Method
Participants
Forty volunteers (31 female, nine male), naive to the paradigm and objective, participated for course credit or payment. The participants were between 19 and 30 years old (M = 22.4, SD = 3.21), had normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity and normal color vision (both tested with Oculus Binoptometer 3). Participants attested written understanding and consent before the experiment started. The experiment was conducted in accordance with the ethical standards of the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Psychology at Philipps-University Marburg.
Apparatus
Participants sat in a comfortable chair in a dimly lit and sound attenuated room. They used their index and middle fingers of both hands to press one of four buttons at the backside of a gamepad (Speedlink STRIKE Gamepad). The stimuli were presented on a LCD-IPS screen (Cambridge Research Systems, Display++ LCD Monitor 32 in., 1,920 × 1,080 pixels, 120 Hz), placed 100 cm in front of participants. A Windows 7 PC running E-Prime Professional (2.0.10.356) controlled stimulus presentation and response collection.
Stimuli and general procedure
The search displays always consisted of 16 items—14 circles (distractors) and two diamonds (targets)—sparsely distributed on an imaginary 12 × 7 grid (see Fig. 1A). All items had a diameter of 1.66° and were presented with a minimal distance of 1.66° between any two items. The two diamond targets could appear in eight target locations, four on each side of the screen, arranged semicircularly on both sides with an average distance of 14.52° (SD = 0.47) from screen center. The two target positions were selected randomly among these locations in each trial. Fourteen distractors were placed randomly on the grid. The stimuli were gray (RGB 122, 122, 122; 55.85 cd/m2) or colored on a black background (RGB 0, 0, 0; 0.16 cd/m2); color was chosen randomly for each item in each trial from a set of four colors: turquois (RGB 0, 170, 136; 55.86 cd/m2), light blue (RGB 18, 152, 197; 55.80 cd/m2), dark blue (RGB 64, 131, 237; 55.86 cd/m2), or purple (RGB 111, 111, 240; 55.84 cd/m2). A gray digit (Gill Sans MT, font height 0.46°) was placed in the center of each item. The targets contained different random digits between 1 and 4; distractors contained random digits from 5 to 8.
In each trial, the search display included one gray and one colored diamond target along with 14 circle distractors. The proportions of colored and gray distractors varied systematically across consecutive trials, from “gray plateaus” in which all distractors were presented in gray, to “color plateaus” in which all distractors were colored (cf. Fig. 1). In the gray plateaus (three successive trials), the colored target was the only colored item in the search display, whereas in color plateaus (also three successive trials), the gray target was the only gray item in the display. The plateau was followed by a transition phase of 13 trials in which the proportion of colored distractors successively increased by one (transition from gray plateau to color plateau) or decreased by one (transition from color plateau to gray plateau) in every trial. An experimental block consisted of 32 trials, starting with a plateau (three trials), followed by a transition phase (13 trials), the opposite plateau (three trials), and a transition phase back to the first plateau (13 trials; see Fig. 1B). The starting plateau was balanced across the participants.
Experimental procedure
The experiment consisted of two experimental sessions on separate days with one day in between. The first session started with two practice blocks of the free-choice visual search task (16 trials each), in which the participants were free to select either target. These practice blocks consisted of balanced displays containing a constant proportion of eight colored and eight gray items. After the practice, participants worked through 21 blocks of the free-choice task. The session ended with the Corsi block and the color Stroop task of the PEBL Test Battery. Participants then filled the follow-up survey, in which they were asked to report their strategy for selecting the target and to report any regularities they might have noticed. The second session started with the forced-choice task, which was followed by the informed-choice task. Although the instructions differed across the free-choice, informed-choice, and forced-choice task, the search display and general procedure was the same in all three tasks.
Free-choice task
The free-choice task was conducted to evaluate which target was chosen in each trial. Participants were informed that the search displays always contained two shape targets (diamonds among circles) and that they were free to choose either one. They were asked to identify the digit presented inside the chosen target and to respond by pressing the corresponding button on the gamepad (1, 2, 3, or 4). Participants were neither informed about the trial sequence, nor that one target was always colored and one always gray, nor were they given particular instructions about how to choose between the targets.
Trials started with a central fixation dot surrounded by a thin line. After 1,000 ms the thin line disappeared, and after 500 ms the search display was shown. It was presented until participants reported the chosen target, and then it was replaced by an empty black screen presented for 800 ms. After each block, performance feedback (mean response time and accuracy) was presented, followed by a pause of at least 5 s. Participants performed 672 trials of the free-choice task (21 blocks with 32 trials).
Informed-choice task
In the informed-choice task, participants were informed about the dynamically changing trial sequence and about the number of trials in the plateau and transition phase. Participants were encouraged to adapt their target choice to the changing color ratio, but they were also told that they were still free to select any target in each trial. Participants performed 224 trials (seven blocks with 32 trials) of the informed-choice task.
Forced-choice task
In the forced-choice task, participants searched for a specific target (either the gray or the colored target) throughout an entire block. This task allowed us to assess response times (RTs) for each target in the trial sequence. A textual cue indicated the relevant target at the beginning of each block. Participants performed 448 trials, split into seven blocks (with 32 trials) in each search condition.
Individual differences: Covariates
In addition to the three variations of the visual search task, we assessed the individual working memory capacity and the capability of attentional filtering, which are known to be associated with attentional control (e.g., Fukuda & Vogel, 2011; Fukuda, Woodman, & Vogel, 2015; Jost, Bryck, Vogel, & Mayr, 2011; Robison & Unsworth, 2017).
Participants performed a computerized version of the Corsi block-tapping task, to assess individual visuospatial short-term memory capacity (Corsi block span), and the color Stroop task, as a measure for the capacity to inhibit irrelevant information (RT increase in incongruent relative to congruent trials). Both tasks were taken from the PEBL Test Battery (PEBL Portable 0.14; Mueller & Piper, 2014).
Data analysis
Throughout the analyses, we used Bayesian estimation (Kruschke & Liddell, 2018) and made inferences based on parameter differences. When explicit models were applied, we report the posteriors of the parameters and differences of interest. The ranges of the 95% highest probability density (HPD) intervals are reported in square brackets after the estimates. If the 95% HPD interval of a difference does not include zero, a null effect is highly improbable.
When percentages of target choices were assessed (e.g., in the plateaus or the overall transitions), the success probabilities of binomial distributions over the trial repetitions were estimated. They are reported as percentages or differences in percentage points with their 95% HPD intervals. For comparisons of RTs, the BEST procedure (a Bayesian version of a two-sample t test, see Kruschke, 2013) was applied, and means, differences, and the 95% HPDs are reported. For investigating potential correlations, a Bayesian estimation of Pearson’s coefficient (see Ly, Verhagen, & Wagenmakers, 2016; tests were executed in JASP [JASP Team, 2018]) was conducted (and r is reported with its 95% HPDs). If not indicated otherwise, the two-sided test was used.
Target choices
Target choices were assessed in the free-choice task and compared to the choices in the informed-choice task, separately for the gray-to-color and color-to-gray transition. Trials with button presses not corresponding to a digit presented inside one of the targets were removed (2.6% in free choice, 2.8% in informed choice). To quantitatively analyze the target choices, a graphical Bayesian model was implemented, which estimates the adaptive choice (AC) tendency and PSE with an S-shaped psychometric function as visualized in Fig. 2. This function describes the probability of selecting the target whose color state was unique in the plateau and became more frequent during a transition. It can be formalized with a sigmoid based on a cumulative Gaussian distribution (cf. Wichmann & Hill, 2001):
$$ \varPsi \left(t, AC, PSE\right)=\frac{1}{2}\operatorname{erf}\left(-\frac{\sqrt{AC}\ast \left(t- PSE\right)}{\sqrt{2}}\right)+\frac{1}{2}, $$
(1)
where t is the trial in the transition, erf is the Gaussian error function, PSE is the trial at which the function crosses the .5 level, and AC the slope of the function (cf. Fig. 2), and an index of how strongly participants adapt. Formally, AC is the precision (inverse of the variance) of the underlying Gaussian distribution. The PSE is the point of subjective equality—that is, the trial in the transition at which observers select each target at the chance level of .5.
This function is at the core of the graphical Bayesian model we used to estimate the participant and mean parameters (see Fig. 3). At the inner levels, a binomial distribution models the number of trials (yjt) (out of all repetitions njt of that transition trial t) in which participant j selects a target of a particular type (i.e., the colored or gray one). The success probability θjt is calculated via the psychometric function Ψ(t, ACj, PSEj) (Eq. 1) for each trial in the transition. The overall distributions of ACμ and PSEμ are sampled as means of the individual estimates. The priors used in the evaluation are stated in Fig. 3. They have been selected to be weakly informative: The PSE priors are broad normal distributions centered at the objective point of equality (T7). Gamma distributions on the AC parameters assign most of the density to values in the range from 0 to 5, which corresponds to the spectrum from entirely flat adaptation curves to immensely steep ones that drop from 1 to 0 from one trial to the next (cf. Fig. 2).
The estimation procedure was implemented in PyMC3 (Salvatier, Wiecki, & Fonnesbeck, 2016) and 20,000 samples were drawn using NUTS (Hoffman & Gelman, 2014) after the same number of tuning iterations. For our experiments, we report the AC and PSE means over participants and assess relationships between estimates for different transitions on the participant level.
Response times
The RTs in the forced-choice task, in which participants had to select a specific target during one block, were analyzed as an index of the efficiency of finding either target. The mean RTs were assessed for each trial in the plateaus (P1–P3) and transitions (T1–T13) and were aggregated separately for trials in which participants selected the gray or the colored target. With the number of distractors sharing the target’s color state increasing or decreasing during the transition, this task is somewhat similar to a standard visual search task in which the set size increases or decreases. Therefore, the usual data pattern with RTs depending on the set size was expected. To analyze search efficiency in the forced-choice task, we estimated search slopes by fitting the following linear function to the mean RTs at the transitions trials t:
$$ \mathrm{RT}(t)={\mathrm{RT}}_{\mathrm{T}7}+\left(t-7\right)\cdotp \mathrm{slope}. $$
(2)
Subtracting the value 7 from the trial position in the transition centers the function at T7, so that RTT7 is an estimate of the RT at the center of the transition, where both color states were equally frequent. The slope parameter reflects the increase (or decrease, for negative values) in difficulty in milliseconds per item.
We fitted this function with a Bayesian graphical model, similar to the one depicted in Fig. 3, so that RTT7j and slopej were estimated at the participant level and fed into the overall estimates RTT7μ and slopeμ. The priors were set up as follows: \( {\mathrm{RT}}_{\mathrm{T}{7}_j},\mathrm{Normal}\left(\mu =\mathrm{1,000}, SD=300\right) \); slopej, Normal(μ = 0, SD = 40); common standard deviation of all RT(t),\( {SD}_{{\mathrm{RT}}_j},\mathrm{Normal}\left(\mu =0, SD=500\right) \).
Results
Target choices
Free-choice and informed-choice tasks
Overall, the target choice results revealed that participants adapted their choice to the trial sequence in free choice task (Fig. 4). However, adaptive choice behavior was more pronounced in the informed-choice task than in the free-choice task.
In the plateaus in the free-choice task (Fig. 4, upper panels), participants showed a tendency to select the target in a unique color state. These targets were selected with a probability of 57% [55% to 59%] (colored target in the gray plateau) and 53% [51% to 55%] (gray target in the color plateau). Participants then adapted their target choices to the changing color ratio to some extent, although in a relatively weak form, with selection frequencies barely reaching 60% close to the plateaus (however, note that this is a range similar to that in Irons & Leber, 2016). In the informed-choice task, in which participants had been informed about the regularities and were encouraged to make use of them, color targets in the gray plateaus were selected with a probability of 83% [81% to 85%], and gray targets in the color plateaus with a probability of 82% [80% to 85%] (Fig. 4, lower panels). Participants thus selected the target whose color state was more unique in the display, a tendency that was strongest close to the plateaus and weaker toward the center of the transition, where presumably estimating the color ratio became more difficult.
The aggregated adaptation curves in Fig. 4 provide a coarse summary of the overall observer behavior. However, as Irons and Leber (2016) showed, there can be large individual differences in adaptive choice behavior. To deal with the greater noise on the individual data level, we applied the differentiated observer model described in the introduction. The posterior distributions of the mean AC and PSE estimates are visualized in Fig. 5.
The estimated mean AC parameters confirmed the presence of adaptation to the color state ratio in the free-choice task (see Fig. 5A). For both the gray-to-color and color-to-gray transitions, AC was estimated at .005 (and the difference between gray-to-color and color-to-gray was only .000019 [– .001 to .003]. The participant-level plots in Fig. 5C, which depict predicted adaptation curves and AC estimates, provide some intuition about the typical range of these values. To demonstrate that the estimated adaptation did not automatically result from the prior distributions and the model structure, we fitted simulated null-model data as a baseline (see the gray “Null-Sim” distribution in Fig. 5A). The null data were generated with a .5 probability of selecting either target (i.e., the equivalent to the flat line in Fig. 2). The difference between the baseline distributions estimated from the null data was .002 (with the HPD interval [.001 to .003] excluding “no difference” zero).
For the informed-choice task, AC was estimated at .32 [.25 to .41] for gray to color and .26 [.21 to .34] for color to gray. The difference between the two was .06 [–0.041 to 0.17], but zero was still within the HPD interval [– .41 to .01]. The AC estimates were substantially larger in the informed-choice than in the free-choice task. The lower 95% HPD boundaries on the differences were more than .2 above zero.
We additionally calculated correlations of the AC estimates and the Corsi and Stroop scores that were reported to relate to attentional control. We found no correlation for Stroop incongruence costs with AC estimates, in either the free-choice task or the informed-choice task. The Corsi block span, however, correlated with the individual AC estimates, but only in the gray-to-color transition in the free-choice task (r = .43 [.135 to .641], with a Bayes factor of 16.19 for the alternative hypothesis that the correlation is positive; one-sided test; see Fig. 6). Given that similar correlations were absent in the other task, the other transition direction, and all tasks and transition directions in Experiment 2, the presence of a correlation in this case should probably not be overinterpreted.
The PSE parameter models the subjective point at which selection was equally affected by both color states, with observers deciding for each target with a probability of 50%. As Fig. 5B shows, for the free-choice task, the PSE was slightly later than T7, which marks the transition trial in which both color states were represented by the same number of distractors. For gray-to-color transitions, the PSE was located at 7.6 [7.2 to 7.9], and for color-to-gray, at 7.5 [7.2 to 7.9]. These estimates differed only marginally, by 0.24 [– 0.07 to 0.55], with zero lying inside the HPD interval. Only the PSE for gray-to-color differed substantially from the estimate from the simulated null data. For color-to-gray transitions, “no-difference” zero was still in the HPD interval (estimated as 0.48 [–0.06 to 1.09]), which was due to the broad distributions reflecting the uncertainty in the estimates.
In the informed-choice task, the PSE for gray-to-color transitions was estimated at 7.8 [7.6 to 8], and the one for color to gray at 8 [7.8 to 8.2]. The PSE of color to gray seemed to occur relatively late in this task. However, for the difference of 0.24, “no-difference” zero fell just inside the HPD interval [– 0.07 to 0.55]. Both PSE estimates differed substantially from the simulated null data.
Response times
Forced-choice task
RTs in the forced-choice task were considered as a measure of search difficulty when searching for a single and defined target (i.e., either the colored or the gray target). The forced-choice RTs are depicted in Fig. 7A.
The analysis showed that search difficulty for either target depended on the number of distractors in similar color states in the display. Overall, performance was very accurate: An incorrect target was reported in 2.1% of trials, and a number not contained in the display in 1.8%. For statistical comparisons, the mean RTs in correct trials were pooled separately for transitions and plateaus (i.e., all data points within in a transition range, or plateau, were considered as independent samples). The fastest responses were observed for targets whose color state was unique in a plateau—that is, when searching for the colored target in the gray plateau (1,027 ms [1,000 to 1,053]) and for the gray target in the colored plateau (1,118 ms [1,090 to 1,148]). Responses were much slower when participants searched for the gray target in the gray plateau (difference from “color target in gray plateau”: 175 ms [134 to 217]) or the color target in the color plateau (difference from “gray target in color plateau”: 217 ms [169 to 265]); in both cases, zero, “no difference,” is far outside the HPDs.
To quantify the impact of the changing color ratio on the efficiency of selecting either target, we estimated the slopes of the RTs and intercepts at T7. The search slopes and RTs at T7 during transition in forced choice are reported in Table 1. RTs were higher for colored than for gray targets (by 64 ms [42 to 86] in color-to-gray, and by 64 ms [43 to 86] in gray-to-color, transitions). All slopes showed the expected pattern: When the number of items that shared the target’s color state became smaller, slopes were negative, reflecting the facilitation from having to inspect fewer items. When the number of items that shared the target’s color state increased, slopes were positive, indicating increasing search difficulty.
Table 1 Forced-choice task: Estimated means of the response times (RTs) at T7 and the search slopes for different targets and transitions in Experiment 1
Slopes in color-to-gray transitions showed that when coming from a colored plateau, the facilitation for a colored target by removing colored distractors was much stronger than the negative impact of adding gray distractors was for the gray target (the difference was 6.34 ms/item [2 to 10.8]; in gray to color, the difference was only 0.25 ms [−3.81 to 4.4]).
Figure 7A shows that the RT pattern for the gray and colored targets intersect far from the center of the transition. Estimating these intersection points on the basis of the RTT7 and slope estimates reported in Table 1 puts the intersection from gray to color at T3 (estimated at 2.9 [− 0.78 to − 4.8]), and the one from color to gray at T10 (estimated at 9.5 [8.5 to 10.5]). Keeping in mind that the gray-to-color transition began with a gray plateau and the color-to-gray transition led into one, this result shows that the intersections were shifted by more than two trials toward the gray plateau.
Free-choice and informed-choice tasks
RTs in the free-choice and informed-choice tasks are depicted in Fig. 7B and C. Since these RTs were not of primary interest, they are shown for completeness but are not analyzed in detail. The overall pattern shows that in the free-choice task, RTs did not vary much over the trial sequence, whereas in the informed-choice task, they were more variable and increased toward the center of the transition. This RT increase could hint that target choice was most difficult at the center of the transition, likely because the color ratio was hard to estimate.
Discussion
The results of Experiment 1 showed that observers were tuning their attentional control settings toward the changing color state ratio even though color was not a target-defining dimension. Participants selected the colored target most frequently when it was unique, in the gray plateau, and the gray target when it was unique, in the color plateau. The AC estimates in the free-choice task showed that during the transition, participants shifted their target preference to the other target. PSE estimates further showed that the point at which participants became more likely to prefer the other target was near or shortly after the center of the transition. However, the results also revealed asymmetries in the AC behavior and the PSEs. Participants were better in adapting in the gray-to-color transition, in which they started by predominantly selecting the color target, than in the color-to-gray transition, in which they started by predominantly selecting the gray target. This was indicated by a later PSE and a smaller AC estimate in the color-to-gray transition. Although the HPD intervals suggested that these differences were rather uncertain, the color target seemed more likely to lead observers into target choice adaptation than the gray one.
To our surprise, we found no correlations of the AC estimates with known covariates of attentional control, except for the gray-to-color transition in the free-choice task. In this transition, items of the same color state but heterogeneous in hue were added across trials. The heterogeneity in hue might have increased working memory demands, and capacity limitations thus might have had an impact on adaptive choice behavior. If so, this finding suggests that observers can adapt their choices with little effort when items of homogeneous hue (as in the gray color state) were added across trials, while adding items of heterogeneous hue hindered adaptation.
This consideration receives support from the RT pattern observed in the forced-choice task: RTs also showed asymmetries between gray-to-color and color-to-gray transitions, as evidenced by differences in the absolute slopes and intersection points of search for gray and colored targets (cf. Fig. 7A). This suggests that hue heterogeneity might not have influenced only target choices but also RTs when searching for either target.