C. E., Etling, K. M., & Brekke, N. (1996). A new look at anchoring effects: basic anchoring and its antecedents. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 125(4), 387.
Article
Google Scholar