Introduction

Deficits in emotion regulation are thought to play an important role in the onset and maintenance of emotional disorders such as depression and anxiety (Jackson, Malmstadt, Larson, & Davidson, 2000). The effects of different types of emotion regulation have been examined extensively. These can include acceptance-based strategies (i.e., experience one’s emotions fully), emotion suppression (i.e., suppress emotional thoughts), and distraction (Bryant, Moulds, & Guthrie, 2001; Campbell-Sills, Barlow, Brown, & Hofmann, 2006a, 2006b; Hayes et al., 2004; Kashdan, Barrios, Forsyth, & Steger, 2006). A popular approach to emotion regulation suggests that regulation strategies can be categorized into antecedent-focused strategies (e.g., cognitive reappraisal) and response-focused strategies (e.g., expressive suppression) (e.g., Gross, 1998b). Previous research indicates that using reappraisal or suppression can lead to markedly different consequences. For instance, previous research has shown that reappraisal is associated with positive emotional experiences, such as increased life satisfaction (Kashdan et al., 2006), lower symptoms of depression, higher levels of environmental mastery, personal growth, self-acceptance, and a clearer purpose in life (Garnefski, Teerds, Kraaij, Legerstee, & van den Kommer, 2004; Gross & John, 2003). Conversely, suppression has been associated with negative emotional experiences, such as higher levels of depressive symptoms and lower self-esteem (Gross & John, 2003), increases in physiology, including increased eye blinking and sympathetic nervous system activation (Gross & Levenson, 1993, 1997), and increased distress and negative affect (Ehring, Tuschen-Caffier, Schnulle, Fischer, & Gross, 2010; Kashdan et al., 2006).

Often neglected within this literature however, is an examination of individual differences. In particular, alexithymia is a personality dimension that has a strong potential link with emotion regulation. Alexithymia is a trait in which individuals have a reduced ability to regulate, identify, and communicate feelings or emotions (Franz, Schaefer, Schneider, Sitte, & Bachor, 2004; Sifneos, 1973). Importantly, alexithymia seems to be characterized by an emotion regulation style overreliant on suppression, rather than reappraisal, strategies (e.g., Dimsdale et al., 1986; Franz et al., 2004; Grabe et al., 2004; Kessler, Kammerer, Hoffmann, & Traue, 2010; Richter et al., 2006; Swart, Kortekaas, & Aleman, 2009). For instance, Kessler et al. found a positive relationship between TAS scores and individual differences in expressive suppression. Similarly, Swart et al. found significant relationships between alexithymia scores and the Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ; Gross & John, 2003). Specifically, they found that high alexithymics were more likely to use suppression than were low alexithymics. Conversely, Swart et al. also found that high alexithymics were less likely to use reappraisal than were low alexithymics, although this effect was smaller than the effect obtained for suppression. A potential consequence of these phenomena is that individuals high and low in alexithymia might have developed different expertise in the utilization of reappraisal and suppression, leading to potential implications for the relative efficiency of different regulation strategies for high and low alexithymics. For instance, if individuals who are low in alexithymia use suppression less frequently than do high alexithymics, they should be less efficient when they attempt to use suppression, because they are less used to this type of strategy. Following Swart et al.’s results, the opposite pattern could be expected for reappraisal: Individuals with high levels of alexithymia should be comparatively inefficient when attempting to use reappraisal, because they are less used to this technique than are low alexithymics.

The goal of this study was therefore to examine whether alexithymia modulates suppression- and reappraisal-based emotion regulation. In order to examine this question, we recorded event-related brain potentials (ERPs) to emotional pictures. ERPs of emotional stimuli provide a reliable index of emotional processing and are known to be sensitive to emotion regulation (e.g., Hajcak, Moser, & Simons, 2006; Hajcak & Olvet, 2008; Moser, Most, & Simons, 2010). In addition, different subdivisions of ERPs to emotional pictures can refer to distinct subcomponents of emotional processing (for a review, see Olofsson, Nordin, Sequeira, & Polich, 2008). Specifically, early ERPs to emotional pictures (~100–300 ms), including P1/N1 and P2/N2 complexes, are often thought to reflect rapid and probably automatic mobilization of attentional resources by emotional information (Carretié, Hinojosa, Martin-Loeches, Mercado, & Tapia, 2004; Olofsson et al., 2008; Olofsson & Polich, 2007; Schupp, Cuthbert, et al., 2004; Schupp, Junghofer, Weike, & Hamm, 2003), whereas the late positive potential (LPP) to emotional pictures is sensitive to the motivational relevance of stimuli and is thought to reflect an overt, postperceptual analysis of the features of the emotional stimuli/situation in order to facilitate its processing and encoding (Cuthbert, Schupp, Bradley, Birbaumer, & Lang, 2000; Schupp, Junghofer, Weike, & Hamm, 2004). Therefore, the morphology and timing of a potential modulation of ERPs to emotion regulation by alexithymia might provide insights into the nature of attentional and cognitive processes where alexithymia and emotion regulation interact. In the present study, alexithymia was measured with the revised Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS; Taylor, Ryan, & Bagby, 1985), the TAS-20 (Bagby, Parker, & Taylor, 1994; Taylor, Bagby, & Parker, 1997), one of the most widely used measures of alexithymia (Taylor, 2000). During the experiment, participants were exposed to blocks of negative and neutral pictures. Before each block of pictures, participants were instructed to reappraise, suppress, or simply attend to each image.

Method

Participants

Fifty-one healthy students and staff from the University of Leeds (U.K.) were recruited. Participants were excluded if they were left-handed or had a neurological condition. In addition, given the potentially disturbing nature of the negative stimuli used in the study, potentially vulnerable participants were also excluded. These were defined as participants who had a history of psychiatric illness, those taking psychotropic medication, participants who have seen a psychologist/psychiatrist in the past 6 months, and participants who scored above 15 on the Beck Depression Inventory (Beck, Ward, Mendelson, Mock, & Erbaugh, 1961), since this is the threshold for possible depression in community samples (Beck & Steer, 1987). Of the 51 participants tested, 5 participants were excluded (3 participants had excessive EEG artifacts, and 2 were behavioral outliers). This left a final sample of 46 to be analyzed. Although much research on alexithymia has relied on a taxometric approach in which participants were typically chosen from low and high ends of alexithymia measures, recent research suggests that alexithymia is better characterized as a dimensional, rather than a taxometric, construct (Ferguson et al., 2009; Ferguson, Wiliams, O'Connor, Bibby, & O'Carroll, 2008; Parker, Keefer, Taylor, & Bagby, 2008). Consistent with this approach, we chose to test participants from a broad range of the TAS distribution, instead of considering only the low and high ends, and we analyzed our data using a correlational approach. However, in order to also obtain a more standard analysis of our ERP data, we also performed analyses of variance (ANOVAs) comparing individuals scoring high and low on the TAS. Therefore, we defined high-TAS and low-TAS groups on the basis of a TAS cutoff score (48) that allowed us to create two groups that had mean scores similar to those in previous research (Franz, Schaefer, & Schneider, 2003).1 Using this approach, of the 46 participants included, 28 were low TAS (10 males and 18 females; TAS-20 = 36.46), and 18 were high TAS (7 males and 11 females; TAS-20 = 56.56), with a mean age of 24.56 years (SD = 6.73). This approach using both correlational and group-based analyses allowed us to (1) follow the standard of ANOVA-based analysis used in ERP research, (2) retain the dimensional nature of alexithymia to be analyzed via correlation, and (3) ground our operationalization of alexithymia in previous research (Franz et al., 2003). Participants were paid £20 for their participation. All participants provided informed consent, and the experiment was approved by the University Departmental Ethics Committee.

Stimuli

Two hundred forty color pictures were chosen from the International Affective Picture System (IAPS; Lang, Bradley, & Cuthbert, 1999), depicting 120 unpleasant, highly to moderately arousing events (e.g., mutilation, individuals held at gunpoint, images of starvation, car accidents) and 120 neutral events (e.g., household objects, neutral faces), which were medium in valence and low in arousal. In the present study, the IAPS instruction and affective ratings manual was used to assess normative ratings of the images used (Lang et al., 1999). This showed that the two categories (negative and neutral) differed significantly from each other in IAPS normative valence ratings (M = 5.0 and 2.2 for neutral and unpleasant contents, respectively) and normative arousal ratings (M = 3.2 and 6.2 for neutral and unpleasant contents, respectively).

Alexithymia

The TAS-20 (Bagby, Parker and Taylor 1994; Bagby, Taylor and Parker 1994; Taylor et al., 1992) was used to measure the construct alexithymia. Participants were asked to rate each of the 20 statements (e.g., I am able to describe my feelings easily) on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). The internal consistency of the scale is well established (Taylor et al., 1992). Research has also indicated high levels of convergent, discriminant, and concurrent validity for the scale (Bagby, Taylor, & Parker, 1994; Zech, Luminet, Rimé, & Wagner, 1999). In the present study, the scale yielded good internal consistency with an alpha of .90. In addition to overall alexithymia score, the subdimensions of alexithymia were also explored. These include difficulty identifying feelings (DIF), difficulty describing feelings (DDF) and externally oriented thinking (EOT). The DIF is used to measure individual differences in perceived difficulty in identifying emotions, the DDF is used to measure the perceived difficulty in describing emotions, and the EOT is used to measure the tendency of individuals to focus their attention externally (Bagby, Parker, & Taylor, 1994; Taylor et al., 1997). Previous research suggests that these subscales are not always highly intercorrelated and may reflect distinct facets of alexithymia (see Gohm & Clore, 2000; Loas et al., 2001).

Procedure and design

The experiment consisted of 9 practice and 240 experimental trials divided into three blocks (attend, reappraisal, and suppression). The 240 experimental trials consisted of 120 negative and 120 neutral images from the IAPS (Lang et al., 1999). In each block, there were 80 trials, each with 40 negative and 40 neutral images randomly intermixed. Each block was assigned to a specific regulation strategy (attend, reappraise, or suppress). The order of the reappraise, attend, and suppress blocks was fully counterbalanced across participants. Within each block, there were two 1-min breaks, one halfway through and one at the end.

Participants were initially given written instructions about each regulation task before the experiment (adapted from Gross, 1998a). Next, the researcher discussed the details of each regulation strategy with the participant to ensure full understanding of the task. Moreover, specific regulation instructions were shown again on a screen before each block of trials. For attend blocks, participants were simply asked to focus on the images carefully. In the reappraisal condition, however, participants received the following instructions:

Firstly you will see a cross, please focus on this cross and wait for a picture to appear. It is important that you focus on the picture carefully. Please try and adopt a detached, unemotional and neutral attitude as you view the image. In other words, as you view the image, try to think about what you are seeing objectively, in terms of the technical aspects of the events you observe. Please try to think about what you are seeing in such a way that you don't feel anything at all.

Example 1: If you see an image of an injured individual, try and think about the image in a distant and detached fashion, or even try and imagine the situation improving. In such an instance you may even adopt a medical perspective where you would objectively and analytically evaluate the image in a more problem focused manner. In other words, try not to think about the image in a personally-relevant, emotional way.

Example 2: If you see an image where someone is being threatened by a gun or knife, again try and imagine the situation in a distant and detached fashion. You may even imagine the image is a scene from a film, where the individuals are just actors, and therefore no one is in any real danger.

In the suppression condition, participants received the following instructions:

Firstly you will see a cross, please focus on this cross and wait for a picture to appear. It is important that you focus on the picture carefully. If you have any feelings while focusing on the image, please try your best not to let these feelings show. That is try to behave in such a way that a person watching you would not know you were feeling anything.

On each trial, participants were initially presented with a black fixation cross at the center of the screen for 2,000 ms. IAPS pictures were then displayed for 8,000 ms; the order of these pictures was fully randomized within each block. After each picture, participants were shown a screen containing a self-report measure of negative affect using a 7-point scale (1 = weak negative affect and 7 = strong negative affect). Participants responded to the scale using the keyboard.

Electrophysiological recording, data reduction, and analysis

EEG was recorded with a 128-channel net connected to a high-input amplifier (Electrical Geodesics, Inc., Eugene, OR) at a rate of 500 Hz (0.01- to 100-Hz bandwidth) and with an impedance of ≤20 kΩ. EEG data were recorded using a Cz reference and were digitally converted to an average mastoids reference. EEG data were analyzed using the ERP module of BESA 5.1 (MEGIS Software GmbH, Gräfelfing, Germany). Following inspection of raw data, bad channels (<5%) were replaced using a spherical spline interpolation method implemented in BESA. Data was filtered offline (0.1–40 Hz), corrected for eye movements (Berg & Scherg, 1994), segmented into epochs between 200 ms before (baseline) and 1,500 ms after stimulus onset, and baseline corrected. For each channel, epochs with a difference between the maximum and minimum voltage amplitude of >120 μV and a maximum difference between two adjacent voltage points of >75 μV were rejected (after eye movement artifact correction).

In line with past ERP research using visual emotional stimuli, data analysis focused on anterior, central, and posterior sites, since these are the main sites where emotion effects occur (Herrmann et al., 2008; Junghofer, Bradley, Elbert, & Lang, 2001; Pastor et al., 2008; Schupp et al., 2000; Schupp, Junghofer, et al., 2004). Electrode data were averaged according to six scalp regions in which data from specific sets of electrodes were pooled together: left anterior, right anterior, left central, right central, left posterior, and right posterior. The left anterior clusters included EGI electrodes 13, 20, 24, 27, 28, and 29; the right anterior clusters included 111, 112, 117, 118, 123, and 124; the left central clusters included 30, 35, 36, 37, 42, and 53; the right central clusters included 80, 86, 87, 93, and 104; the left posterior clusters included 58, 59, 60, 65, 66, and 67; and the right posterior clusters included 84, 85, 91, 92, 97, and 98. Following a careful visual inspection of our waveforms and a review of the literature on early ERP correlates of emotion (Carretié et al., 2004; Olofsson et al., 2008; Olofsson & Polich, 2007; Schupp, Junghofer, et al., 2004), mean ERP amplitudes were extracted from four latency windows: 60–100 ms (P1), 90–130 ms (N1), 170–220 ms (P2), and 220–280 ms (N2). Additionally, the LPP was analyzed in consecutive 200-ms time bins from 400 up to 1,400 ms. ERP waveforms locked to the onset of emotional pictures were created for each of the six experimental conditions: negative–suppression, negative–reappraisal, negative–attend, neutral–suppression, neutral–reappraisal, and neutral–attend.

In order to examine potential relationships between emotion regulation and alexithymia, we first computed a difference score between ERPs to negative and ERPs to neutral pictures in order to obtain an index of the magnitude of emotion-related brain activity. Computing this score was motivated by the fact that the difference between ERPs to emotional and neutral stimuli is widely seen as a reliable index of emotion-related neural activity (e.g., Olofsson et al., 2008) and by the fact that our analyses showed that this difference was, overall, significant in our data (see the Results section). We next computed Pearson correlations testing the relationship between TAS scores and the difference score separately for each regulation condition, each time window, and each electrode cluster. In order to conform to standard ERP statistical analyses, we also computed a 3 (anterior–posterior: frontal vs. central vs. posterior) × 2 (hemisphere: right vs. left) × 2 (image: negative vs. neutral) × 3 (regulation: attend vs. reappraisal vs. suppression) × 2 (TAS: high vs. low) mixed ANOVA for each time window. If differences in TAS scores modulate emotional processing differently according to reappraisal and suppression, negative versus neutral contrasts should have different magnitudes for high and low TAS, depending on whether suppression or reappraisal is being employed. Therefore, we predicted a regulation × image × TAS interaction. If this interaction was found in any time window, subsidiary analyses focused on TAS × image interactions separately for each level of regulation and, next, on negative versus neutral contrasts for each TAS group if the subsidiary interaction was significant. Anterior–posterior, hemisphere, regulation, and image were the within-subjects factors, and TAS the between-subjects factor. Greenhouse–Geisser corrections were applied where relevant. For the self-report data, a 2 (image: negative vs. neutral) × 3 (regulation: attend vs. reappraisal vs. suppression) × 2 (TAS: high vs. low) mixed ANOVA was conducted, with image and regulation as the within-subjects factors and TAS as the between-subjects factor.

Results

Self-report data

The mixed ANOVA computed on self-reported negative affect revealed a main effect of image, F(1, 45) = 195.22, p < .001, with greater levels of negative affect elicited by negative images, as was expected. A regulation × image interaction,F(1, 45) = 34.86, p < .001, was also found, reflecting a somewhat reduced effect of image in the reappraisal condition, as compared with the suppression and attend conditions, F(1, 45) = 116.63, 176.1, 213.2, respectively, all ps < .001, consistent with previous research (Gross, 1998a). Similar to previous studies (Newton & Contrada, 1994; Papciack, Feuerstein, & Siegel, 1985; Rabavilas, 1987), no reliable differences in self-reported emotional feelings were found between low- and high-TAS individuals (c.f., Table 1). However, an unexpected correlation between TAS scores and differences between self-report scores for emotional and neutral images was found for reappraisal, r = ˗.39, p < .01. No significant correlations were found for the suppression or attend conditions.

Table 1 Mean negative affect ratings for the three regulation tasks in those low and high in TAS (with standard deviations in parentheses)

ERP data

A visual inspection of our data revealed a typical differentiation between negative and neutral image conditions in both early and late time windows, replicating previous research (Olofsson et al., 2008). A closer inspection indicates that this pattern varies between high and low TAS during the suppression condition, in which the differentiation between negative and neutral images is smaller in high-than in low-TAS individuals. This effect seems to be particularly robust in the P2/N2 complex. Both the correlational and ANOVA analyses,explained in more detail below, confirmed these observations.

Correlation analysis

In order to obtain an index of emotional processing, we computed difference scores between ERPs to negative and neutral images for each regulation condition (attend, reappraisal, and suppression), time window, and scalp region (frontal, central, and posterior). Computing difference scores was possible because the differentiation between negative and neutral waveforms was, overall, significant (see Fig. 1 and ANOVAs below). Each difference score was then correlated with TAS scores. Since we did not find major regional differences (with one exception noted below), we averaged all regional clusters into a whole-scalp cluster. As is shown in Table 2, significant negative correlations between TAS and ERP difference scores were found in both early and late time windows—specifically, during the suppression condition. Although no significant correlations with reappraisal were observed using whole-scalp clusters, we found a positive correlation between TAS and image difference scores in the posterior region during reappraisal in the time window of 1,000–1,200 ms, r = .30, p < .05.

Fig. 1
figure 1

ERPs for negative and neutral stimuli by regulation strategy and by TAS group. Waveforms are an average of electrodes in left–right frontal, central, and posterior clusters. Amplitude in microvolts (μV) is on the y axis, and time is on the x axis

Table 2 Pearson correlation coefficients testing the relationship between TAS scores and ERP difference scores (negative minus neutral) separately for each regulation condition in each time window

In order to examine the relative contribution of the TAS subscales to the observed main correlations between TAS and suppression difference scores, we ran correlations between the three subscales (DIF, DDF, and EOT) and the ERP difference scores in the suppression condition, using whole-scalp clusters. As is shown in Table 3, the relationship between TAS scores and emotion difference scores in the suppression condition seems to be driven mostly by the DIF subscale.

Table 3 Pearson correlation coefficients testing the relationship between DIF, DDF, or EOT scores and the ERP difference scores (negative minus neutral) for the suppression condition

In order to explore this question further, we tested whether the correlations between the ERP difference scores and DIF were more negative than the correlations between ERP difference scores and DDF and EOT, using the procedure recommended by Meng, Rosenthal, and Rubin (1992). Only time windows where the correlations with DIF were significant were considered. With the exception of EOT in P1 and DDF in N1, the correlations between ERP difference scores and DIF were significantly more negative than the correlations involving DDF and EOT, using a one-tailed significance criterion. The two-tailed significance criterion was exceeded for all comparisons in the N2 time window and in the difference between DIF and EOT in the 400-600 ms time window.

Overall, these results indicate a negative relationship between alexithymia scores and the ERP differentiation between emotional and neutral pictures during suppression, but not during reappraisal or attend conditions. In addition, these results indicate that this pattern of findings is driven by the DIF subscale of the TAS-20.

ANOVAs

As was explained in the Method section, we ran separate TAS × regulation × image × anterior–posterior × hemisphere ANOVAs for each time window. No reliable effects were found for the P1/N1 complex; therefore, we describe below only effects starting at the P2 time window (see Fig. 2).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Scalp maps of mean amplitude differences between negative and neutral conditions plotted for the P2 and N2 components and the 400- to 600-ms time window in those low in TAS during the suppression condition. Maxima and minima are specific to each scalp map

For the P2 component, we found a significant main effect of image, F(1, 44) = 16.43, p < .001, with negative images showing, overall, a greater positivity than did neutral images. An anterior–posterior × image interaction was also found, F(2, 88) = 5.07, p < .05, reflecting a larger effect of image in frontal and central than in posterior sites, F(1, 44) = 19.83, 16.92, 4.80, p <.001, p <.001, p < .05, respectively. More important, a significant regulation × image × TAS interaction was found, F(2, 88) = 5.32, p < .01. Subsidiary analyses indicated that there was a significant TAS × image interaction in the suppression condition only, F(1, 44) = 9.24, p < .01, reflecting an effect of image significant only for low-TAS individuals, F(1, 27) = 14.83, p < .001. This finding indicates that P2 amplitudes during negative, as compared with neutral, image viewing were more positive during the suppression condition exclusively for low-TAS individuals.

For the N2 component, a significant main effect of image was found, F(1, 44) = 37.72, p < .001, indicating that negative images had an overall greater positivity than did neutral images. An anterior–posterior × image interaction was also found, F(2, 88) = 5.96, p < .01, probably reflecting a larger effect of image in frontal and central than in posterior sites, F(1, 44) = 36.50, 42.26, 19.10, respectively, all ps < .001. Similar to P2 findings, we also found a significant regulation × image × TAS interaction, F(2, 88) = 4.54, p < .05. Subsidiary analyses indicated that there was a significant TAS × image interaction in the suppression condition only, F(1, 44) = 7.11, p < .01, driven by a significant image effect in low-TAS individuals, F(1, 27) = 21.20, p < .001. This finding indicates that N2 amplitudes during negative, as compared with neutral, image viewing were more positive (i.e., reduced N2 negativity) during the suppression condition exclusively for low-TAS individuals.

Finally, in order to examine the LPP, we also analyzed consecutive 200 ms time windows after 400 ms (400–600, 600–800, 800–1,000, 1,000–1,200, 1,200–1,400). A robust image effect was found in all time windows (all ps < .001). There was also a robust anterior–posterior × image interaction found across all LPP time windows (all ps < .01). During the earliest of these time windows (400–600 ms), there were stronger image differences in central and posterior than in frontal sites, F(1, 44) = 114.20, 105.82, 71.44, respectively, all ps < .001. This became progressively more prominent in central than in frontal and posterior sites by 600–800 ms, F(1, 44) = 142.77, 92.10, 99.06, respectively, all ps < .001. For the rest of the LPP time windows, there were stronger image differences in frontal and central than in posterior sites [800–1,000 ms, F(1, 44) = 99.42, 125.09, 70.82, all ps < .001; 1,000–1,200 ms, F(1, 44) = 61.59, 61.18, 22.76, all ps < .001; 1,200–1,400 ms, F(1, 44) = 56.64, 43.16, 8.26, p < .001, p < .001, p < .01 (in respective order)]. Consistent with previous research, these findings indicate that the LPP is a slow-going wave that is initially maximal over centro-parietal sites and becomes increasingly more pronounced at anterior sites over time (Diedrich, Naumann, Maier, Becker, & Bartussek, 1997). A significant regulation × image × TAS interaction was found only in the earliest of these time windows (400–600 ms), F(2, 88) = 4.09, p < .05. This interaction was driven by a significant TAS × image interaction exclusive to the suppression condition, F(1, 44) = 4.36, p < .05. Subsidiary analyses revealed that the effect of image in the suppression condition was significant for both low- and high-TAS participants, although the magnitude of the effect was larger for low-TAS, F(1, 27) = 36.69, p < .001,than for high-TAS, F(1, 18) = 18.80, p < .001, individuals.

Discussion

The main finding of this study is that emotion-related ERP activity is inversely related to alexithymia scores during attempts at suppressing emotional responses. Correlational analyses indicated that this effect can be observed during both early (<300 ms) and late (>300 ms) emotion-related ERPs. ANOVAs suggested that this effect was the strongest in the P2 and N2 and at the beginning of the LPP (400–600 ms), where significant three-way TAS × regulation × image interactions were observed. These findings are consistent with the suggestion that alexithymia is linked to a greater expertise in the utilization of suppression-based regulation techniques because of their more frequent utilization by high alexithymics and less frequent utilization by low alexithymics (e.g., Franz et al., 2004; Swart et al., 2009). Finally, we did not find convincing evidence of a positive relationship between TAS scores and ERPs to emotion during reappraisal. We discuss these results in more detail below.

These findings are consistent with the notion that alexithymia determines the efficiency of suppression techniques because individuals scoring high or low in alexithymia vary regarding how frequently they utilize suppression to regulate emotions. Low alexithymics are less used to suppression and, therefore, are less likely to possess developed prior schemata to facilitate the utilization of this technique. Consequently, they are more likely to draw on central cognitive resources to perform the task. Given that response-focused regulation strategies are usually difficult and demanding in cognitive resources (Richards & Gross, 2000), low alexithymics are therefore more likely to be prone to failures in their attempts at successful utilization of suppression. In contrast, individuals scoring high in alexithymia are more frequent users of suppression and, therefore, more likely to have developed overtrained schemata of efficient suppression techniques, increasing thereby their chances of success when using suppression. This explanation is consistent with previous research indicating that alexithymia scores are linked to a more frequent utilization of suppression techniques (e.g., Dimsdale et al., 1986; Franz et al., 2004; Grabe et al., 2004; Kessler et al., 2010; Richter et al., 2006; Swart et al., 2009). This explanation is also consistent with research suggesting that long-term expertise in regulation techniques improves efficiency in controlling emotional responses (Dalgleish, Yiend, Schweizer, & Dunn, 2009; Wegner & Gold, 1995). This explanation also fits with research suggesting that suppression-based regulation is usually demanding in cognitive resources (Richards & Gross, 2000). Finally, this explanation is also consistent with theoretical models suggesting that failures of suppression lead to “ironic effects”—that is, the paradoxical facilitation of processing of the to-be-suppressed information (Wegner, 1994). This phenomenon can be caused by a depletion in cognitive resources (Ellis & Ashbrook, 1987; Eysenck, Derakshan, Santos, & Calvo, 2007), and it can generalize to paradoxical effects of suppression in emotion regulation (Dalgleish et al., 2009).

An important finding of this study is that evidence of a relationship between alexithymia and expressive suppression was found in both early and late ERP correlates of emotion. Emotion-related ERPs in late time windows (>300ms) are usually referred to as the LPP and are thought to reflect overt, postperceptual emotional appraisal processes (Cuthbert et al., 2000; Schupp, Junghofer, et al., 2004), and previous research has shown that it can be reduced by effective emotion regulation (e.g., Hajcak & Nieuwenhuis, 2006). However, the strongest effects in our study were found in early ERPs to emotion and, in particular, the P2/N2 complex. Emotion-related ERPs occurring in these time windows are thought to reflect a rapid mobilization of attention by emotionally relevant stimuli (for a review, see Olofsson et al., 2008). Furthermore, emotion-related ERPs in these time windows possess several attributes characteristic of automatic processes (Schupp et al., 2008). The present study might, therefore, suggest that alexithymia modulates the effects of suppression on early automatic processing of emotion. This explanation implies that a wilful, controlled attempt to suppress emotions can affect early automatic processes. One could argue that effects of suppression occur in later stages of the processing stream, since they rely on conscious controlled processes(Cisler & Koster, 2010; Dunning & Hajcak, 2009; Johnson, 2009), and therefore, effects occurring in the P2/N2 latencies might be too early to be influenced by suppression. However, our regulation task used a blocked design, in which participants were in a sustained cognitive mode (suppression, reappraisal, or attend) during the entire duration of a block. Therefore, a specific regulation strategy was already in place at the onset of each picture, enabling the capture of their effects in early time windows. Furthermore, previous research has shown that “ironic” effects of suppression can be indexed by early ERP components. For instance, previous research has shown that suppressing a focus word can lead to a paradoxical increase in attention toward the word during a lexical decision task and that this can be detected as early as 260 ms post-stimulus-onset (Giuliano & Wicha, 2010). Finally, previous research indicates that indices of rapid emotional processing can be influenced by ongoing cognitive tasks (King & Schaefer, 2011; Pessoa, McKenna, Gutierrez, & Ungerleider, 2002).

A possible alternative explanation could posit that an increased N2 negativity could reflect the implementation of inhibitory control in order to actively suppress negative emotions (Lewis et al., 2008). This would be consistent with results in the literature showing a link between the N2 and inhibitory control (Nieuwenhuis, Yeung, van den Wildenberg, & Ridderinkhof, 2003). However, our results do not support this explanation. If N2 amplitude is related to an increase in inhibitory control involved in attempts to suppress emotions, larger N2 negativities should be observed for negative than for neutral items during suppression. In fact, N2 negativity was reduced, rather than increased, for emotional items, as compared with neutral items, in low-TAS individuals, and no significant difference in N2 amplitude was observed for high-TAS individuals during suppression. Another related explanation could be based on the concept of cognitive efficiency (Dennis & Chen, 2007; Eysenck & Calvo, 1992; Gray et al., 2005), according to which a smaller N2 negativity could reflect a more efficient utilization of cognitive control resources. Applied to our findings, it could mean that low-TAS individuals are more efficient at suppressing emotional, rather than neutral, information. However, it is unlikely and counterintuitive to assume that suppressing responses to emotionally negative stimuli would require less effort (or fewer cognitive resources) than suppressing responses to neutral pictures, which renders this explanation implausible.

A different alternative explanation suggests that the increased effect of image in the P2/N2 window might reflect an early detection of the need to suppress emotional responses. In this case, our results would reflect a better efficiency in the rapid detection of the need to suppress for low-TAS, as compared with high-TAS, individuals. This would be consistent with previous research indicating that early ERPs to the recognition of emotional faces are delayed in high alexithymics (Vermeulen, Luminet, de Sousa, & Campanella, 2008). Although this is a potentially valid explanation, it does not fully explain our results. First, there is no a priori reason why early ERPs to emotion should be linked to the detection of the need to suppress in a specific way (rather than, for instance, the need to reappraise). If a rapid mobilization of cognitive resources to successfully suppress emotions can be adaptive, so is the need to gather resources to reappraise. This would suggest that an enhancement of early ERPs to emotion in low-TAS individuals should be observed for both regulation conditions. Yet this effect is seen only in suppression, and not in reappraisal. Second, if our results reflected a better early detection of the need to suppress in low-TAS individuals, we should expect that this would lead to a more efficient subsequent regulation. Therefore, we could predict that an enhancement of early ERPs to emotion in low-TAS individuals would be followed by a greater reduction of the LPP. However, our results show exactly the opposite pattern—that is, an inverse correlation between TAS and LPP magnitude during suppression.

A visual inspection of the waveforms suggests an apparent trend toward a positive relationship between alexithymia levels and the magnitude of ERPs to emotion during reappraisal. Yet statistical analyses did not provide convincing support for these observations. First, correlations between emotional–neutral ERP difference scores and TAS scores were nonsignificant, apart from a single positive correlation in a late time window (1,000–1,200 ms). Second, no significant TAS × image interaction was found in the reappraisal condition. However, we did find a significant regulation × image interaction for P2 specifically for low-TAS. Specific contrasts between negative and neutral ERPs were significant (p < .05) for attend and suppression, but not in the reappraisal condition. In addition, the effect size of the contrasts was larger for suppression than for attend, F = 14.83 and F = 5.58, respectively. This result is consistent with previous research (Hajcak & Nieuwenhuis, 2006) showing that reappraisal inhibits emotion-related ERPs. It also suggests that the emotion-related P2 is facilitated during suppression for low-TAS individuals, consistent with our overall results in the suppression condition. These findings in the reappraisal condition, added to the positive correlation found in the 1,000-1,200 ms window, appear to suggest that reappraisal can inhibit emotion-related ERPs for low-TAS individuals. Yet a number of important caveats need to be taken into account. First, this result is specific to the P2 component. Second, correlations between TAS and reappraisal difference scores are largely nonsignificant. Third, self-report data involving reappraisal are incongruent with ERP results in the reappraisal condition, since a negative correlation was found between TAS and self-report difference scores. A number of potential explanations are possible to account for the lack of convincing evidence of potential relationships between alexithymia and reappraisal. First, it might be possible that the effect of alexithymia on emotion regulation is mostly determined by differences in suppression, rather than reappraisal. Consistent with this explanation, Swart et al. (2009) have shown that although those high and low in alexithymia differ in how likely they are to use both suppression and reappraisal, the effect sizes of the differences between individuals scoring high and low in alexithymia were substantially larger for suppression than for reappraisal, F = 33.4 and F = 6.8, respectively. Second, a relationship between alexithymia and reappraisal might exist, but it might be necessary to work with a sample of high-TAS individuals with a TAS mean score approaching the extreme end of the distribution in order to obtain significant effects in reappraisal. Third, the apparent dissociation between self-report and physiological data is not uncommon in alexithymia (Newton & Contrada, 1994; Papciack et al., 1985; Rabavilas, 1987). Reliable self-reports of emotional responses are difficult to obtain from individuals scoring high in alexithymia, probably because emotional self-reports rely on the very abilities that are impaired in alexithymia—that is, the ability to identify and communicate feelings or emotions(Sifneos, 1973).

A number of additional questions remain open to debate. First, our findings of a correlation between TAS and suppression were not accompanied by analog results in the self-report data. This is consistent with a number of previous studies that have shown a decoupling between self-reports and physiological data in alexithymics (Newton & Contrada, 1994; Papciack et al., 1985; Rabavilas, 1987). As was mentioned previously, this phenomenon might be related to the fact that alexithymia is linked to an alteration of the abilities on which emotional self-reports are usually reliant, such as the ability to identify and communicate feelings (Franz et al., 2004; Sifneos, 1973). Further research will be needed to investigate whether interactions between alexithymia and emotion regulation can be reflected in objective behavioral correlates of emotion (e.g., the emotional startle eyeblink; Jackson et al., 2000; Lissek et al., 2007). Second, our results show that a dimensional approach to alexithymia can lead to largely similar results as a categorical approach, since our correlational approach led to conclusions that were broadly similar to the conclusions implied by the ANOVA data. However our categorical approach had a lower statistical power because it relied on obtaining significant F-tests of interactions, which is a requirement that can often mask the rejection of the null hypothesis (Rosnow & Rosenthal, 1989). Specifically, our correlational approach indicated that the relationship between TAS and emotion-related ERPs was significant in a broader range of time windows than those detected by our ANOVAs. These findings are consistent with recent evidence suggesting that alexithymia is better operationalized as a dimensional, rather than a more classical, taxometric approach (Ferguson et al., 2008; Ferguson et al., 2009; Parker et al., 2008). However, we acknowledge that the relative merits of dimensional versus taxometric approaches still need to be tested more systematically. Third, in relation to the previous point, we chose to recruit participants with a low potential for mood disorders (see the Method section), which minimizes the number of individuals in the extreme high end of the TAS distribution. As was mentioned previously, this approach was chosen in order to prevent potentially vulnerable participants from being exposed to high-arousal negative pictures. It has to be said, however, that this potential weakness should work against our hypotheses, since it minimizes the contrast between high- and low-TAS groups. Obtaining significant differences in this context suggests, therefore, that our findings are robust and do not depend on operationalizing alexithymia as a contrast between extreme ends of the alexithymia spectrum. Fourth, future research should adopt a multidimensional approach in which questionnaires such as the ERQ (Gross & John, 2003) are explored in addition to the TAS-20, alongside questionnaires assessing major personality dimensions such as neuroticism.

Finally, an important finding of our correlational analysis is that the difficulty in identifying feelings subscale seems to account for most of the effects of the present study. This finding strengthens the viewpoint that alexithymia is not necessarily a unitary construct (Coffey, Berenbaum, & Kerns, 2003; Gohm & Clore, 2000; Koven & Thomas, 2010) and that different facets of alexithymia might play specific functional roles. Previous research has divided alexithymia facets into emotional clarity (DIF + DDF) and attention to emotion (EOT), and these two dimensions have been associated to differences in cognitive processing (Coffey et al., 2003; Koven & Thomas, 2010). The present findings are partially consistent with this view with regard to the differences between DIF and EOT. However, we also observed differences between DIF and DDF, which suggests that individual differences in the difficulty in identifying feelings plays a unique role in emotion regulation above and beyond the capacity to describe and communicate any feelings. Future research will be needed to confirm these findings and explore these questions further. In particular, a future avenue of research should explore whether individual differences in the DIF facet play a unique role in emotion regulation and in cognitive efficiency.

In conclusion, the results of this study suggest that alexithymia modulates both early and late emotion-related ERPs during attempts to suppress emotions. Specifically, alexithymia scores are inversely related to the magnitude of emotion-related ERP activity during suppression. These findings argue for the need to take into account affective individual differences when investigating emotion regulation processes.