Klein and Lawrence (2011, 2012) developed a taxonomy of attention in which resource allocation in the domains of space, time, and activity can be controlled exogenously or endogenously. One method for exploring the endogenous control of attention in time is the temporal cueing paradigm pioneered by Kingstone (1992). This is a simple, novel, and fruitful method used to explore the control of temporal attention, whereby alphabetic cues are employed to inform participants of various experimental contingencies so that their attention could be modified appropriately. Kingstone modeled this methodology on the Posner cueing paradigm (Posner, Snyder, & Davidson, 1980) for exploring endogenously generated shifts of visuospatial attention. In his fourth experiment, Kingstone had observers engage in a two-alternative speeded-choice task in which targets for response were preceded by alphabetic temporal cues that indicated the likely time of target onset; after either a short or long stimulus-onset asynchrony (SOA). The cues provided accurate temporal information 80% of the time. For targets presented at the early SOA, Kingstone found a reaction time (RT) advantage following valid (relative to invalid) temporal cues, an effect that has been dubbed a temporal cueing effect (TCE). Within this paradigm, data from the shorter SOA provide the temporal cueing effect of interest because, as noted by Kingstone (this is a paraphrase), “performance at the long SOA is highly contaminated by the fact that subjects know by default that if a target stimulus does not appear at the short SOA, then it must appear at the long one” (1992, pp. 93–94). Thus, when the target is expected at the short SOA but is “invalidly” presented at the long SOA, participants can adjust or “reorient” their temporal expectancy once the short SOA elapses without presentation of the target.

It is important to highlight Kingstone’s methodological and empirical contributions to the field of temporal attention through his invention of this paradigm, that we will refer to as “Kingstonian,” because his contributions are often ignored. For example, in their highly cited neuroimaging study of spatial and temporal expectancies, Coull and Nobre (1998) minify Kingstone’s paradigmatic contribution, saying in their introduction, “By contrast, there has been little or no research investigating attentional orienting to a particular point in time (but see Kingstone, 1992)” (p. 7426). After presenting their interesting findings, they say,

Although the benefits of spatial cueing are well established, this is the first demonstration that such benefits also can manifest themselves when attention is paid to moments in time. One exception is the study by Kingstone (1992), which investigated the interaction between attending to form and attending to temporal interval but did not investigate temporal cueing per se. (p. 7431)

Also, in a recent paper discussing the relation between temporal attention (as explored using the temporal cueing paradigm developed by Kingstone) and alerting (as explored in warning signal experiments), Weinbach and Henik, (2012) fail to cite Kingstone (1992), opting instead to cite Coull and Nobre (1998) and a review paper by Correa (2010). The Correa paper, which was published in Nobre and Coull’s (2010) book Attention and Time, also neglects to cite Kingstone.Footnote 1

Whereas endogenous control is top down and volitional, exogenous control is bottom up and reflexive (Jonides, 1981). This is an issue within the current literature, as pointed out by Weinbach and Henik (2012, 2013), as these are entangled in the methodological design of many temporal cueing paradigms. In accord with Klein and Lawrence’s (2012) emphasis on the applicability of endogenous and exogenous modes of control in the domain of time, Lawrence and Klein (2013) developed a methodology suitable for isolating endogenous versus exogenous control of alerting, or attention in time. Alerting is defined as the preparation and maintenance of arousal to process stimuli (Posner & Petersen, 1990). In Lawrence and Klein’s (2013) design, participants were presented with continuous mono (i.e., diotic) white noise. Signals were presented by briefly shifting from mono to stereo (i.e., dichotic) sound (uncorrelated white noise to both ears). Even without a global increase in amplitude, this change is easily detected by observers. Lawrence and Klein used these signals with or without a global increase in intensity (i.e., isointense).Footnote 2 A signal that does not increase in intensity allows us to explore purely endogenous control systems of temporal attention, as there is no attention-grabbing component; that is, signals with no intensity change require a more volitional form of processing. The individual is alerted in a volitional manner, as they can use top-down processing to increase arousal to better process stimuli. Signals that do increase in intensity reflexively grab the attention of an individual and, when they can be used to predict the time of the target, incorporate both exogenous and endogenous control systems. In the present study, we will be using this signal intensity manipulation, in which a change from mono to stereo presentation of noise is or is not accompanied by an intensity change.

Implementing stereo signals with and without an intensity change into a Kingstonian temporal cueing paradigm allows us to compare purely endogenous temporal attention to a condition in which both endogenous and exogenous temporal attention are involved, allowing us to explore the interaction between these two modes of temporal attention. Signals that do not increase in intensity will be referred to as no-intensity signals, while signals that do increase in intensity will be referred to as hi-intensity signals. In addition, combining these methodological features will allow us to ameliorate an inherent limitation in the prototypical Kingstone temporal cueing paradigm: The cue indicates the likely fore-period information and simultaneously notifies the observer to start their internal timer. This means the endogenously generated preparation generated by the meaning (i.e., temporal informativeness) of the cue from the exogenously generated arousal rendered from the onset of the cue are problematically paired together (Weinbach & Henik, 2012). Although alerting and temporal cueing have been identified as different mechanisms, their effects are typically confounded (Weinbach & Henik, 2013). We hope to alleviate this with our method, which allows for the separation of these two distinct, but complementary behavioral requirements of a temporal cueing paradigm: interpreting the relevant temporal information, and starting the appropriate internal timer. The informative cue akin to that used by Kingstone will be presented on screen to provide temporal information, and an auditory signal (as in Lawrence & Klein, 2013) will subsequently indicate the requisite instruction to start the timer set according to the information provided by the cue.

We (McCormick, Redden, Lawrence, & Klein, 2017) conducted two prior experiments implementing these features in a two-alternative forced-choice discrimination task (see Fig. 1). In one experiment, the no-intensity and hi-intensity signals were randomly intermixed in the same block of trials. We found a main effect of signal intensity but no TCE. This failure led to the hypothesis that the amount of mental effort required to detect the no-intensity warning signal may have interfered with an observer’s ability to effectively make use of the temporal cue. When the signal types are mixed, one must always be in a mental state to anticipate a no-intensity warning signal, lest one miss the signal. Following a similar logic in a different domain (Shore, McLaughlin, & Klein, 2001), we tested this hypothesis in the second experiment by presenting the two intensity conditions in separate blocks. The main effect of intensity was replicated, with faster responses with a hi-intensity signal compared with the no-intensity signal. Furthermore, in support of our mental effort hypothesis, we obtained a TCE with the hi-intensity signals and not with the no-intensity signals. In these two preliminary studies, then, we saw no TCE when signal intensities were mixed, nor when using exclusively no-intensity signals in combination with the temporal cueing paradigm. Accordingly, we were left without any clear conclusions regarding how the two modes of alerting may interact.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Left half: A within-block intensity discrimination paradigm looking at the temporal cueing effects (invalid RT − valid RT). This manipulation (n = 40) produced no temporal cueing effects (TCEs) in either the no-intensity or the hi-intensity condition. Right half: A between-block intensity discrimination paradigm looking at temporal cueing effects across two signal intensities. This manipulation (n = 40) produced a TCE in the hi-intensity condition, but not in the no-intensity condition. Error bars are 95% confidence intervals

The hypothesis that mental effort may mitigate TCEs has precedent in the literature (Correa, Lupiáñez, Milliken, & Tudela, 2004). Correa et al. showed that TCEs can be modulated by stimulus–response demands, whereby robust TCEs were obtained in detection tasks but were considerably less robust when the task was discrimination. Additionally, occupying working memory during a temporal cueing experiment can eliminate a TCE (de la Rosa, Sanabria, Capizzi, & Correa, 2012). This support, in combination with our pilot findings, led us to simplify our target task from discrimination to detection. Based on our earlier robust finding of an effect of signal intensity (exogenous temporal attention) and our expectation (based on Correa et al., 2004) that when using a detection task we would obtain a TCE (endogenous temporal attention), our key question is whether these two forms of control of temporal attention would be additive or interactive.

Method

Participants

Fifty participants were run in this experiment in a computer laboratory. Data collection was conducted in groups of four to 10, until 40 participants (mean age = 20.98 years, 11 males, seven left-handed) met our trial count criterion (see Results section). Previous studies measuring a behavioral effect have tested an average of n = 9.5 (min = 7, max = 12)Footnote 3 subjects per condition. To ensure that our methods would be at least as powerful as the studies existing in the literature, we elected to aim for a sample that quadrupled these previous sample sizes. A power calculation based on a cueing effect (8 ms; d = .49) in one condition from our pilot studies reported in the Introduction (McCormick et al., 2017) showed that with a sample size of 40 and a Type I error of .05, the a priori power of the current experiment was 0.85. All participants were undergraduate students at Dalhousie University. Participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and did not have any known neurological or cognitive disorders.

Apparatus

The experiment was presented on ten 24-inch Mac monitors connected to a Mac Mini running OS X with a 2.66 GHz Intel Core 2 Duo processor and a NIVIDIA GeForce 9400 256 MB graphics card. A set of 10 headphone monitors (Sony MDR-101LP) were used. Acceptable volume settings ranged between three and five. Participants sat at a maximum distance of 102 cm from the screen. Game-pad controllers [Xbox 360 wired controllers] were used by participants to make responses.Footnote 4

Stimuli

Temporal cues, which could take the form of either the letter “S” or “L” (.5 degrees of visual angle; DVA) with equal probability were presented at the center of the screen until the presentation of the target. These cues were blue (RGB: 0, 0, 255) on a gray (RGB: 119, 119, 199) background. Targets took the form of circles (.5 DVA) that appeared at the center of the screen. Targets were either black (RGB: 0, 0, 0) or white (RGB: 255, 255, 255). Participants received on-screen feedback in the form of a single digit (.5 DVA) after making their response, which reflected their reaction time on that trial to the 10th of a second.

Mono white noise was constantly presented throughout a trial. A sampling frequency of 44100 Hz was used. Auditory warning signals were presented in the form of stereo white noise for a duration of 100 ms. In the ‘no-intensity warning signal condition, there was no change in intensity relative to the mono baseline. In the hi-intensity warning signal condition, the ambient volume was doubled.

Procedure

Participants were instructed that the temporal information provided by cues would help optimize performance as the cues indicated with high probability (80%) whether the fore period between the auditory warning signal and the target would be short or long. A practice block was conducted consisting of 40 trials that were not included in the analysis.

Mono noise was presented in both ears continuously throughout the task. Trials began with a blue letter in the center of a gray screen. This letter was a temporal information cue that could either be an S or an L to represent a probable 400 ms or 1,600 ms fore period between the later auditory warning signal and the presentation of the target, respectively. These cues were 80% predictive. The cue remained on-screen until the target was presented. The auditory warning signal was presented after a random exponential (i.e., “nonaging”) interval within the range of 2 to 10 seconds (mean = 4 s) to indicate the target was imminent. The auditory warning signal was either the hi-intensity or no-intensity change from mono to stereo noise and was presented for a duration of 100 ms. Either 400 or 1,600 ms after this auditory cue, a black or white target was presented for either 1,000 ms or until the participant responded. Simple detection responses were made by pressing both triggers on the gamepad. Participants were instructed that they were required to press the response apparatus at least halfway down to register a response. If participants responded before the target was on the screen, then a message that read “Too Early!” was presented in red (RGB: 255, 0, 0). If they failed to respond during this 1,000 ms window, then a message that read “Miss!” was presented in red. This experiment has 40 trials per block, with 12 blocks in total (Fig. 2).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Four different trial runs in the current experiment: valid long, valid short, invalid short, and invalid long. White noise is played throughout the task. 1. The trial begins with either a short (S) or long (L) temporal cue. This indicates (with 80% probability) whether the target will follow the signal (Panel 2) by a short or long SOA. 2. After a random fixation interval (2–10 s), an auditory signal is presented through headphones. This signal is a 100-ms change from mono to stereo noise. Importantly, this signal either increased in intensity from the background stereo noise (hi-intensity signal) or maintained the same intensity (no-intensity signal) as the background. 3. After the short or long SOA, a white or black target (50/50) is presented at fixation. Participants are instructed to press both triggers as soon as they detect a circle. If participants respond within the allotted 1,000 ms, then feedback of their reaction time speed is presented at the center of the screen (e.g., 234)

Results

Data preprocessing

Before beginning this experiment, we decided to only include participants with greater than 75% of trials completed. The was consistent with our two prior pilot experiments (see Fig. 1). We first removed any trials that we called anticipatory responses, which included responses after a signal but before a target (12.9%), or responses faster than 150 ms (2.2%). We then eliminated trials that were slower than 600 ms (1.1%), and when participants missed responding to the target (.25%). For the remainder of trials, we eliminated trials based on inappropriate trigger responses, which included no maximum response, pressing only one trigger, and multiple responses (6.05%). After excluding these trials, we checked to see who met our previously set criterion for percentage of trials completed, which resulted in the removal 10 of the 50 participants.

Statistical tests

Results from 40 participants (11 male, seven left-handed, mean age = 20.98years) were included in statistical analysis. As noted in the Introduction, once the short SOA has expired without a target, performance on targets presented at the long SOA is “confounded” by the possibility that temporal reorienting will obviate the cueing manipulation. Therefore, our primary analyses will focus on response times to targets presented at the short SOA.

We performed a 2 (signal intensity) × 2 (cue condition) ANOVA on mean RT to targets presented at the short SOAFootnote 5 using the EZ package within the R statistics software (Lawrence, 2016; R Core Team, 2016). We observed a significant effect for the cue condition, F(1, 39) = 37.76, p < .001, ηp2 = .068, with faster RTs for the valid (284 ms) than for the invalid (299 ms) cue condition. There was also a significant effect of signal intensity, F(1, 39) = 32.23, p < .001, ηp2 = .08, with faster RTs for the hi-intensity condition (278 ms) than for the no-intensity condition (295 ms). There was no interaction effect between cue condition and signal intensity, F(1, 39) = .051, p = .823, ηp2 < .001.

The additivity seen here in reaction time suggests that the two forms of temporal attention are independent and operating at different stages of processing. If this is really the case, then according to Sternberg (1969), we should also see an additive pattern if we analyze the variances of RT. We therefore performed a 2 (signal intensity) × 2 (cue condition) ANOVA on RT variance. In this analysis we observed significant main effects of cue condition, F(1, 39) = 14.78, p < .001, ηp2 = .05, and signal intensity, F(1, 39) = 11.37, p = .0017, ηp2 = .048. The interaction between cue condition and signal intensity, F(1, 39) = 2.32, p = .136, ηp2 = .008, was not significant (Fig. 3).Footnote 6

Fig. 3
figure 3

A within-block, mixed-intensity detection paradigm run in the current experiment comparing the mean reaction times of valid (black dashed line) and invalid (gray solid line) cues across our two signal intensities (error bars are Fisher’s least significant difference)

Discussion

Our analysis demonstrates that the two forms of temporal attention explored here are independent: temporal cueing (endogenous temporal attention) and signal intensity (exogenous temporal attention) had additive effect on both RT and its variance. Following Sternberg’s (1969) additive factors method, this pattern strongly implies that these two factors and therefore these two forms of temporal attention are affecting different stages of processing,

Whereas we can be confident that “different” stages of processing are affected by the two forms of temporal attention explored here, from the results of the present experiment we cannot make confident assertions about which stages of processing each form is affecting. That noted, there are some hints in the literature. Posner (Posner & Boies, 1971; Posner, Klein, Summers, & Buggie, 1973) generated converging evidence for his proposal (Posner, 1975) that the alertness or preparation generated in warning signal studies did not affect the rate of accumulation of information about the subsequent target, but instead affected when the attention/decision system would consult this accumulating information. In particular, the observed responds sooner when alert than when not alert, which predicts that so long as targets are not brief or masked, then the reaction time advantage associated with alertness will be accompanied by a disadvantage in accuracy (a prediction confirmed by Posner et al., 1973). Lawrence and Klein (2013) pointed out that in the kinds of studies on which Posner’s proposal was based, the warning signals could be endogenously used to focus attention in time but also entailed the possibility of exogenous temporal attention (arousal) because the warning signals entailed a change on intensity. Importantly, Lawrence and Klein confirmed Posner’s proposal while also demonstrating that the speed–accuracy trade-off it predicted was confined to high-intensity signals that could be used to prepare. Thus both Posner’s proposal and Lawrence and Klein’s findings strongly suggest that the effect of high-intensity signal is probably not affecting the rate of information accrual about the target and instead is affecting when the attention/decision to initiate a response is made. Although one might have imagined that with a simple RT task, like we have used here, the temporal cueing manipulation might be affecting such a late stage, the additivity we have found provides evidence against this. Moreover, there are several studies using the Kingstonian paradigm (Correa, Lupiáñez, Madrid, & Tudela, 2006; Correa, Sanabria, Spence, Tudela, & Lupiáñez, 2006) that suggest that early stages of processing are enhanced by temporal expectancy. These findings from the literature suggest that the temporal cueing paradigm, that we believe is primarily manipulating temporal attention endogenously, is operating on an early stage of processing and that the effect of a hi-intensity signal, that we believe has its effect primarily exogenously, is affecting a later (decision or response) stage of processing. We offer this simply as our best guess at this time. Further experimentation is needed to be confident.

Future experiments should continue with this tradition of measuring independence of alerting and preparation stages within the different domains of the Klein and Lawrence (2012) attention model. The isolation of the temporal cue and alerting signal can provide the ability to study their effects independently, something that has been missing from the current body of literature (Weinbach & Henik, 2012). One possible next step is to isolate which stage of processing was impacted by these manipulations to further expand our knowledge related to the independence of these alerting mechanisms. Developing a paradigm with a discrimination task in which we can successfully observe TCEs with both the hi-intensity and no-intensity signals would be ideal, as this would allow us to observe the relation between speed and accuracy under these two signaling conditions. It is important to continue to develop this taxonomy of attention and explore how it might incorporate novel forms of temporal attention (such as that elicited when we experience rhythmic stimuli; Barnes & Jones, 2000; for a review, see Nobre & Ede, 2018) because we believe that the distinction between endogenous and exogenous control is both important and too often ignored.